Syllabus Theories Intergroup Relations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Syllabus Theories Intergroup Relations Theories of Intergroup Relations 1 Psychology 9234 Fall, 2015 Theories of Intergroup Relations Fatih Uenal, Room A-12 Time: Tue 16:00-18:00 Room: 271 Dragos Kampüsü Office hours: by appointment Email: [email protected] Substance: This course includes an introduction to and comparison of most extant theories of intergroup relations: those theories that explain how human groups form and relate to one another. It may be of relevance to students in psychology, sociology, political science, communications, history, anthropology, economics, or to anyone interested in group conflict, diplomacy, oppression, discrimination, society, or culture. One of the major questions we will consider is whether different kinds of theories are required to explain intergroup relations based on different kinds of group definitions, including nationality (the nation-state), gender, race, ethnicity, sexual orientation, and class. We will also consider the type of phenomena and level of analysis each theory addresses, that is, whether theories concern mainly the psychological state of an individual, the social-cultural context, the social-structural context, the economics of social arrangements, collective behavior, institutionalized patterns, and so forth. Because of the theoretical abstractness of some of the works, I have also provided a “concrete” reading dealing with each topic for each subject (e.g., works of autobiography, fiction, commentary, history, etc.). Such readings are indented on the reading list below. Original readings and important writers are emphasized in the readings selected, but research concerning elaborating on each theory will also be read. Objectives: Students will learn to recognize the level of analysis theories use, the kind of process or mechanisms they prescribe, the family of theories to which the theory belongs, as well as the basic tenets and assumptions of each theory. They will gain practice in weighing a variety of kinds of evidence in the social sciences used to test theories. They will also acquire expertise in a variety of forms of intergroup relations, the problems that can arise therein, and what solutions have been proposed for such problems. They will also learn what kinds of research each theory has generated and the kinds of phenomena each theory describes and ignores. Requirements: This course requires the active participation on the part of students. It will also require a fairly heavy load of reading original sources. Each week, students will submit a 1 page written commentary of the readings for that week to the instructor cite by 9 a.m. on Monday. These summaries should state what each student felt the most important and most interesting points of the readings were, as well as reactions and questions. The instructor will use these commentaries to guide the contents of the discussion. Each week, each student is expected to read all the non-starred readings, which will be posted on IstSehrNet (except the S&P book). Students will each present one or more of the single-starred readings (or another relevant reading on the topic the student finds, with instructor’s approval) to the class once during the semester. The final graded assignment can be done in ONE of three ways: A 10 page review paper on a relevant topic, a Wikipedia page on an intergroup theory, or a research proposal for a study the student intends to conduct. This assignment is due on Nov. 29. The final assignment comprises 30% of the course grade, regular class participation (including thought-provoking written and verbal commentaries) comprises 50% of the course grade, and starred presentations comprise the remaining 20%. Theories of Intergroup Relations 2 Text: Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999/2001). Social dominance. New York Cambridge University Press. Assigned and Optional Readings by TopiC and Date * indicates a reading a student may choose to present in class; ** indicates a content reading for interest. If presented, readings with ** should be compared & contrasted with assigned readings. 1. Introduction & Overview of course (30.08. 2015) Sidanius & Pratto (2001). Chapter 1 Pratto, F. Henkel, K. & Lee, I. (2013). Stereotypes and prejudice from an intergroup relations perspective: Their relation to social structure. In C. Stangor & C. Crandall (Eds.), Stereotyping and Prejudice. New York: Psychology Press. Intergroup StruCtural Relations and PsyChology 2. Realistic Group Conflict Theory (06.09.2015) Angelou, M. (1969). I know why the caged bird sings (Chapter 27). New York: Random House. Campbell, D. T. (1965). Ethnocentric and other altruistic motives. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 283-311). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1966). Formation of outgroup attitudes and stereotypes: Experimental verification (pp. 271-295). Groups in harmony and tension. New York Octagon Books. Berdahl, J. L. (2007). The sexual harassment of uppity women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 425- 437. *Kinder, D. R. & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Threat and advantage. In Divided by Color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. (pp. 49-91). Chicago & London: University of Chicago Press. *Bornstein, G. (2003). Intergroup conflict: Individual, group, and collective interests. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 129-145. *JaCkson, L. M., & Esses, V. M. (2000). Effects of perceived economic competition on people’s willingness to help empower immigrants. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 3, 419-435. 753. *Reskin, B. F. (1988). Bringing the men back in: Sex differentiation and the devaluation of women's work. Gender & Society, 2, 58-81. *Zubrinsky, C. L., & Bobo, L. (1996). Prismatic metropolis: Race and residential segregation in the City of Angels. Social Science Research, 25, 335-374. Theories of Intergroup Relations 3 *Hepworth, J. T., & West, S. G. (1988). Lynchings and the economy: A time-series reanalysis of Hovland and Sears (1940). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 239-247. 3. Roles, Hierarchies, and Inequality (13.09.2015) Sidanius & Pratto (1999). Chapter 2 Crow Dog, M., & Erdoes, R. (1990). Lakota Woman (pp. 55-72). New York Harper Perennial. Hedges, C. & SaCCo, J. (2012). Days of theft. Days of Destruction Days of Revolt. New York: Nation Books. (pp. 1-57). Guioi, C. & Chuntao, W. (2006). The Martyr. Will the boat sink the water? The life of China’s peasants. New York Public Affairs. (pp. 3-26). Byerly, V. (1986). Clara Thrift. Hard Times Cotton Mill Girls: Personal histories of womanhood and poverty in the South. Ithaca, New York ILR Press. (pp. 110-122). Marin, P. (1991, July 8). The prejudice against men. The Nation, 253, Issue 2, 46-51. Amiry, S. (2010). Prisons are for men and Nothing makes sense, why should I? Nothing to lose but your life. Doha, Qatar: Bloomsbury Qatar Foundation Publishing. (pp. 33-39, 99- 107). Note: Amiry is a middle-aged educated Lebanese woman who is trying to cross from the West Bank into Israel for day-labor with young men she sometimes employs and who are in the habit of making this journey daily. 4. Social Structure and Group Identities (20.09.2015) Johnson, J. W. (1927). Chapter 1. The autobiography of an ex-coloured man. New York: Random House. (pp. 3-23). Perry, P. (2001). White means never having to say you’re ethnic. In J. A. Holstein & J. F. Gubrium (Eds.), Inner lives and social worlds. New York: Oxford University Press. (pp. 362- 380). Taifel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Little, D. (2002). Orientalism, American style. American Orientalism. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press. (pp. 9-42). *Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T. & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 631-638. *Alexander, M., Brewer, M. B., & Herrmann, R. K. (1999). Images and affect: A functional analysis of out-group stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 78-93. *Gibson, J. R. (2006). Do strong identities fuel intolerance? Evidence from the South African case. Political Psychology, 27, 665-705. Theories of Intergroup Relations 4 *Eagly, A. H. & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of men and women into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 745-754. *Ellemers, N., Wilke, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (1993). Effects of legitimacy of low group or individual status on individual and collective identity enhancement strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 766-778. * Mullin, B., & Hogg, M. A. (1998). Dimensions of subjective uncertainty in social identification and minimal intergroup discrimination. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 345-365. *Cottrell, C. A. & Neuberg, S. J. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A socio- functional threat-based approach to ‘prejudice.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 770-789. *Ysseldyk, R., Matheson, K. & Anisman, H. (2010). Religiosity as Identity: Toward an understanding of religion from a social identity perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 60-71. 5. Intergroup Segregation and Contact (27.09.2015) Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Creating a sense of “Opposite Sides.” In J. A. Holstein & J. F. Gubrium (Eds.), Inner lives and social worlds. New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 386-404.) Pamuk, O. (1998). I am your beloved uncle. My name is Red. London: Faber & Faber. (pp. 26-32). This is a chapter from Nobel-prize winner Orhan Pamuk’s novel. To me it represents an interesting moment of cross-cultural contact. Wormer, K. van & Falkner, J. (2012). Learning about cognitive dissonance and race relations: A study of the personal narratives of older White southern women who grew up with maids. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 22, 392-408. Dixon, J. & Durrheim, K. (2003). Contact and the ecology of racial division: Some varieties of informal segregation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 1-23.
Recommended publications
  • Social Norms and Social Influence Mcdonald and Crandall 149
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Social norms and social influence Rachel I McDonald and Christian S Crandall Psychology has a long history of demonstrating the power and and their imitation is not enough to implicate social reach of social norms; they can hardly be overestimated. To norms. Imitation is common enough in many forms of demonstrate their enduring influence on a broad range of social life — what creates the foundation for culture and society phenomena, we describe two fields where research continues is not the imitation, but the expectation of others for when to highlight the power of social norms: prejudice and energy imitation is appropriate, and when it is not. use. The prejudices that people report map almost perfectly onto what is socially appropriate, likewise, people adjust their A social norm is an expectation about appropriate behav- energy use to be more in line with their neighbors. We review ior that occurs in a group context. Sherif and Sherif [8] say new approaches examining the effects of norms stemming that social norms are ‘formed in group situations and from multiple groups, and utilizing normative referents to shift subsequently serve as standards for the individual’s per- behaviors in social networks. Though the focus of less research ception and judgment when he [sic] is not in the group in recent years, our review highlights the fundamental influence situation. The individual’s major social attitudes are of social norms on social behavior. formed in relation to group norms (pp. 202–203).’ Social norms, or group norms, are ‘regularities in attitudes and Address behavior that characterize a social group and differentiate Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045, it from other social groups’ [9 ] (p.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of Cultural Contact
    1 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF CULTURAL CONTACT Deborah A. Prentice and Dale T. Miller thnic diversity currently preoccupies a sizable segment of U.S. society, from employers and school administrators, who must E manage diversity within institutional settings, to politicians and social scientists, who must formulate policies for addressing the com- peting claims of different ethnic groups. The issue of diversity is fraught with anxiety. Ethnic conflicts in many countries around the world attest to the potential for relations across cultural boundaries to go seriously and destructively awry. Moreover, Americans' own struggles with race have left many pessimistic about the prospects for achieving positive, stable relations between ethnic groups. With new waves of immigrants coming from Asia and Latin America and higher birth rates among mi- nority than majority groups, the U.S. population is becoming, and will continue to become, ethnically and culturally more diverse. Thus, how to promote positive relations across group boundaries is a question of paramount importance. Thirty or forty years ago, psychologists thought they had an answer to this question. The contact hypothesis posited that if members of dif- ferent ethnic groups interact with each other on an equal-status basis in pursuit of common goals, positive intergroup relations will result (Allport 1954). This hypothesis was so appealing that it spawned hun- dreds of studies designed to test and refine its claims. The results have been less than encouraging. Yes, equal-status contact can have positive results, but only if many conditions obtain: the contact should be mean- ingful and have the potential to extend beyond the immediate situation; the individuals should be as similar as possible on all dimensions be- sides group membership; the contact should be voluntary, extended in duration, and varied across contexts; and so on (for a more complete list, see Stephan 1985).
    [Show full text]
  • Intergroup Dialogue in Higher Education: Definition, Origins, and Practices
    Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) • DOI: 10.1002/aehe.3204 Intergroup Dialogue in Higher Education: Definition, Origins, and Practices NTERGROUP DIALOGUE IS AN INNOVATIVE PRACTICE IN Ihigher education that promotes student engagement across cultural and social divides, fostering learning about social diversity and inequali- ties and cultivating an ethos of social responsibility. This approach to diver- sity education on college and university campuses responds to a growing need for educational practices that prepares students to live, work, and lead in a complex, diverse, and stratified society (Banks, 2002; Chesler, Lewis, and Crowfoot, 2005; Guarasci and Cornwell, 1997; Gurin, 1999; hooks, 1994; Hurtado, Milem, Clayton-Pedersen, and Allen, 1999; Sleeter and McLaren, 1995; Stephan and Stephan, 2001; Schoem, Frankel, Zúñiga, and Lewis, 1993; Tatum, 1997). Intergroup dialogue (IGD), the focus of this monograph, is one of several dialogue and deliberation practices currently being used on college and uni- versity campuses in the United States. Many of these practices seek to foster conversation about contentious issues in collaborative ways (Schoem and others, 2001; Zúñiga and Nagda, 2001). One model, Study Circles (Flavin-McDonald and Barrett, 1999; McCoy and Sherman, 1994; McCoy and McCormick, 2001), emphasizes community building and social action. Study Circles bring community members together in small groups to build rela- tionships, deliberate about community issues, and explore actions to effect change in their communities (also see http://www.studycircles.org). Another model, Sustained Dialogue (Parker, 2006; Saunders, 1999, 2003), draws from work in international conflict resolution and peace building. In Sustained Dialogue, stu- dents of diverse backgrounds come together to build mutual respect, identify Intergroup Dialogue in Higher Education 1 issues of conflict, and generate action plans, including workable agreements to conflicts or disputes (also see http://www.sustaineddialogue.org).
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Intergroup Bias: Perceptions and Attitudes in Rhesus Macaques
    ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION The Evolution of Intergroup Bias: Perceptions and Attitudes in Rhesus Macaques Neha Mahajan, Margaret A. Martinez, and Gil Diesendruck Natashya L. Gutierrez Bar-Ilan University Yale University Mahzarin R. Banaji Laurie R. Santos Harvard University Yale University Social psychologists have learned a great deal about the nature of intergroup conflict and the attitudinal and cognitive processes that enable it. Less is known about where these processes come from in the first place. In particular, do our strategies for dealing with other groups emerge in the absence of human-specific experi- ences? One profitable way to answer this question has involved administering tests that are conceptual equivalents of those used with adult humans in other species, thereby exploring the continuity or discontinuity of psychological processes. We examined intergroup preferences in a nonhuman species, the rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta). We found the first evidence that a nonhuman species automatically distinguishes the faces of members of its own social group from those in other groups and displays greater vigilance toward outgroup members (Experiments 1–3). In addition, we observed that macaques spontaneously associate novel objects with specific social groups and display greater vigilance to objects associated with outgroup members (Experiments 4–5). Finally, we developed a looking time procedure—the Looking Time Implicit Association Test, which resembles the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995)—and we discovered that macaques, like humans, automatically evaluate ingroup members positively and outgroup members negatively (Experiments 6–7). These field studies represent the first controlled experiments to examine the presence of intergroup attitudes in a nonhuman species.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study Among Turkish-Dutch Muslims Maykel Verkuyten
    Religious Group Identification and Inter-Religious Relations: A Study Among Turkish-Dutch Muslims Maykel Verkuyten To cite this version: Maykel Verkuyten. Religious Group Identification and Inter-Religious Relations: A Study Among Turkish-Dutch Muslims. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, SAGE Publications, 2007, 10 (3), pp.341-357. 10.1177/1368430207078695. hal-00571655 HAL Id: hal-00571655 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00571655 Submitted on 1 Mar 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 2007 Vol 10(3) 341–357 Religious Group Identifi cation and Inter-Religious Relations: A Study Among Turkish-Dutch Muslims Maykel Verkuyten Utrecht University Following social identity theory, this research examines the relationship between group identifi cation and intergroup relations by focusing on religion. Religious as well as Dutch national group identifi cation was examined among Turkish-Dutch Muslims. Identifi cation was studied in relation to general affective ratings of multiple religious groups (Muslims, Christians, Hindustanis, Jews and non-believers) and the endorsement of Islamic group rights. The results show that Muslim identifi cation was more like a nominal (high or ‘total’ identifi cation) than a continuous variable, and that many participants showed low identifi cation with the national group.
    [Show full text]
  • Theories on Intergroup Relations and Emotions: a Theoretical Overview1
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Psychologica Theories on intergroup relations and emotions: A theoretical overview 7 Theories on intergroup relations and emotions: A theoretical overview • pág. 7-33 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_57­‑2_1 Theories on intergroup relations and emotions: A theoretical overview1 Ana Figueiredo2, Joaquim Pires Valentim3 and Bertjan Doosje4 Abstract The present manuscript is a theoretical revision of the major theories about intergroup relations and emotions, developed over the last decades in the field of social psychol- ogy. Following a general chronological order, we first revise the first individualistic approaches to the field of intergroup relations and social conflict. Afterwards, we revise the realistic group conflict theory and relative deprivation theory. Next, we discuss the social identity and the self-categorization theories and highlight some of the most studied dimensions of intergroup relations within this framework. To conclude, we present some of the most influential appraisal theories of emotions and the theory of intergroup emotions. We claim for an integration of several different approaches when studying intergroup relations and, more specifically, the role of different emotions in explaining instances of intergroup conflict. Keywords: theoretical revision; social identity; intergroup relations; emotions 1 This work was supported by a PhD grant from the Foundation for Science and Technology, Portugal [grant number SFRH / BD / 36056 / 2007]. 2 University of Coimbra. Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences.Email:[email protected] 3 University of Coimbra. Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences. Email: [email protected] 4 University of Amsterdam.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution and Future of Diversity at Work
    Journal of Applied Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 102, No. 3, 483–499 0021-9010/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000161 The Evolution and Future of Diversity at Work Quinetta Roberson Ann Marie Ryan Villanova University Michigan State University Belle Rose Ragins University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee This article examines the evolution of diversity in the Journal of Applied Psychology. To begin, we explore foundations of the concept of diversity, including its appearance in both applied contexts and the scholarly literature. We then review the literature on diversity, including the development of its conceptualization and operationalizations over time, in the Journal and in the field of applied psycho- logical science at large. We also examine the processes underlying the effects of diversity, and specific outcomes of diversity in organizations. To conclude, we offer a future research agenda that highlights diversity-related topics and issues important for advancing an understanding of diversity and moving the field forward, especially within the Journal. This work makes several contributions to research on diversity in organizations. First, we provide a lens for examining change in the study of diversity over time as well as a critical examination of the benefits and challenges associated with these changes. Second, we review the underlying mechanisms and key contextual influences on diversity effects in organizations. Third, our review examines the explanatory power of current diversity research and then uses this to develop a research agenda. By organizing the broad body of literature that exists on diversity, our article offers a sharp picture of what gaps in knowledge exist and where future research should focus.
    [Show full text]
  • Theories of Intergroup Relations References
    Theories of Intergroup Relations References • Tajfel, H., and J. Turner: The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior, The Psychology of Intergroup Relations, ed. S. Worchel and W. Austin, Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall: (1986) 7-24. • Ioannou, C., A. Rustichini and S. Qi: Group Payoffs as Public Signals, Journal of Economic Psychology, 48 (2015), 89-105. Christos A. Ioannou 2/25 Terms • In-group: The social group to which an individual perceives herself or himself as belonging. • Out-group: Any group other than the one to which individuals perceive themselves as belonging. Christos A. Ioannou 3/25 Authoritarian Personality Theory Authoritarian personality is a state of mind or attitude characterized by belief in absolute obedience or submission to someone else's authority, as well as the administration of that belief through the oppression of one's subordinates. It usually applies to individuals who are known or viewed as having an authoritarian, strict, or oppressive personality towards subordinates. The theory consists of three components. 1 Authoritarian submission: A high degree of submission to authorities who are perceived to be established and legitimate in the society in which one lives. 2 Authoritarian aggression: A general aggressiveness, directed against various persons, that is perceived to be sanctioned by established authorities. Christos A. Ioannou 3 Conventionalism: A high degree of adherence to the 4/25 social conventions that are perceived to be endorsed by society and its established authorities. Authoritarian Personality Theory (Cont.) 3 Conventionalism: A high degree of adherence to the social conventions that are perceived to be endorsed by society and its established authorities.
    [Show full text]
  • Religion and Reducing Prejudice
    GPI0010.1177/1368430216629566Group Processes & Intergroup RelationsBurch-Brown and Baker 629566research-article2016 G Group Processes & P Intergroup Relations I Article R Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 1 –24 Religion and reducing prejudice © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1368430216629566 gpir.sagepub.com Joanna Burch-Brown1 and William Baker2 Abstract Drawing on findings from the study of prejudice and prejudice reduction, we identify a number of mechanisms through which religious communities may influence the intergroup attitudes of their members. We hypothesize that religious participation could in principle either reduce or promote prejudice with respect to any given target group. A religious community’s influence on intergroup attitudes will depend upon the specific beliefs, attitudes, and practices found within the community, as well as on interactions between the religious community and the larger social environment in which it is embedded. Basing our proposals on findings from the literature on prejudice formation and prejudice reduction allows us to outline useful directions for future studies of religion and prejudice. Keywords contact hypothesis, group processes, prejudice, prejudice reduction, religion, religiosity, social identity theory Paper received 4 April 2014; revised version accepted 23 November 2015. In this paper, we propose a set of simple hypoth- under certain facilitating conditions (e.g., equality eses about ways in which religions might tend to of status between the groups, successful coopera- promote or reduce prejudice. Our method is to tion, affirmation of positive distinct identities, build on established findings from the study of etc.). Conversely, we predict that religious com- prejudice reduction generally, and apply these munities will tend to increase prejudice to the findings to the study of religion and prejudice.
    [Show full text]
  • Intergroup Relations
    Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 38 Intergroup Relations When is My Group More Important than Yours? LUISA BATALHA ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS ISSN 1652-9030 UPPSALA ISBN 978-91-554-7113-2 2008 urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-8502 !" # $ # # % & ' (' & ) *& & $ + & , - . - / 0 & & 12 & & ()3 42545""652 5& $ # # & ' 7 ' $ # 8 $ # & + ' ' $ # # # $ ' $ $ & ( 9# ## # # $ & $ # $ $5' $ ## # $ $ # 9 & *:' $ ' # $ & ) ' 5 $ $ $ 5 $ # & - # ( # ' $ $ & ( ' $ ## ' $ ; ' $ & $ ' $ $ 9 # & < $ $ # & ' ## ' # ' # & # $ # $ $ # & $ $ # $ # 9 9# 9 ! "##$ %&'$"(# = * ) ((3 1"54 ()3 42545""652 5 ! !!! 5" > !?? &:&? 0 @ ! !!! 5"A Cover ilIustration “Tug of War” by Brian Jones List of Papers I Batalha, L., Akrami, N., & Ekehammar, B. (2007). Outgroup favoritism: The role of power perception, gender, and conserva- tism. Current Research in Social Psychology, 13, 38-49. II Batalha, L. (2007). Affirmative action: The role of ideological context, self interest, and conservative ideologies. Manuscript submitted for publication. III Batalha, L., Ekehammar, B., & Akrami, N. (2007). Affirmative action: The
    [Show full text]
  • Redefining Climate Change Inaction As Temporal Intergroup Bias: Temporally Adapted Interventions for Reducing Prejudice May Help Elicit Environmental Protection
    Accepted Manuscript Redefining climate change inaction as temporal intergroup bias: Temporally adapted interventions for reducing prejudice may help elicit environmental protection Rose Meleady, Richard J. Crisp PII: S0272-4944(17)30105-6 DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.08.005 Reference: YJEVP 1157 To appear in: Journal of Environmental Psychology Received Date: 23 June 2017 Revised Date: 27 August 2017 Accepted Date: 30 August 2017 Please cite this article as: Meleady, R., Crisp, R.J., Redefining climate change inaction as temporal intergroup bias: Temporally adapted interventions for reducing prejudice may help elicit environmental protection, Journal of Environmental Psychology (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.08.005. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Running head: TEMPORAL INTERGROUP BIAS Redefining climate change inaction as temporal intergroup bias: Temporally adapted interventions for reducing prejudice may help elicit environmental protection Rose Meleady 1 & Richard J. Crisp 2 1School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, 2 Department of Psychology, Durham University MANUSCRIPT Word Count: 6438 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Rose Meleady, School of Psychology, UniversityACCEPTED of East Anglia, NR4 7TJ, UK. Email: [email protected], Tel: +44 1603 591760.
    [Show full text]
  • June 17 – 20, 2015 Sheraton | Ann Arbor, MI
    Schedule at a Glance The 10th Annual National Intergroup Dialogue Institute | June 17 – 20, 2015 Sheraton | Ann Arbor, MI Wednesday, June 17 8:00 am – 9:00 am Registration Michigan Prefunction 9:00 am – 10:30 am Welcome/Introduction and Institute Roadmap Michigan I Kelly Maxwell, Co-Director, IGR and Psychology Faculty Monita Thompson, Co-Director, IGR and Assistant Dean of Students Mark Chesler, Professor Emeritus of Sociology 10:30 am – 10:45 am Break with Snacks Michigan Prefunction 10:45 am – 1:00 pm Institute Clusters Cluster Rooms Introductions, Guidelines, Brave Spaces and Comfort/Safety/Learning Edges (Stage 1) Group A: Adrienne Dessel and Jake Cinti Petit I Group B: Nitesh Singh and Sara Crider Petit II Group C: Taryn Petryk and Will Frey Michigan III Group D: Sharonda Simmons and Becky Christensen Michigan IV Group E: Shana Schoem and Laura Sánchez-Parkinson Michigan V Group F: Charles Behling and Monita Thompson Grande III 1:00 pm – 2:00 pm Lunch Michigan II 2:00 pm – 3:15 pm The Four Stages of IGD Michigan I Roger Fisher, Co-Associate Director, IGR Susan King, Outreach Specialist and Instructor, IGR 3:15 pm – 3:30 pm Break 3:30 pm – 5:15 pm Institute Clusters Cluster Rooms Creating a Shared Meaning of Dialogue (Stage 1) Dialogue/Debate/Discussion Activity Social Identity Profile General Q and A The 2015 National Intergroup Dialogue Institute | The Program on Intergroup Relations | University of Michigan 1 Schedule at a Glance The 10th Annual National Intergroup Dialogue Institute | June 17 – 20, 2015 Sheraton | Ann
    [Show full text]