Killing Behaviour of Adult Brood Parasites

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Killing Behaviour of Adult Brood Parasites 1 SPECIAL: ANECDOTES IN ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR 2 Killing behaviour of adult brood parasites 3 Šulc M.1,*, Štětková G.1,2, Jelínek V. 1, Czyż B.3, Dyrcz A.3, Karpińska O.4, 4 Kamionka-Kanclerska K.4, Rowiński P.4, Maziarz M.5, Gruszczyński A.5, Hughes A.E.6, 5 Honza M. 1 61Institute of Vertebrate Biology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, Brno, Czech Republic 72Department of Botany and Zoology, Faculty of Sciences, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech 8Republic 93Department of Behavioural Ecology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland 104Department of Forest Zoology and Wildlife Management, Warsaw University of Life 11Sciences (SGGW), Warsaw, Poland 125Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland 136Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, U.K. 14*corresponding author: Michal Šulc ([email protected]) 15 16Summary 17Decades of studies have revealed the striking adaptations of avian brood parasites for 18their unique reproductive lifestyle. Several have reported that adult brood parasites 19sometimes kill host nestlings, although the reasons for this behaviour remain unclear. 20Using continuous video-recording and camera traps, we observed the same behaviour in 21the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus, showing that both host and parasite nestlings can 22be killed. The latter has never previously been observed in any avian brood parasite. 23Here, we review this phenomenon and discuss possible explanations. 24 25Keywords: nestling infanticide, chick ejection, farming, Mafia, co-evolution 26 27Main text 28Infants are an obvious target for extermination because of their vulnerability, therefore they 29are often exploited by various predators (Weidinger 2009). Apart from this relatively simple 30predator–prey relationship, there is also the phenomenon of infanticide when a young 31offspring is killed by an adult animal of the same species. This has been observed in 32numerous species from diverse taxonomic groups including insects, fish, amphibians, birds 33and mammals (Hrdy 1979; Hausfater & Hrdy 2017). Although this behaviour was originally 34considered to be pathological (especially cases of filial infanticide when a parent kills their 35own offspring), we now know that it may be an adaptive behaviour, e.g. reducing competition 36for limited resources (Hoogland 1985) or allowing monopolization of reproduction (Haines et 37al. 2018). 38 Over the last 130 years, adults of several avian brood parasitic species have been also 39observed killing host nestlings (Table 1) for reasons that are less understood. This behaviour 40has predominantly been observed in the two most frequently studied parasitic species, the 41common cuckoo Cuculus canorus (15 nests) and the brown-headed cowbird Molothrus ater 42(20 nests) but also in the Himalayan cuckoo Cuculus saturatus (one nest) and the Shining 43bronze cuckoo Chrysococcyx lucidus (two nests). Indirect evidence for this behaviour has also 44been found in the great spotted cuckoo Clamator glandarius. It appears to be a relatively rare 45phenomenon; we found only 24 studies (including this one) reporting about 41 events (Table 461). Many reports have involved only circumstantial evidence; however, more recent studies 47have recorded this behaviour on video-cameras (Table 1). Using continuous video-recording 48and camera traps, we observed this killing behaviour in the common cuckoo (hereafter 49cuckoo) in four different hosts: the European robin Erithacus rubecula, the wood warbler 50Phylloscopus sibilatrix, the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus and the reed 51warbler Acrocephalus scirpaceus. Here, we present five pieces of evidence (four videos and 52photos; Videos 1-4 and Figure 1) showing the adult cuckoos ejecting nestlings out of host 53nests. Three of these cases are particularly interesting because the nestlings were young 54cuckoos (Videos 1 and 2, Figures 1 and 2). All events are independent observations recorded 55in four geographically separate breeding areas in the Czech Republic and Poland and 56therefore were performed by different cuckoo females. We believe that our literature review 57and video evidence demonstrating parasitic behaviour during these incidents may help to 58explain this peculiar behaviour of brood parasites. 59 All studies apart from one (Igl 2003) showed that only females of parasites exhibit this 60behaviour which is supported also by our recordings. The act cannot be seen as predation 61because parasites never preyed on nestlings (but see, Wyllie 1975). Since there appear to be 62no cases where parasites ejected nestlings from nest of non-host species (Weidinger 2009, and 63>1000 video-monitored nests on non-host species, K. Weidinger, unpubl. data), it seems that 64parasitic killing is aimed only at their hosts. From behaviour of parasites while ejecting 65nestlings it seems that it is an intentional act. Parasitic females grabbed the nestling and tossed 66it out of the nest immediately after arrival at the nest (Videos 1 and 2). If there were multiple 67nestlings in the nest, parasites systematically removed them one at a time (Video 4 and see 68also video published at Youtube by P. Elliott; Elliott 1999). In some cases, they tried to eject 69even under host attacks (Videos 3 and 4). Moreover, in two of our videos (Videos 3 and 4) it 70is possible to see that parasites visited nests repeatably despite initially being attacked and 71flushed away by the hosts. The number of ejected nestlings varied from one to the whole 72clutch of six (Table 1). No study reported parasitism after the ejection event occurred (but see, 73Sheppard 1996) which could indicate that parasites were aware that the host nests were not 74suitable for parasitism. From the above, and due to the fact that observed ejection behaviour is 75identical in phylogenetically distant parasitic species of cuckoos and cowbirds and convergent 76evolution could take place, we suggest that this behaviour is adaptive (Losos 2011). 77 Birds of several species have been reported to kill immature birds of the same or other 78species in contexts other than predation: for example, to compete for nesting sites (Kattan 792016), mates (Freed 1986), paternal investment (Veiga 1990) or food (Belles-Isles & Picman 801986; Freed 1987). In brood parasites, two other hypotheses have been proposed to explain 81the killing of host nestlings by adult brood parasites: the “Mafia” hypothesis and the 82“farming” hypothesis (Soler et al. 2017). The Mafia hypothesis proposes that parasites cause 83nest failure to punish a host that ejected a parasitic egg and hence to enforce its compliance in 84the future. This tactic is presumably only effective in parasitic species where offspring do not 85kill host nestlings, as only in these cases may the host parents still benefit by raising their own 86young while also accepting a parasite egg or nestling (Zahavi 1979). Thus, this strategy could 87evolve in e.g., the great spotted cuckoo and the brown headed cowbird, but not the common 88cuckoo (and other Old World cuckoos) where the young cuckoo chick usually evicts all host 89eggs or chicks (Reboreda et al. 2017). In contrast, the farming hypothesis could relate to all 90parasite species. It suggests that parasites cause failure of the host nests to force the host to re- 91nest and so to increase the opportunity for future parasitism. Both hypotheses seem to be 92valid, however, only in the context of destroying eggs (Soler et al. 1995; Hoover & Robinson 932007) not ejecting nestlings. 94 The reason why these two interesting hypotheses have not been tested is because 95nestling ejection by adult brood parasites is relatively rare. Brown-headed cowbirds ejected 96host nestlings at 11 of 334 video-monitored host nests (summary data from Granfors et al. 972001; Stake & Cavanagh 2001; Stake et al. 2004), and we recorded this behaviour at five of 98311 host nests for the common cuckoo (summary data from four different localities, for 99details, see Supplementary material). It therefore seems to be similarly prevalent in both these 2 100parasitic species (chi-square test: χ 1=1.26, P=0.26). Moreover, we found that nestling 101ejection cannot be predicted by parasitism rate because it occurred both in rarely (e.g. the 102European robin Erithacus rubecula, see Supplementary material) and frequently parasitized 103host species (e.g. great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus, see Supplementary 104material). 105 Because of the small sample sizes involved, at present we can only speculate about the 106validity of these hypotheses in explaining killing behaviour. In cowbirds, we would expect 107killing behaviour to occur at nests where hosts rejected the parasitic egg if the Mafia 108hypothesis were true (see above). However, observations at ten video-recorded host nests did 109not support this prediction because all of them were assessed as non-parasitized (Granfors et 110al. 2001; Stake & Cavanagh 2001). Therefore, it seems that cowbirds do not kill host 111nestlings to punish hosts for their non-cooperation. 112 For the farming hypothesis, we had two predictions; 1) parasites should kill all 113nestlings to make the host re-nest, and 2) the killing parasite should benefit from the host re- 114nesting and parasitize the replacement nest. We found that ejection of all nestlings occurred 115only in seven of 19 and 11 of 14 nests in cowbirds and cuckoos, respectively (Table 1). 116Therefore, it seems that parasites (especially cowbirds) often do not succeed in making hosts 117re-nest. However,
Recommended publications
  • Host Alarm Calls Attract the Unwanted Attention of the Brood Parasitic
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Host alarm calls attract the unwanted attention of the brood parasitic common cuckoo Attila Marton 1,2*, Attila Fülöp 2,3, Katalin Ozogány1, Csaba Moskát 4,5 & Miklós Bán 1,3,5 It is well known that avian brood parasites lay their eggs in the nests of other bird species, called hosts. It remains less clear, however, just how parasites are able to recognize their hosts and identify the exact location of the appropriate nests to lay their eggs in. While previous studies attributed high importance to visual signals in fnding the hosts’ nests (e.g. nest building activity or the distance and direct sight of the nest from vantage points used by the brood parasites), the role of host acoustic signals during the nest searching stage has been largely neglected. We present experimental evidence that both female and male common cuckoos Cuculus canorus pay attention to their host’s, the great reed warbler’s Acrocephalus arundinaceus alarm calls, relative to the calls of an unparasitized species used as controls. Parallel to this, we found no diference between the visibility of parasitized and unparasitized nests during drone fights, but great reed warblers that alarmed more frequently experienced higher rates of parasitism. We conclude that alarm calls might be advantageous for the hosts when used against enemies or for alerting conspecifcs, but can act in a detrimental manner by providing important nest location cues for eavesdropping brood parasites. Our results suggest that host alarm calls may constitute a suitable trait on which cuckoo nestlings can imprint on to recognize their primary host species later in life.
    [Show full text]
  • The Common Cuckoo Is an Effective Indicator of High Bird Species
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN The common cuckoo is an effective indicator of high bird species richness in Asia and Europe Received: 16 February 2017 Federico Morelli1, Anders Pape Møller2, Emma Nelson3, Yanina Benedetti1, Wei Liang 4, Accepted: 19 May 2017 Petra Šímová 1, Marco Moretti5 & Piotr Tryjanowski6 Published: xx xx xxxx Common cuckoo Cuculus canorus is a charismatic bird species with a dominant presence in human culture: from folklore legends to nowadays there is evidence of cuckoos being a prime candidate as a surrogate of bird diversity. Recent studies demonstrated that the cuckoo can predict hotspots of taxonomic diversity and functional diversity of bird communities in European countries. In this study, we demonstrated that the cuckoo is an excellent bioindicator at multi-spatial scale, extending cuckoo surrogacy from Europe to Asia. Even using three different survey methods (transect, square, point counts), comparing the new findings with results of our research in Europe, sites where the cuckoo is present were characterized by greater species richness, while the cuckoo was absent from sites with low species richness. The goodness of fit of models based on point counts ranged between 71 and 92%. Furthermore, the cuckoo population trend mirrors the average population trend and climate suitability of overall bird communities in Europe. The common cuckoo is therefore a suitable intercontinental bioindicator of hotspots of bird richness, even under climate change scenarios or in areas where the species co-occurs with other cuckoo species, opening a new avenue for standardized citizen science on bird biodiversity surveys worldwide. Why is the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus a fascinating bird species for humans? What are the main reasons for the species being known as “a messenger of spring and morality”1, and why is it so conspicuous in human culture? A review of folklore shows clearly that the enigmatic cuckoo has driven the collective imagination of people throughout the world for thousands of years.
    [Show full text]
  • Individually Distinctive Vocalization in Common Cuckoos (Cuculus Canorus)
    1 1 Individually distinctive vocalization in Common Cuckoos 2 (Cuculus canorus) 3 4 1 2 3 5 Sándor Zsebők , Csaba Moskát , Miklós Bán * 6 7 8 1Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Systematic Zoology and Ecology, Eötvös 9 University, H-1117 Budapest, Pázmány P. sétány 1/C., Budapest, Hungary 10 2MTA-ELTE-MTM Ecology Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, a joint 11 research group of the Biological Institute of Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Pázmány 12 Péter sétány 1/C., H-1117 Budapest, Hungary and the Hungarian Natural History Museum, 13 Baross u. 13., Budapest, H-1088, Hungary 14 3MTA-DE "Lendület" Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department of Evolutionary 15 Zoology, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, H-4010, Hungary 16 17 *Corresponding Author: Miklós Bán, [email protected] 18 19 Keywords: Common Cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, sound analysis, individual discrimination, 20 classification accuracy, acoustic signatures 21 22 Word count: 5,932 23 24 25 2 26 ABSTRACT 27 28 Distinctive individual vocalizations are advantageous in several social contexts. Both genetic 29 and environmental effects are responsible for this phenomenon resulting in different 30 frequencies and time domains of sounds in birds. This individuality can be utilized in 31 breeding bird censuses and abundance estimates. In this study we explored the individuality 32 of the advertisement calls of male Common Cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) with the aims of 33 describing the acoustic ways in which individuals differ from each other, and characterizing 34 the practical requirements for using statistical learning methods for individual recognition. 35 We collected calls from a Hungarian cuckoo population and conducted discriminant function 36 analysis on acoustic parameters to distinguish individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Bird Checklists of the World Country Or Region: Ghana
    Avibase Page 1of 24 Col Location Date Start time Duration Distance Avibase - Bird Checklists of the World 1 Country or region: Ghana 2 Number of species: 773 3 Number of endemics: 0 4 Number of breeding endemics: 0 5 Number of globally threatened species: 26 6 Number of extinct species: 0 7 Number of introduced species: 1 8 Date last reviewed: 2019-11-10 9 10 Recommended citation: Lepage, D. 2021. Checklist of the birds of Ghana. Avibase, the world bird database. Retrieved from .https://avibase.bsc-eoc.org/checklist.jsp?lang=EN&region=gh [26/09/2021]. Make your observations count! Submit your data to ebird.
    [Show full text]
  • Zambia and Zimbabwe 28 Ovember – 6 December 2009
    Zambia and Zimbabwe 28 ovember – 6 December 2009 Guide: Josh Engel A Tropical Birding Custom Tour All photos taken by the guide on this tour. The Smoke that Thunders: looking down one end of the mile-long Victoria Falls. ITRODUCTIO We began this tour by seeing one of Africa’s most beautiful and sought after birds: African Pitta . After that, the rest was just details. But not really, considering we tacked on 260 more birds and loads of great mammals. We saw Zambia’s only endemic bird, Chaplin’s Barbet , as well as a number of miombo and broad-leaf specialties, including Miombo Rock-Thrush, Racket-tailed Roller, Southern Hyliota, Miombo Pied Barbet, Miombo Glossy Starling, Bradfield’s Hornbill, Pennant-winged ightjar, and Three-banded Courser. With the onset of the rainy season just before the tour, the entire area was beautifully green and was inundated with migrants, so we were able to rack up a great list of cuckoos and other migrants, including incredible looks at a male Kurrichane Buttonquail . Yet the Zambezi had not begun to rise, so Rock Pratincole still populated the river’s rocks, African Skimmer its sandbars, and Lesser Jacana and Allen’s Gallinule its grassy margins. Mammals are always a highlight of any Africa tour: this trip’s undoubted star was a leopard , while a very cooperative serval was also superb. Victoria Falls was incredible, as usual. We had no problems in Zimbabwe whatsoever, and our lodge there on the shores of the Zambezi River was absolutely stunning. The weather was perfect throughout the tour, with clouds often keeping the temperature down and occasional rains keeping bird activity high.
    [Show full text]
  • Rejection Behavior by Common Cuckoo Hosts Towards Artificial Brood Parasite Eggs
    REJECTION BEHAVIOR BY COMMON CUCKOO HOSTS TOWARDS ARTIFICIAL BROOD PARASITE EGGS ARNE MOKSNES, EIVIN ROSKAFT, AND ANDERS T. BRAA Departmentof Zoology,University of Trondheim,N-7055 Dragvoll,Norway ABSTRACT.--Westudied the rejectionbehavior shown by differentNorwegian cuckoo hosts towardsartificial CommonCuckoo (Cuculus canorus) eggs. The hostswith the largestbills were graspejectors, those with medium-sizedbills were mostlypuncture ejectors, while those with the smallestbills generally desertedtheir nestswhen parasitizedexperimentally with an artificial egg. There were a few exceptionsto this general rule. Becausethe Common Cuckooand Brown-headedCowbird (Molothrus ater) lay eggsthat aresimilar in shape,volume, and eggshellthickness, and they parasitizenests of similarly sizedhost species,we support the punctureresistance hypothesis proposed to explain the adaptivevalue (or evolution)of strengthin cowbirdeggs. The primary assumptionand predictionof this hypothesisare that somehosts have bills too small to graspparasitic eggs and thereforemust puncture-eject them,and that smallerhosts do notadopt ejection behavior because of the heavycost involved in puncture-ejectingthe thick-shelledparasitic egg. We comparedour resultswith thosefor North AmericanBrown-headed Cowbird hosts and we found a significantlyhigher propor- tion of rejectersamong CommonCuckoo hosts with graspindices (i.e. bill length x bill breadth)of <200 mm2. Cuckoo hosts ejected parasitic eggs rather than acceptthem as cowbird hostsdid. Amongthe CommonCuckoo hosts, the costof acceptinga parasiticegg probably alwaysexceeds that of rejectionbecause cuckoo nestlings typically eject all hosteggs or nestlingsshortly after they hatch.Received 25 February1990, accepted 23 October1990. THEEGGS of many brood parasiteshave thick- nestseither by grasping the eggs or by punc- er shells than the eggs of other bird speciesof turing the eggs before removal. Rohwer and similar size (Lack 1968,Spaw and Rohwer 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • Ecology, Morphology, and Behavior in the New World Wood Warblers
    Ecology, Morphology, and Behavior in the New World Wood Warblers A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy Brandan L. Gray August 2019 © 2019 Brandan L. Gray. All Rights Reserved. 2 This dissertation titled Ecology, Morphology, and Behavior in the New World Wood Warblers by BRANDAN L. GRAY has been approved for the Department of Biological Sciences and the College of Arts and Sciences by Donald B. Miles Professor of Biological Sciences Florenz Plassmann Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 ABSTRACT GRAY, BRANDAN L., Ph.D., August 2019, Biological Sciences Ecology, Morphology, and Behavior in the New World Wood Warblers Director of Dissertation: Donald B. Miles In a rapidly changing world, species are faced with habitat alteration, changing climate and weather patterns, changing community interactions, novel resources, novel dangers, and a host of other natural and anthropogenic challenges. Conservationists endeavor to understand how changing ecology will impact local populations and local communities so efforts and funds can be allocated to those taxa/ecosystems exhibiting the greatest need. Ecological morphological and functional morphological research form the foundation of our understanding of selection-driven morphological evolution. Studies which identify and describe ecomorphological or functional morphological relationships will improve our fundamental understanding of how taxa respond to ecological selective pressures and will improve our ability to identify and conserve those aspects of nature unable to cope with rapid change. The New World wood warblers (family Parulidae) exhibit extensive taxonomic, behavioral, ecological, and morphological variation.
    [Show full text]
  • Download This PDF File
    Koedoe 19: 49-62(1976) A REVISED CHECK-LIST OF BIRDS IN THE KALAHARI GEMSBOK NATIONAL PARK M. G. L. MILLS Kalahari Gemsbok National Park Private Bag X890 P.O. Gemsbokpark 8815 Abstract - A more complete list of the birds in the Kalahari Gemsbok National Park is given, including the results of three and a half years of observations. A total of 214 species have now been identified for the Park, among which 75 are resident throughout the year, 37 are migrants, 14 are nomads and 88 are vagrants. Introduction Several lists of the birds in the Kalahari Gemsbok National Park, Republic of South Africa, have been published in recent years (de Villiers 1958; Prozesky and Haagner 1962; Broekhuysen, Broekhuysen, Martin, Martin, Martin and Morgan 1968; Maclean 1970). With the exception of Maclean (1970) these lists have been drawn up by the authors during short visits to the Park. However, Maclean only worked in the southern area for 19 months. This list represents three and a half years observations over the entire Park, with additional records from Liversidge (pen. comm.). Several groups of birds, notably the rap tors, pipits, larks and warblers, are difficult to identify in the field. Where any doubt exists as to the identity of a species, it has been omitted unless a specimen has been taken. Systematic List An indication of the status of each species in the Park is given accord­ ing to the following key (after Kemp 1974); R - resident (found in the Park throughout the year) M - migrant (a species known to come to South Africa on a regular annual migration) N - nomad (a regular, temporary visitor to the area) V - vagrant (an irregular, temporary visitor to the area).
    [Show full text]
  • Phylloscopus Sibilatrix
    Phylloscopus sibilatrix -- (Bechstein, 1793) ANIMALIA -- CHORDATA -- AVES -- PASSERIFORMES -- SYLVIIDAE Common names: Wood Warbler; Pouillot siffleur European Red List Assessment European Red List Status LC -- Least Concern, (IUCN version 3.1) Assessment Information Year published: 2015 Date assessed: 2015-03-31 Assessor(s): BirdLife International Reviewer(s): Symes, A. Compiler(s): Ashpole, J., Burfield, I., Ieronymidou, C., Pople, R., Wheatley, H. & Wright, L. Assessment Rationale European regional assessment: Least Concern (LC) EU27 regional assessment: Least Concern (LC) At both European and EU27 scales this species has an extremely large range, and hence does not approach the thresholds for Vulnerable under the range size criterion (Extent of Occurrence 10% in ten years or three generations, or with a specified population structure). The population trend appears to be stable, and hence the species does not approach the thresholds for Vulnerable under the population trend criterion (30% decline over ten years or three generations). For these reasons the species is evaluated as Least Concern within both Europe and the EU27. Occurrence Countries/Territories of Occurrence Native: Albania; Andorra; Armenia; Austria; Azerbaijan; Belarus; Belgium; Bosnia and Herzegovina; Bulgaria; Croatia; Cyprus; Czech Republic; Denmark; Estonia; Finland; France; Georgia; Germany; Greece; Hungary; Ireland, Rep. of; Italy; Latvia; Liechtenstein; Lithuania; Luxembourg; Macedonia, the former Yugoslav Republic of; Malta; Moldova; Montenegro; Netherlands; Norway; Poland; Romania; Russian Federation; Serbia; Slovakia; Slovenia; Spain; Canary Is. (to ES); Sweden; Switzerland; Turkey; Ukraine; United Kingdom Vagrant: Faroe Islands (to DK); Iceland; Portugal; Gibraltar (to UK) Population The European population is estimated at 7,060,000-11,100,000 pairs, which equates to 14,100,000-22,100,000 mature individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Adult Brood Parasites Feeding Nestlings and Fledglings of Their Own Species: a Review
    J. Field Ornithol., 69(3):364-375 ADULT BROOD PARASITES FEEDING NESTLINGS AND FLEDGLINGS OF THEIR OWN SPECIES: A REVIEW JANICEC. LORENZANAAND SPENCER G. SEALY Departmentof Zoology Universityof Manitoba Winnipeg,Manitoba R3T 2N2 Canada Abstract.--We summarized 40 reports of nine speciesof brood parasitesfeeding young of their own species.These observationssuggest that the propensityto provisionyoung hasnot been lost entirely in brood parasitesdespite the belief that brood parasiticadults abandon their offspringat the time of laying.The hypothesisthat speciesthat participatein courtship feeding are more likely to provisionyoung was not supported:provisioning of young has been observedin two speciesof brood parasitesthat do not courtshipfeed. The function of this provisioningis unknown, but we suggestit may be: (1) a non-adaptivevestigial behavior or (2) an adaptation to ensure adequatecare of parasiticyoung. The former is more likely the case.Further studiesare required to determinewhether parasiticadults commonly feed their genetic offspring. ADULTOS DE AVES PARAS•TICASALIMENTANDO PICHONES Y VOLANTONES DE SU PROPIA ESPECIE: UNA REVISION Sinopsis.--Resumimos40 informes de nueve especiesde avesparasiticas que alimenaron a pichonesde su propia especie.Las observacionessugieren que la propensividadde alimentar a los pichonesno ha sido totalmente perdida en las avesparasiticas, no empecea la creencia de que los parasiticosabandonan su progenie al momento de poner los huevos.La hipttesis de que las especiesque participan en cortejo de alimentacitn, son milspropensas a alimentar los pichonesno tuvo apoyo.Las observacionesde alimentacitn a pichonesse han hecho en dos especiesparasiticas cuyo cortejo no incluye la alimentacitn de la pareja. La funcitn de proveer alimento se desconoce.No obstante,sugerimos que pueda ser: 1) una conducta vestigialno adaptativa,o 2) una adaptacitn parc asegurarel cuidado adecuadode los pi- chonesparasiticos.
    [Show full text]
  • Polistes Wasps and Their Social Parasites: an Overview
    Ann. Zool. Fennici 43: 531–549 ISSN 0003-455X Helsinki 29 December 2006 © Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 2006 Polistes wasps and their social parasites: an overview Rita Cervo Dipartimento di Biologia Animale e Genetica, University of Florence, via Romana 17, I-50125 Florence, Italy (e-mail: rita.cervo@unifi.it) Received 10 Dec. 2005, revised version received 29 Nov. 2006, accepted 6 May 2006 Cervo, R. 2006: Polistes wasps and their social parasites: an overview. — Ann. Zool. Fennici 43: 531–549. Severe brood care costs have favoured the evolution of cheaters that exploit the paren- tal services of conspecifics or even heterospecifics in both birds and social insects. In Polistes paper wasps, three species have lost worker castes and are dependent on hosts to produce their sexuals, while other species use hosts facultatively as an alternative to caring for their own brood. This paper offers an overview of the adaptations, strategies and tricks used by Polistes social parasites to successfully enter and exploit host social systems. Moreover, it also focuses on the analogous solutions adopted by the well-known brood parasite birds, and stresses the evolutionary convergence between these two phy- logenetically distant taxa. A comparative analysis of life-history patterns, as well as of phylogenetic relationships of living facultative and obligate parasitic species in Polistes wasps, has suggested a historical framework for the evolution of social parasitism in this group. As with avian brood parasites, the analysis of adaptation and counter adaptation dynamics should direct the future approach for the study of social parasitism in Polistes wasps.
    [Show full text]
  • Nest Size Matters: Common Cuckoos Prefer to Parasitize Larger Nests of Oriental Reed Warblers
    Nest Size Matters: Common Cuckoos Prefer to Parasitize Larger Nests of Oriental Reed Warblers Longwu Wang Guizhou Normal University School of Life Sciences https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7020-4397 Gangbin He Guizhou Normal University Canchao Yang Hainan Normal University Anders Pape Møller Universite Paris-Saclay Wei Liang ( [email protected] ) Hainan Normal University Research Keywords: brood parasitism, common cuckoo, nest preference, nest size, Oriental reed warbler Posted Date: February 18th, 2021 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-204677/v1 License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. Read Full License Page 1/13 Abstract Background Avian brood parasites leave parental care of their offspring to foster parents. Theory predicts that parasites should select for large host nests when they have sucient available host nests at a given time. We developed an empirical experimental design to address this hypothesis by studying nest choice of common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) among nests of its Oriental reed warbler (Acrocephalus orientalis) hosts. Results We presented two groups of experimental nests: 1) nest dyads comprise one large and one small articial nest from reed leaves, and 2) nest triads tied together use the modied old own warbler nests including enlarged, reduced and medium sized nests to elicit parasitism by common cuckoos. We predicted that cuckoos prefer larger nests over medium sized ones, and over the smallest nest. Our experimental ndings show that common cuckoo females generally prefer large nests over medium or small sized nests. Furthermore, experiments showed that cuckoo parasitism was signicantly more common than in previous studies of the same warbler population.
    [Show full text]