Modeling Second-Language Learning from a Psychological Perspective

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Modeling Second-Language Learning from a Psychological Perspective Modeling Second-Language Learning from a Psychological Perspective Alexander S. Rich Pamela J. Osborn Popp David J. Halpern Anselm Rothe Todd M. Gureckis Department of Psychology, New York University asr443,pamop,david.halpern,anselm,todd.gureckis @nyu.edu { } Abstract which finished in third place for the en es data set, second place for es en, and third place for Psychological research on learning and mem- fr en. ory has tended to emphasize small-scale lab- oratory studies. However, large datasets of Learning and memory has been a core focus of people using educational software provide op- psychological science for over 100 years. Most portunities to explore these issues from a of this work has sought to build explanatory the- new perspective. In this paper we describe ories of human learning and memory using rela- our approach to the Duolingo Second Lan- tively small-scale laboratory studies. Such studies guage Acquisition Modeling (SLAM) com- have identified a number of important and appar- petition which was run in early 2018. We ently robust phenomena in memory including the used a well-known class of algorithms (gradi- ent boosted decision trees), with features par- nature of the retention curve (Rubin and Wenzel, tially informed by theories from the psycho- 1996), the advantage for spaced over massed prac- logical literature. After detailing our mod- tice (Ruth, 1928; Cepeda et al., 2006; Mozer et al., eling approach and a number of supplemen- 2009), the testing effect (Roediger and Karpicke, tary simulations, we reflect on the degree to 2006), and retrieval-induced forgetting (Ander- which psychological theory aided the model, son et al., 1994). The advent of large datasets and the potential for cognitive science and pre- such as the one provided in the Duolingo SLAM dictive modeling competitions to gain from challenge may offer a new perspective and ap- each other. proach which may prove complementary to lab- 1 Introduction oratory scale science (Griffiths, 2015; Goldstone and Lupyan, 2016). First, the much larger sam- Educational software that aims to teach people ple sizes may help to better identify parameters new skills, languages, and academic subjects have of psychological models. Second, datasets cover- become increasingly popular. The wide-spread ing more naturalistic learning situations may allow deployment of these tools has created interest- us to test the predictive accuracy of psychological ing opportunities to study the process of learning theories in a more generalizable fashion (Yarkoni in large samples. The Duolingo shared task on and Westfall, 2017). Second Lanugage Acquisition Modeling (SLAM) Despite these promising opportunities, it re- was a competitive modeling challenge run in early mains unclear how much of current psychologi- 2018 (Settles et al., 2018). The challenge, orga- 1 cal theory might be important for tasks such as nized by Duolingo , a popular second language the Duolingo SLAM challenge. In the field of ed- learning app, was to use log data from thousands ucation data mining, researchers trying to build of users completing millions of exercises to pre- predictive models of student learning have typi- dict patterns of future translation mistakes in held- cally relied on traditional, and interpretable, mod- out data. The data was divided into three sets cov- els and approaches that are rooted in cognitive en es ering Spanish speakers learning English ( ), science (e.g., Atkinson, 1972b,a; Corbett and An- es en English speakers learning Spanish ( ), and derson, 1995; Pavlik and Anderson, 2008). How- fr en English speakers learning French ( ). This ever, a recent paper found that state-of-the-art re- paper reports the approach used by our team, sults could be achieved using deep neural net- 1http://duolingo.com works with little or no cognitive theory built in (so 223 Proceedings of the Thirteenth Workshop on Innovative Use of NLP for Building Educational Applications, pages 223–230 New Orleans, Louisiana, June 5, 2018. c 2018 Association for Computational Linguistics called “deep knowledge tracing”, Piech et al., instance correctly, and designing a model that can 2015). Khajah, Lindsey, & Mozer (2016) com- achieve high performance using this feature set. pared deep knowledge tracing (DKT) to more standard “Bayesian knowledge tracing” (BKT) 2.1 Feature Engineering models and showed that it was possible to equate We used a variety of features, including features the performance of the BKT model by additional directly present in the training data, features con- features and parameters that represent core aspects structed using the training data, and features that of the psychology of learning and memory such as use information external to the training data. Ex- forgetting and individual abilities (Khajah et al., cept where otherwise specified, categorical vari- 2016). An ongoing debate remains in this com- ables were one-hot encoded. munity whether using flexible models with lots of data can improve over more heavily structured, 2.1.1 Exercise features theory-based models (Tang et al., 2016; Xiong We encoded the exercise number, client, session, et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2017). format, and duration (i.e., number of seconds to For our approach to the SLAM competition, we complete the exercise), as well as the time since decided to use a generic and fairly flexible model the user started using Duolingo for the first time. structure that we provided with hand-coded, psy- chologically inspired features. We therefore posi- 2.1.2 Word features tioned our entry to SLAM somewhat in between Using spaCy2, we lemmatized each word to pro- the approaches mentioned above. Specifically, we duce a root word. Both the root word token and the used gradient boosting decision trees (GBDT, Ke original token were used as categorical features. et al., 2017) for the model structure, which is a Due to their high cardinality, these features were powerful classification algorithm that is known to not one-hot encoded but were preserved in single perform well across various kinds of data sets. columns and handled in this form by the model (as Like deep learning, GBDT can extract complex described below). interactions among features, but it has some ad- Along with the tokens themselves we encoded vantages including faster training and easier inte- each instance word’s part of speech, morphologi- gration of diverse inputs. cal features, and dependency edge label. We no- We then created a number of new ticed that some words in the original dataset were psychologically-grounded features for the paired with the wrong morphological features, SLAM dataset covering aspects such as user per- particularly near where punctuation had been re- severance, learning processes, contextual factors, moved from the sentence. To fix this, we re- and cognate similarity. After finding a model that processed the data using Google SyntaxNet3. provided the best held-out performance on the test We also encoded word length and several word data set, we conducted a number of “lesioning” characteristics gleaned from external data sources. studies where we selectively removed features Research in psychology has suggested certain from the model and re-estimated the parameters in word features that play a role in how difficult a order to assess the contribution of particular types word is to process, as measured by how long read- of features. We begin by describing our overall ers look at the word as well as people’s perfor- modeling approach, and then discuss some of the mance in lexical-decision and word-identification lessons learned from our analysis. tasks. Two such features that have somewhat inde- pendent effects are word frequency (i.e., how often 2 Task Approach does the word occur in natural language; Rayner, 1998) and age-of-acquisition (i.e., the age at which We approached the task as a binary classification children typically exhibit the word in their vo- problem over instances. Each instance was a sin- cabulary; Brysbaert and Cortese, 2011; Ferrand gle word within a sentence of a translation exer- et al., 2011). We therefore included a feature that cise and the classification problem was to predict encoded the frequency of each word in the lan- whether a user would translate the word correctly guage being acquired, calculated from Speer et al. or not. Our approach can be divided into two components—constructing a set of features that is 2https://spacy.io/ informative about whether a user will answer an 3https://github.com/ljm625/syntaxnet-rest-api 224 (2017), and a feature that encoded the mean age- 0 indicating the same time, 0.25 indicating a rela- of-acquisition (of the English word, in English tive shift by 6 hours, etc.). We then discretized this native speakers), derived from published age-of- variable into 20-minute bins and computed the en- acquisition norms for 30,000 words (Kuperman tropy of the empirical frequency distribution over et al., 2012), which covered many of the words these bins. A lower entropy score indicated less present in the dataset. Additionally, words shar- variability in the times of day a user started their ing a common linguistic derivation (also called exercises. “cognates”; e.g., “secretary” in English and “sec- The entropy score might also give an indication retario” in Spanish), are easier to learn than words for context effects on users’ memory. A user prac- with dissimilar translations (De Groot and Keijzer, ticing exercises more regularly is more likely to be 2000). As an approximate measure of linguis- in the same physical location when using the app, tic similarity, we used the Levenshtein edit dis- which might result in better memory of previously tance between the word tokens and their transla- studied words (Godden and Baddeley, 1975). tions scaled by the length of the longer word. We found translations using Google Translate4 and 2.1.4 Positional features calculated the Levenshtein distance to reflect the To account for the effects of surrounding words letter-by-letter similarity of the word and its trans- on the difficulty of an instance, we created several lation (Hyyro¨, 2001).
Recommended publications
  • Universidad De Almería
    UNIVERSIDAD DE ALMERÍA MÁSTER EN PROFESORADO DE EDUCACIÓN SECUNDARIA OBLIGATORIA Y BACHILLERATO, FORMACIÓN PROFESIONAL Y ENSEÑANZA DE IDIOMAS ESPECIALIDAD EN LENGUA INGLESA Curso Académico: 2015/2016 Convocatoria: Junio Trabajo Fin de Máster: Spaced Retrieval Practice Applied to Vocabulary Learning in Secondary Education Autor: Héctor Daniel León Romero Tutora: Susana Nicolás Román ABSTRACT Spaced retrieval practice is a learning technique which has been long studied (Ebbinghaus, 1885/1913; Gates, 1917) and long forgotten at the same time in education. It is based on the spacing and the testing effects. In recent reviews, spacing and retrieving practices have been highly recommended as there is ample evidence of their long-term retention benefits, even in educational contexts (Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh, Nathan & Willingham, 2013). An experiment in a real secondary education classroom was conducted in order to show spaced retrieval practice effects in retention and student’s motivation. Results confirm the evidence, spaced retrieval practice showed higher long-term retention (26 days since first study session) of English vocabulary words compared to massed practice. Also, student’s motivation remained high at the end of the experiment. There is enough evidence to suggest educational institutions should promote the use of spaced retrieval practice in classrooms. RESUMEN La recuperación espaciada es una técnica de aprendizaje que se lleva estudiando desde hace muchos años (Ebbinghaus, 1885/1913; Gates, 1917) y que al mismo tiempo ha permanecido como una gran olvidada en los sistemas educativos. Se basa en los efectos que producen el repaso espaciado y el uso de test. En recientes revisiones de la literatura se promueve encarecidamente el uso de estas prácticas, ya que aumentan la retención de recuerdos en la memoria a largo plazo, incluso en contextos educativos (Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh, Nathan & Willingham, 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • Learning Efficiency Correlates of Using Supermemo with Specially Crafted Flashcards in Medical Scholarship
    Learning efficiency correlates of using SuperMemo with specially crafted Flashcards in medical scholarship. Authors: Jacopo Michettoni, Alexis Pujo, Daniel Nadolny, Raj Thimmiah. Abstract Computer-assisted learning has been growing in popularity in higher education and in the research literature. A subset of these novel approaches to learning claim that predictive algorithms called Spaced Repetition can significantly improve retention rates of studied knowledge while minimizing the time investment required for learning. SuperMemo is a brand of commercial software and the editor of the SuperMemo spaced repetition algorithm. Medical scholarship is well known for requiring students to acquire large amounts of information in a short span of time. Anatomy, in particular, relies heavily on rote memorization. Using the SuperMemo web platform1 we are creating a non-randomized trial, inviting medical students completing an anatomy course to take part. Usage of SuperMemo as well as a performance test will be measured and compared with a concurrent control group who will not be provided with the SuperMemo Software. Hypotheses A) Increased average grade for memorization-intensive examinations If spaced repetition positively affects average retrievability and stability of memory over the term of one to four months, then consistent2 users should obtain better grades than their peers on memorization-intensive examination material. B) Grades increase with consistency There is a negative relationship between variability of daily usage of SRS and grades. 1 https://www.supermemo.com/ 2 Defined in Criteria for inclusion: SuperMemo group. C) Increased stability of memory in the long-term If spaced repetition positively affects knowledge stability, consistent users should have more durable recall even after reviews of learned material have ceased.
    [Show full text]
  • Osmosis Study Guide
    How to Study in Medical School How to Study in Medical School Written by: Rishi Desai, MD, MPH • Brooke Miller, PhD • Shiv Gaglani, MBA • Ryan Haynes, PhD Edited by: Andrea Day, MA • Fergus Baird, MA • Diana Stanley, MBA • Tanner Marshall, MS Special Thanks to: Henry L. Roediger III, PhD • Robert A. Bjork, PhD • Matthew Lineberry, PhD About Osmosis Created by medical students at Johns Hopkins and the former Khan Academy Medicine team, Os- mosis helps more than 250,000 current and future clinicians better retain and apply knowledge via a web- and mobile platform that takes advantage of cutting-edge cognitive techniques. © Osmosis, 2017 Much of the work you see us do is licensed under a Creative Commons license. We strongly be- lieve educational materials should be made freely available to everyone and be as accessible as possible. We also want to thank the people who support us financially, so we’ve made this exclu- sive book for you as a token of our thanks. This book unlike much of our work, is not under an open license and we reserve all our copyright rights on it. We ask that you not share this book liberally with your friends and colleagues. Any proceeds we generate from this book will be spent on creat- ing more open content for everyone to use. Thank you for your continued support! You can also support us by: • Telling your classmates and friends about us • Donating to us on Patreon (www.patreon.com/osmosis) or YouTube (www.youtube.com/osmosis) • Subscribing to our educational platform (www.osmosis.org) 2 Contents Problem 1: Rapid Forgetting Solution: Spaced Repetition and 1 Interleaved Practice Problem 2: Passive Studying Solution: Testing Effect and 2 "Memory Palace" Problem 3: Past Behaviors Solution: Fogg Behavior Model and 3 Growth Mindset 3 Introduction Students don’t get into medical school by accident.
    [Show full text]
  • Testing Effect”: a Review
    Trends in Neuroscience and Education 5 (2016) 52–66 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Trends in Neuroscience and Education journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tine Review article Neurocognitive mechanisms of the “testing effect”: A review Gesa van den Broek a,n,1, Atsuko Takashima a,1, Carola Wiklund-Hörnqvist b,c, Linnea Karlsson Wirebring b,c,d, Eliane Segers a, Ludo Verhoeven a, Lars Nyberg b,d,e a Radboud University, Behavioural Science Institute, 6500 HC Nijmegen, The Netherlands b Umeå University, Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging (UFBI), 901 87 Umeå, Sweden c Umeå University, Department of Psychology, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden d Umeå University, Department of Integrative Medical Biology, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden e Umeå University, Department of Radiation Sciences, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden article info abstract Article history: Memory retrieval is an active process that can alter the content and accessibility of stored memories. Of Received 16 October 2015 potential relevance for educational practice are findings that memory retrieval fosters better retention Received in revised form than mere studying. This so-called testing effect has been demonstrated for different materials and po- 27 May 2016 pulations, but there is limited consensus on the neurocognitive mechanisms involved. In this review, we Accepted 30 May 2016 relate cognitive accounts of the testing effect to findings from recent brain-imaging studies to identify Available online 2 June 2016 neurocognitive factors that could explain the testing effect. Results indicate that testing facilitates later Keywords: performance through several processes, including effects on semantic memory representations, the se- Testing effect lective strengthening of relevant associations and inhibition of irrelevant associations, as well as po- Retrieval tentiation of subsequent learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Probabilistic Models of Student Learning and Forgetting
    Probabilistic Models of Student Learning and Forgetting by Robert Lindsey B.S., Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, 2008 A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Colorado in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Computer Science 2014 This thesis entitled: Probabilistic Models of Student Learning and Forgetting written by Robert Lindsey has been approved for the Department of Computer Science Michael Mozer Aaron Clauset Vanja Dukic Matt Jones Sriram Sankaranarayanan Date The final copy of this thesis has been examined by the signatories, and we find that both the content and the form meet acceptable presentation standards of scholarly work in the above mentioned discipline. IRB protocol #0110.9, 11-0596, 12-0661 iii Lindsey, Robert (Ph.D., Computer Science) Probabilistic Models of Student Learning and Forgetting Thesis directed by Prof. Michael Mozer This thesis uses statistical machine learning techniques to construct predictive models of human learning and to improve human learning by discovering optimal teaching methodologies. In Chapters 2 and 3, I present and evaluate models for predicting the changing memory strength of material being studied over time. The models combine a psychological theory of memory with Bayesian methods for inferring individual differences. In Chapter 4, I develop methods for delivering efficient, systematic, personalized review using the statistical models. Results are presented from three large semester-long experiments with middle school students which demonstrate how this \big data" approach to education yields substantial gains in the long-term retention of course material. In Chapter 5, I focus on optimizing various aspects of instruction for populations of students.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases and Their Effect on Mobile Learning: the Example of the Continued Influence Bias and Negative Suggestion Effect
    Cognitive Biases and their Effect on Mobile Learning: The Example of the Continued Influence Bias and Negative Suggestion Effect Christina Schneegass Fiona Draxler LMU Munich LMU Munich Munich, Germany Munich, Germany [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT Cognitive biases can consciously and subconsciously affect the way we store and recall previously learned information. In the use case of mobile learning, biases such as the Negative Suggestion Effect (NSE) can make us think a statement is correct because we wrongfully selected it in a previous multiple-choice test. In some cases, the suggestion effect is so persistent that even corrections can not stop us from drawing assumptions based on the misinformation we once learned. This effect is called the Continued Influence Bias (CIB). To avoid the creation of such incorrect and sometimes persistent memories, learning applications need to be designed carefully. In this position paper, we discuss the influence of the presented number of answer options, feedback, and lesson design on the strength of the NSE and CIB and provide recommendations for countermeasures. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for third-party components of this work must be honored. For all other uses, contact the owner/author(s). CHI’20, April 25, 2020, Honolulu HI, USA © 2020. Proceedings of the CHI 2020 Workshop on Detection and Design for Cognitive Biases in People and Computing Systems Copyright held by the owner/author(s).
    [Show full text]
  • Spacing Learning Events Over Time: What the Research Says
    Spacing Learning Events Over Time: What the Research Says Will Thalheimer, PhD A Work-Learning Research, Inc. Publication. © Copyright 2006 by Will Thalheimer. You may share this report as long as you share it intact and whole and don’t charge a price for it or include it in any priced event or product. Published February 2006 Spacing Learning Over Time Will Thalheimer, PhD How to cite this report using APA style: Thalheimer, W. (2006, February). Spacing Learning Events Over Time: What the Research Says. Retrieved November 31, 2006, from http://www.work-learning.com/catalog/ Obviously, you should substitute the date on which the document was downloaded for the fictitious November 31 date. Published 2006 The Author: Will Thalheimer is a learning-and-performance consultant and research psychologist specializing in learning, cognition, memory, and performance. Dr. Thalheimer has worked in the workplace learning field, beginning in 1985, as an instructional designer, simulation architect, project manager, product leader, trainer, consultant, and researcher. He has a PhD from Columbia University and an MBA from Drexel University. He founded Work- Learning Research in 1998 to help client organizations create learning environments to maximize performance and help learning professionals—instructional designers, e-learning developers, trainers, performance consultants, talent managers, and chief learning officers— utilize research-based knowledge to build effective learning-and-performance solutions. Dr. Thalheimer can be contacted at [email protected] or at the Work-Learning Research phone number below for inquiries about learning audits, workshops, speaking engagements, high-level instructional design, and consulting on e-learning, classroom training, learning measurement, learning strategy, and business strategy for learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Applying Cognitive Science Principles to Promote Durable and Efficient
    Outline 1. Do tests only measure learning, or can they also promote learning? Testing effect 2. Should you review/practise the material you are Applying Cognitive trying to learn soon after you first encounter the material, or should you wait? Science Principles to Spacing effect Promote Durable and 3. During practice, should items of the same Efficient Learning type/topic be grouped together, or should they be mixed with items of other types/topics? Interleaving Sean Kang, PhD • General conclusions 2 August 2019 1 “A curious peculiarity of our memory is that things are impressed better by Purpose of Tests / Quizzes active than by passive repetition. I mean that in learning (by heart, for example), when we almost know the piece, it pays better to wait and • Traditionally, an assessment tool recollect by an effort from within, than to look at the book again. If we recover the words in the former way, we shall probably know them the next time round; if in the latter way, we shall very likely need the book once more.” • But testing does not merely measure the (William James, 1890, Principles of Psychology) contents of memory “If you read a piece of text through twenty times, you will not learn it by heart • Taking a test can serve as a learning so easily as if you read it ten times while attempting to recite from time to time and consulting the text when your memory fails.” opportunity, enhancing memory retention to a (Sir Francis Bacon, 1620, Novum organum) greater extent than additional studying… the testing effect “Exercise in repeatedly recalling a thing strengthens the memory.” (Aristotle, 4th century B.C., De Memoria et Reminiscentia) (also referred to as the benefit of retrieval practice) First published study: Abbott (1909) The Testing Effect Testing effect: How does it work? • Journal of Educational Psychology, 1989: 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Learners' Choices and Beliefs About Self-Testing
    Self-testing 1 Running Head: SELF-TESTING Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing Nate Kornell University of California, Los Angeles Lisa K. Son Barnard College In press at Memory. Please do not quote without permission. Address correspondence to: Nate Kornell UCLA Department of Psychology 1285 Franz Hall Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563 Phone: 310-825-7028 Fax: 310-206-5895 Email: [email protected] Self-testing 2 Abstract Students have to make scores of practical decisions when they study. We investigated the effectiveness of, and beliefs underlying, one such practical decision: the decision to test oneself while studying. Using a flashcards-like procedure, participants studied lists of word pairs. On the second of two study trials, participants either saw the entire pair again (pair mode) or saw the cue and attempted to generate the target (test mode). Participants were either asked to rate the effectiveness of each study mode (Experiment 1) or to choose between the two modes (Experiment 2). The results demonstrated a mismatch between metacognitive beliefs and study choices: Participants (incorrectly) judged that the pair mode resulted in the most learning, but chose the test mode most frequently. A post-experimental questionnaire suggested that self-testing was motivated by a desire to diagnose learning rather than a desire to improve learning. Keywords: Self-testing, testing effect, flashcards, judgments of learning Self-testing 3 Acknowledgments We thank Robert A. Bjork for his guidance and support and Bridgid Finn for her suggestions and help in conducting the experiments. Grant 29192G from the McDonnell Foundation to Robert A. Bjork supported this research.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning and Memory Strategy Demonstrations for the Psychology
    LEARNING & MEMORY 1 Learning and Memory Strategy Demonstrations for the Psychology Classroom Jennifer A. McCabe Goucher College 2013 Instructional Resource Award recipient Author contact information: Jennifer A. McCabe Department of Psychology Goucher College 1021 Dulaney Valley Road Baltimore, MD 21204 E-mail: [email protected] Phone: 410-337-6558 Copyright 2014 by Jennifer A. McCabe. All rights reserved. You may reproduce multiple copies of this material for your own personal use, including use in your classes and/or sharing with individual colleagues as long as the author’s name and institution and the Office of Teaching Resources in Psychology heading or other identifying information appear on the copied document. No other permission is implied or granted to print, copy, reproduce, or distribute additional copies of this material. Anyone who wishes to produce copies for purposes other than those specified above must obtain the permission of the author. LEARNING & MEMORY 2 Overview This 38-page document contains an introduction to the resource, background information on learning and memory strategies, a summary of research on undergraduate student metacognition with regard to these strategies, and a collection of classroom demonstrations that allows students to experience real-time the effectiveness of specific learning and memory strategies. References are included at the end of the document. Table of Contents Page I. Introduction 3 II. Background Information on Strategies and Metacognition 4 III. Classroom Demonstrations of Learning and Memory Strategies 5 A. Deep Processing 6 B. Self-Reference Effect 10 C. Spacing Effect 12 D. Testing Effect 16 E. Imagery 17 F. Chunking 22 G.
    [Show full text]
  • How and When Do Students Use Flashcards? Kathryn T
    This article was downloaded by: [Washington University in St Louis] On: 20 August 2012, At: 06:14 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Memory Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pmem20 How and when do students use flashcards? Kathryn T. Wissman a , Katherine A. Rawson a & Mary A. Pyc b a Department of Psychology, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA b Department of Psychology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA Version of record first published: 06 Jun 2012 To cite this article: Kathryn T. Wissman, Katherine A. Rawson & Mary A. Pyc (2012): How and when do students use flashcards?, Memory, 20:6, 568-579 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.687052 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
    [Show full text]
  • Mechanisms Underlying the Spacing Effect in Learning: a Comparison of Three Computational Models
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General In the public domain 2018, Vol. 147, No. 9, 1325–1348 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000416 Mechanisms Underlying the Spacing Effect in Learning: A Comparison of Three Computational Models Matthew M. Walsh Kevin A. Gluck, Glenn Gunzelmann, The RAND Corporation, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania and Tiffany Jastrzembski Air Force Research Laboratory, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio Michael Krusmark Jay I. Myung, Mark A. Pitt, and Ran Zhou Air Force Research Laboratory, Wright-Patterson Air Force The Ohio State University Base, Ohio, and L-3 Communications, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio The spacing effect is one of the most widely replicated results in experimental psychology: Separating practice repetitions by a delay slows learning but enhances retention. The current study tested the suitability of the underlying, explanatory mechanism in three computational models of the spacing effect. The relearning of forgotten material was measured, as the models differ in their predictions of how the initial study conditions should affect relearning. Participants learned Japanese–English paired associates presented in a massed or spaced manner during an acquisition phase. They were tested on the pairs after retention intervals ranging from 1 to 21 days. Corrective feedback was given during retention tests to enable relearning. The results of 2 experiments showed that spacing slowed learning during the acquisition phase, increased retention at the start of tests, and accelerated relearning during tests. Of the 3 models, only 1, the predictive performance equation (PPE), was consistent with the finding of spacing-accelerated relearning. The implications of these results for learning theory and educational practice are discussed.
    [Show full text]