Where Propositional Arguments and Participial Relative Clauses Meet

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Where Propositional Arguments and Participial Relative Clauses Meet Where propositional arguments and participial relative clauses meet Éva Dékány & Ekaterina Georgieva {dekanyeva/ekaterina.georgieva}@nytud.hu Research Institute for Linguistics (Budapest) Workshop “On the nouniness of propositional arguments” (DGfS43) University of Freiburg, 23–26 Feb 2021 Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 1 / 74 Introduction Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 2 / 74 Aims We aim at explaining why it is crosslinguistically common that participial relative clauses (pRCs) and deverbal nominalizations (DVNs) share the same suffix. (1) [Ali-nin pişir-diğ-i] yemek Ali-gen cook-dik-poss:3sg food ‘the food Ali cooked’ [MST, pRC] (2) Ali-nin kitab-I oku-duğ-un-u Ali-gen book-acc read-dik-poss:3sg-acc ‘(that) Ali read the book’ (as a direct obj) [MST, DVN] (Kornfilt 2003) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 3 / 74 Aims This is called the participle-nominalizer polysemy. It is observed in Uralic, Altaic, Quechua and Tibeto-Burman languages (Koptjevskaja-Tamm 1993: 2.2.5; Serdobolskaya & Paperno 2006; Shagal 2018; Noonan 1997). " Given that this is a wide-spread phenomenon, it requires a principled, structure-based account, where the exponents for the participle and the DVN are connected. We investigate this phenomenon in Udmurt and Khanty (Uralic) as well as Modern Standard Turkish and Uyghur, with some reference to Kazakh (Turkic). We argue that no real polysemy is involved, but we use it as a descriptive term. Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 4 / 74 Claims The participle-nominalizer polysemy arises when the structure of DVNs properly contains the structure of pRCs. The containment involves a null nouny element present in DVNs and absent in pRCs. This containment can arise in three different configurations. " the participle–nominalizer polysemy corresponds to different underlying structures; no one size fits all analysis Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 5 / 74 The discussion of the participle-nominalizer polysemy will be framed in the analyses dealing with nominalizations and mixed extended projections (see Borer 1997; Borsley & Kornfilt 2000; Alexiadou 2001; Alexiadou et al. 2011, a.o.) The participle-nominalizer polysemy is somewhat reminiscent of another highly debated topic: noun complement clauses (NCC) and (finite) relative clauses (Arsenijević 2009; Haegeman 2012; de Cuba 2017, a.o.) Even though our main goal is not to approach our data from the perspective of the phenomena found in the case of finite embedded clauses, we will discuss (non-finite) NCCs Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 6 / 74 Roadmap 1 Introduction 2 Unifying participles and nominalizations 3 Case study 1: Udmurt pRC: bare, DVN: mixed projection 4 Case study 2: Modern Standard Turkish pRC: nominalized, DVN: mixed projection 5 Case study 3: Uyghur DVN: AspP modifies a covert N 6 Conclusion 7 Appendix Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 7 / 74 Unifying participles and nominalizations Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 8 / 74 Assumptions about the shared suffix Null hypothesis: the shared suffix spells out ¶ a functional head in the extended VP (with or without a nominalizer) · a nominalizing head which requires an extended VP as its complement. " Thus, the suffix is either "verby" or "nouny". Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 9 / 74 Assumptions about the shared suffix We claim that the suffix is a verbal head in the languages under consideration The suffix has an aspectual meaning (thus, we label it Asp, see also Embick 2004) In Udmurt, Khanty and Uyghur, the perfect past/evidential marker is diachronically related to the ptcp suffix Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 10 / 74 Against a nominalizer analysis of the shared suffix pRCs in Udmurt have no discernible nominal properties the shared suffix occurs in other, clearly verbal contexts, e.g., in passives or as perfect tense forms in Udmurt, Kazakh and Uyghur (Ótott-Kovács 2016; Asarina 2011) pRCs and DVNs in Modern Standard Turkish do have nominal properties, but DVNs are not pluralizable (Kornfilt 2003); the presence of n is said to correlate with Num marking (Alexiadou et al. 2011) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 11 / 74 Background assumption about pRCs similar to finite RCs (but have no relativizer and may lack a left periphery) compose with the head N via an FP (3) DP FP D v+P F0 vP v+ F NP verb noun Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 12 / 74 Proposal We claim that there are two parameters that may vary across languages: (i) pRCs: extended VPs with or without nominalization (4) DP (5) DP FP D FP D AspP F0 DP F0 vP Asp F NP AspP D F NP -sfx verb noun vP Asp noun -sfx verb How to tell if a pRC is nominalized: genitive subject, poss morphology, D-like morphology Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 13 / 74 Proposal (ii) DVNs: the extended VP of the participle is turned into a mixed extended projection or it modifies a covert noun (6) DP (7) FP AspP D AspP F0 vP Asp vP Asp F NP -sfx -sfx verb verb covert noun How to tell if there is a covert N: it alternates with an overt N with the same properties (On mixed extended projections see Borer 1997; Borsley & Kornfilt 2000; Alexiadou 2001, a.o.) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 14 / 74 Case study 1: Udmurt pRC: bare, DVN: mixed projection Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 15 / 74 pRCs and DVNs in Udmurt the suffix -m is employed in both pRCs and DVNs (8) [kil’em ar-in pukt-em] korka ˘ ˘ last year-ine build-asp house ‘the house that was built last year’ [pRC] (9) [Kol’a-len tolon kik pispu mertt-em-ez] ˘ Kolya-gen yesterday two tree.acc plant-asp-poss:3sg šońer evel. ˘ ˘ true neg.cop ‘Kolya’s planting two trees yesterday was wrong/(a) bad (idea).’ [DVN] Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 16 / 74 A closer look at Udmurt pRCs Udmurt pRCs are eventive (3-way division: eventive, resultative, stative participles, cf. Kratzer 2000; Embick 2004); they contain an extended verbal projection: 1 manner/duration adverb 3 2 causative and frequentative morphology 3 3 Acc object 3 (10) [pići dirjaz tros pol kńiga ˘ young time.during.poss:3sg many times book.acc liddź-it-il-em] murt ˘ ˘ ˘ read-caus-freq-asp person ‘a/the person who was made to read a book several times as a child’ They have no nominal properties: overt subjects are not Gen marked; there is no Poss suffix on the participle; no D-like morphology. Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 17 / 74 Udmurt pRCs: analysis Relative clauses in Udmurt have the structure in (11): (11) DP FP D AspP F0 vP Asp F NP -m verb noun pRCs are truncated clauses, AspPs no nominal layer on top of AspP (accounts for the lack of nominal properties) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 18 / 74 The subject of Udmurt pRCs The subject is either PRO or, if overt, is instrumental-marked: (12) [PRO/Ivan-en liddź-il-em] kńiga ˘ ˘ Ivan-ins read-freq-asp book ‘the book read by someone/Ivan’ Nominative subjects are ruled out (in the present-day language): (13)* [mi mertt-em] pispu-os 1pl.nom plant-asp tree-pl ‘the trees planted by us’ (Georgieva & Ótott-Kovács 2016) It has been argued that Udmurt pRCs can also have Gen subjects, but we show that they are possessors (see the Appendix) (see Brykina & Aralova 2012; Serdobolskaya et al. 2012; Georgieva & Ótott-Kovács 2016; Georgieva 2018; Dékány & Georgieva 2020) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 19 / 74 The subject of Udmurt pRCs (cont.) We suggest that internal to the non-finite AspP, no structural case is available for the subject (Dékány & Georgieva 2020) ! the subject is a PRO or is expressed in a PP (14) (14) FP AspP F0 vP Asp NP F -m PP vP noun subject-Ins verb NB: the agent of passives is also Ins marked (F. Gulyás & Speshilova 2014) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 20 / 74 Udmurt pRCs: implications Our analysis is in line with several proposals for Turkic languages according to which (certain) pRCs are reduced (Krause 2001; Hale 2002; Aygen 2011) We have also adopted a structural approach to case in order to explain the Ins-subjects of pRCs The reduced vs full CP analysis of pRCs in Turkic also often involves a structural approach to case (e.g., Krause 2001) Recent studies: pRCs are (bare) vPs + configurational approach to case (e.g., Satık 2020; “nominative as caselessness”, cf. Kornfilt & Preminger 2015) " Problematic, given that Nom-subjects are ungrammatical in Udmurt pRCs Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 21 / 74 DVNs in Udmurt Udmurt DVNs contain an extended verbal projection (cf. frequentative morphology, Acc object, adverbial modification) Nominal properties: Gen subject, Poss morphology on the verb (15) [Kol’a-len tolon kik pispu mertt-em-ez] ˘ Kolya-gen yesterday two tree.acc plant-asp-poss:3sg šońer evel. ˘ ˘ true neg.cop ‘That Kolya planted two trees yesterday was wrong/(a) bad (idea).’ (see Brykina & Aralova 2012; Serdobolskaya et al. 2012; Georgieva & Ótott-Kovács 2016; Georgieva 2018; Dékány & Georgieva 2020) Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 22 / 74 DVNs in Udmurt Proposal: DVNs in Udmurt are mixed extended projections the extended verbal projection is embedded directly under D the subject becomes a derived possessor (16): (16) DVNs in Udmurt DP 0 subject-geni D AspP D -poss vP Asp -m ti verb Dékány & Georgieva Propositional arguments & participles Nouniness (DGfS 43) 23 / 74 Support for the structure of DVNs possessors are Gen marked (17) [Pet’a-len puni-jez] ute.
Recommended publications
  • Turkish Bibliography
    Turkish Bibliography Reference Grammars Deny, J. 1921. Grammaire de la langue turque. Ernest Leroux, Paris. von Gabain, Annette. 1974. Alttürkische Grammatik. Otto Harrassowitz, Wiesbaden. Golstein, B. 1997. Grammaire du turc. L’Harmattan, Paris. Halasi-Kun, T. 1916. Türkische Grammatik. Sammlung Göschen, Berlin. Kissling, Hans-Joachim. 1960. Osmanish-türkische Grammatik. Harrassowitz, Wiesbaden. Kononov, A.N. 1956. Grammatika sovremennogo tureckogo literaturnogo jazyka. Izdatel’stvo Akademii Nauk SSSR, Moscow. Kornfilt, Jaklin. 1997. Turkish. Routledge, London. Lewis, Geoffrey L. 1967. Turkish grammar. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Németh, J. 1962. Turkish grammar. Mouton, The Hague. [bew. van Halasi-Kun 1916] Swift, Lloyd B. 1963. A reference grammar of Modern Turkish. Indiana University Publications, Bloomington. Underhill, Robert. 1976. Turkish grammar. MIT Press, Cambridge. Vaughan, Thomas. 1709. A grammar of the Turkish language. J. Humfreys, London. [facsimile edition: Menston, 1968] Monographs and Dissertations Adamovic, Milan. 1985. Konjugationsgeschichte der Türkischen Sprache. E.J. Brill, Leiden. Aygen, Gülşat. 2002. Finiteness, case, and clausal architecture. Dissertation, Harvard University. Dietrich, Ayşe Pamir. 1997. Towards a syntactic characterization of passives, ergatives and middles in Russian and Turkish. Dissertation, Cornell University. Erguvanlı, Eser Ermine. 1984. The function of word order in Turkish grammar. University of California Press, Berkeley. Erkü, Feride. 1983. Discourse pragmatics and word order in Turkish. Dissertation, University of Minnesota. Foster, Joseph F. 1970. On some phonological rules of Turkish. Dissertation, University of Illinois. Göksel, Aslı. 1993. Levels of representation and argument structure in Turkish. Dissertation, University of London. Johanson, Lars. 1971. Aspekt im Türkischen: Vorstudien zu einer Beschreibung des türkeitürkischen Aspektsystems. Almqvist och Wiksell, Stockholm. Kardeştuncer, Aino E. 1982. Theoretical implications of Turkish vowel harmony.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Introduction in This Paper, We Analyze Embedded Clauses in Uyghur, a Turkic Language Spoken in the Xinjiang Uyghur Autonomous Region of China
    GENITIVE SUBJECT LICENSING IN∗ UYGHUR SUBORDINATE CLAUSES ALYA ASARINA &JEREMY HARTMAN Massachusetts Institute of Technology 1 Introduction In this paper, we analyze embedded clauses in Uyghur, a Turkic language spoken in the Xinjiang Uyghur Autonomous Region of China. We argue that genitive case on the subjects of these clauses is licensed by agreement with a clause-external D head. We also argue that these clauses are full CPs. Putting these claims together, we show that Uyghur exhibits agreement and case-assignment over a CP boundary, a configuration that is inconsistent with Chomsky’s (1998) Phase Impenetrability Condition (PIC). The Uyghur data thus support adopting the weaker version of the PIC proposed by Chomsky (2001). Consider the following examples of embedding in Uyghur.1 (1) Noun Complement: [ men-1N ket-ken-(liq) ] heqiqet-im muhim [ I-gen leave-RAN-(LIQ) ] fact-1sg.poss important ‘The fact that I left is important.’ (Uyghur) (2) Verb complement: Ötk¨ur [ Ajg¨ul-nuN ket-ken-(lik)-i-ni ] di-d-i Ötkür [ Aygül-gen leave-RAN-(LIQ)-3.poss-acc ] say-past-3 ‘Ötkür said that Aygül left.’ (Uyghur) When the embedded clause is the complement of an overt noun, as in (1), possessor agreement is on the embedding noun. We will argue that the optional morpheme -liq in the above examples is a complementizer, and that examples like (1) thus display agreement across a CP boundary. In addition, we will argue that the clause in (2) is also embedded by a head noun, albeit a ∗We would like to thank Vera Gribanova, Sabine Iatridou, Jaklin Kornfilt, Shigeru Miyagawa, David Pesetsky, Masha Polinsky, Norvin Richards, Kirill Shklovsky, Donca Steriade, Yasu Sudo, and the audiences at WAFL VII, CUNY Syntax Supper and MIT Syntax Square for valuable comments and discussion.
    [Show full text]
  • Nominative As No Case at All: an Argument from Raising-To-Accusative in Sakha* JZŽ†• K™§•€†± B O“U§ P§U“†•U§ Syracuse University , January Óþõ¦
    Nominative as no case at all: an argument from raising-to-accusative in Sakha* JZ K§± b Ou§ P§uu§ Syracuse University , January óþÕ¦ Õ. Introduction In many contemporary approaches to the morphosyntax of case, one finds either explicit or implicit references to unmarked structural cases like nominative being assigned. For example: “For the Case/agreement systems, the uninterpretable features are φ-features of the probe and structural Case of the goal N. [...] Structural Case is not a feature of the probes (T, v), but it is assigned a value under agreement [...] e value assigned depends on the probe: nominative for T, accusative for v.” [Chomsky óþþÕ:ä, emphasis added] We refer to this as a positively-specified view of, e.g., nominative case; on this view, nominative only arises as the result of some “action” taken by the grammar (per Chomsky óþþþ, óþþÕ, for example, the action in question is φ-feature agreement). In this paper, we present a novel argument, based on raising-to-accusative constructions in Sakha (Turkic), against this positively-specified view of unmarked case (in particular, of nominative). Instead, we argue, the proper grammatical representation of unmarked cases is as the outright absence of any otherwise assigned case values on the noun phrase. ó. e central question Like other Turkic languages, Sakha has a construction in which the subject of an embedded clause raises to a position where it can receive accusative case (cf. Kornfilt ÕÉßß, Moore ÕÉÉ, on Turkish). A couple of representative examples are given in (Õ–ó): (Õ) min ehigi-ni [C t bügün kyaj-yax-xyt dien ] erem-mit-im (Sakha) I you-Zhh today win-¶±-ópl.«¶f that hope-£Z«±-Õsg.«¶f ‘I hoped you would win today.’ (ó) ehigi bihigi-ni [C t kyajtar-dy-byt dien ] xomoj-du-gut you we-Zhh lose-£Z«±-Õpl.«¶f that become.sad-£Z«±-ópl.«¶f ‘Y’all were disappointed that we lost.’ [Vinokurova óþþ¢:ìäÉ; annotations added following Baker & Vinokurova óþÕþ] *e authors’ names appear in alphabetical order.
    [Show full text]
  • In Eser Erguvanlı -Taylan (Ed.) the Verb in Turkish. Benjamins, Amsterdam, Pp.47-59
    In Eser Erguvanlı -Taylan (ed.) The Verb in Turkish. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp.47-59 A NOTE ON MOOD, MODALITY, TENSE AND ASPECT AFFIXES IN TURKISH1 Guglielmo Cinque University of Venice The limited goal of this contribution is to analyse the order of the mood, modality, tense and aspect, verbal suffixes of Turkish in the light of my (1999) proposal on the functional structure of the clause. My hope is that the exercise, besides explaining away certain apparent counterexamples to a rigid hierarchy of functional projections, may shed a partly new light on this area of the grammar of Turkish. In Cinque (1999), I examined the relative order of free (particles) and bound (suffixes) grammatical morphemes corresponding to mood, modality, tense, aspect and voice distinctions in the languages of the world. The recurrent picture that one finds in this domain is that they not only are rigidly ordered with respect to each other (as partly anticipated in such works as Bybee 1985, Foley and Van Valin 1984, and Dik 1989), but that each of the mood, modality, tense, aspect, and voice categories is made up, at a finer level, of a number of distinct heads, which also appear to be rigidly ordered. The striking match between the order of these grammatical heads and the order of the corresponding adverbs was further taken there to suggest a rich and articulated functional structure above the lexical VP of the clause, where each adverb class corresponds to a mood, modality, tense, aspect or voice head in a one-to-one fashion (as does the specifier to a head in a classical X-bar structure - Chomsky 1970, Kayne 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • 0Xxintroduction 1Xxlooking for Free Relatives in Turkish
    LOOKING FOR FREE RELATIVES IN TURKISH (AND THE UNEXPECTED PLACES THIS LEADS TO) 1 SABINE IATRIDOU Massachusetts Institute of Technology 0xxIntroduction Free Relatives, like I ate what(ever) he cooked have been described in a variety of languages. This paper started out as an investigation of Free Relatives in Turkish. As often happens, it ended up being about a variety of different things. 1xxLooking for Free Relatives in Turkish In Turkish, there are two constructions that look like Free Relatives (FRs), in the sense that they both appear to be headless. I will refer to them as Strategy I and II. Strategy I FRs, as in (1), have been discussed by Kornfilt (2005). They differ from headed relatives, e.g. (2), in being headless and in the placement of agreement morphemes. They are similar to the headed relatives in that they are both nominalized by -dik-, which, predictably, results in their subjects being in the Genitive. (1) Sen-in pişir-dik-ler-in-i yi-yeceğ-im. you-gen. cook-nom.-pl.-2.sg.-acc. eat-fut.-1.sg. ‘I will eat “the things” which you cooked.’ 1 This paper could not have been written without the help of Jaklin Kornfilt. For about a month before WAFL, Jaklin would answer multiple daily messages about how to say this or that in Turkish and she did so extremely patiently and goodnaturedly, never asking where I was headed nor why my questions were about such disjoined sets of phenomena. My debt to her is very large. I also greatly benefited from discussing different aspects of the paper with Rajesh Bhatt, Kai von Fintel, Roumi Pancheva, Sergei Tatevosov and Maziar Toosarvandani.
    [Show full text]
  • ON SOME INFINITIVAL WH CONSTRUCTIONS in TURKISH* Jaklin Kornfilt Syracuse University 0. Introduction the Aim of This Paper Is
    ON SOME INFINITIVAL WHCONSTRUCTIONS IN TURKISH* Jaklin Kornfilt Syracuse University 0. Introduction The aim of this paper is to present a syntactic phenomenon of Turkish concerning infinitival complements and to sketch a principled explanation which refers to more general characteristics of the language. The phenomenon in question consists of a deficiency of infinitival constructions: Turkish has neither infinitival relative clauses, nor infinitival WHquestions; yet, it does have infinitives. Representative examples for each assertion follow, along with corresponding English examples: Infinitival relative clauses: (1) * A h m e t Ayşe ye [jyjp [çp PRO e; oku mak] bir kitapj ] aldi Dat. readInf. a book buypast Intended reading: 'Ahmet bought Ayşe a book to read' (2) John bought Mary [jvjp a book; [çp PRO to read Cj 1 1 Infinitival WHqucstions: (3) *Ahmct Ayşe ye \QP PRO ne oku maki söyledi Dat. what readInf. saypast Some ol" the contents of this paper were presented to a meeting of the EUROTYP project of the European Science Foundation, Theme Group 3: Subordination and Complementation, at a special meeting on Infinitivals and NonFinite Complements, held al Grcgynog, Wales. I wish to thank Nigel Vincent, the group leader, for inviting me to that meeting, to Istvan Kenesei for initiating that invitation, and to the participants of that meeting for interesting discussion and suggestions, in particular to Istvan Kenesei, Jon Ortiz de Urbina, and Maria Luisa Rivero. A less developed version of this paper was published as: "Infinitival WHConstructions and Complementation in Turkish"; in Eurotyp Working Papers: Group 3: Subordination and Complementation, vol. 4; K. Börjars and N.
    [Show full text]
  • Proceedings of the First Workshop on Turkish, Turkic and The
    Tu+ 1: Proceedings of the first workshop on Turkish, Turkic and the languages of Turkey November 21 - November 22, 2015 University of Massachusetts, Amherst Edited by Faruk Akkus¸, Isa˙ Kerem Bayırlı, Deniz Ozyıldız¨ c 2018 Published by GLSA (Graduate Linguistics Student Association) Department of Linguistics University of Massachusetts Integrative Learning Center, 4th Floor 650 North Pleasant Street Amherst, MA 01003 U.S.A. [email protected] glsa.hypermart.net ISBN-13: 978-1983844027 ISBN-10: 1983844020 Thank you The 1st Workshop on Turkish, Turkic and the languages of Turkey (Tu+1) was held at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst on 21-22 November 2015, in collaboration with Yale University. In addition to the two invited talks by Sabine Iatridou (MIT) and Jaklin Kornfilt (Syracuse University), the workshop hosted 23 oral and poster presentations. The presenters came from 17 different institutions, 6 of which were non-US institutions. The articles in this proceedings volume are a selection of papers presented at Tu+1. Our hope is that this workshop will be held annually and that it will provide a platform for discussion of empirical and theoretical questions raised by Turkic languages and the languages spoken within Turkey. We are grateful to the linguistics departments at Yale and at UMass for sup- porting the workshop, morally, technically and financially. Rosetta Berger, Rajesh Bhatt, Robert Frank, Vincent Homer, John Kingston, Tom Maxfield and Michelle McBride deserve special mention. We would also like to thank all those who gave a helping hand during the workshop. We are grateful to the Graduate Linguistic Student Association for publishing this volume, to Leah Chapman for agreeing to design the cover, and last but not least, to our presen- ters and audience for making this workshop an enjoyable learning experience.
    [Show full text]
  • CV January 22 2015 Kornfilt Revised
    JAKLIN KORNFILT 4/16/15 2. 21. 2004 CURRICULUM VITAE Jaklin Kornfilt EDUCATONAL BACKGROUND Highest Degree: Ph.D.: Harvard University, March 1985 Title of Ph.D. dissertation: Case Marking, Agreement, and Empty Categories in Turkish EMPLOYMENT HISTORY May 2003 - present: Professor, Syracuse University August 1991 - May 2003: Associate Professor, Syracuse University Fall 1984 - August 1991: Assistant Professor, Syracuse University Fall 1983 - Fall 1984: Instructor, Syracuse University Academic Affiliations other than at Syracuse University (most recent, limited to the last 9 years): July 2014: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology: Visiting Scientist June, July 2012: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology: Visiting Scientist 2010/11: University of Stuttgart, Institute of Linguistics: Visiting Professor (as Humboldt Prize recipient) 2007/08: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology: Visiting Scientist June 2003: University of Leipzig: Visiting Professor PUBLICATIONS Books and Journals: Forthcoming: with C. Trips (University of Mannheim) (eds.): Phrasal Compounds from a Typological and Theoretical Perspective; special issue of the journal STUF (Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung); Berlin: Akademie Verlag; anticipated publication date: 2015 Under contract: J. Kornfilt Turkish Syntax; Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 2011 J. Kornfilt & J. Whitman (eds.) Studies in Syntactic Nominalization; special issue of Lingua (Vol. 121, #7); Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1159-1313. (Also online: www.sciencedirect.com)
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Leipzig Spring School on Linguistic Diversity; MPI-EVA Jaklin Kornfilt, Syracuse University and MPI-EVA : Topics in Turkic
    Leipzig Spring School on Linguistic Diversity; MPI-EVA Jaklin Kornfilt, Syracuse University and MPI-EVA : Topics in Turkic Syntax 26-28 March, 2008 [email protected] Some Observations on Turkish/Turkic RCs I. If there is an operator in Turkish RCs, it would need to be phonologically empty: Turkish RCs have no WH-pronouns. But they do have gaps, corresponding to the RC targets. Precedents for empty operators? Many European languages have WH-pronouns; some don't. Those which have them don't always exhibit them; instead, they might have a regular complementizer, or neither, as an option: (1) a. The sonata whichi I played ti b. The sonata thati I played ti c. The sonata I played (For an overview of issues connected to such constructions, cf. Comrie 1998, 1999.) Question: Is the element which looks like a regular complementizer (1b.) indeed a complementizer, or is it a "relative pronoun", but homophonous with the complementizer? Both views exist. Their distribution is different; e.g. WH-based relative pronouns can be preceded by a preposition, while that can't: (2) a. The piano on whichi I played the sonata ti b. *The piano on thati I played the sonata ti A mixed view (e.g. Pesetsky 1981): that is the complementizer, not a pronoun; its phrasal position is that of a head (C), not that of a phrase (Spec, CP—the position of the WH-based relative pronouns). The homophony is not a coincidence, nor are the differences in distribution. However, RCs with complementizers exhibit similar island effects that RCs with relative pronouns exhibit; e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • The Subject Position in Spanish Nominalized Infinitives
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 21 Issue 1 Proceedings from PLC 38 Article 2 3-2015 The Subject Position in Spanish Nominalized Infinitives Marcus Berger Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Berger, Marcus (2015) "The Subject Position in Spanish Nominalized Infinitives," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 21 : Iss. 1 , Article 2. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol21/iss1/2 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol21/iss1/2 For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Subject Position in Spanish Nominalized Infinitives This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol21/iss1/2 The Subject Position in Spanish Nominalized Infinitives Marcus Berger* 1 Introduction The Spanish Nominalized Infinitive (NI) has received significant attention in linguistic literature, especially recently, starting with Plann (1981) up through more recent work (Wilkins 1986, Pérez Vázquez 2002, Ramírez 2003, and others). The NI is characterized by an infinitive verb occurring in a decidedly nominal environment. (1) El bonito tiritar del follaje. the beautiful tremble.INF of.the foliage ‘The beautiful trembling of the foliage’ Example (1) shows the infinitive tremble being modified by a determiner, an adjective, and a PP complement. Many authors (Alexiadou et al. 2011, e.g.) have likened the Spanish NI to certain English gerund constructions, in that they both seem to fall on a continuum of nominality. Ross (1973) showed that English gerunds could be divided into separate constructions, some of which behaved more nominally than others.
    [Show full text]
  • Proceedings of the 3Rd Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics
    Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL -3) Volume 3 Edited by 2-4 March 2018 Öner Özçelik and Bloomington, Indiana Amber Kennedy Kent ii (C) ConCALL-3 Proceedings copyrighted by Indiana University, CeLCAR (C) All papers copyrighted by the authors 2018 Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL-3) Volume 3/edited by Dr. Öner Özçelik and Amber Kennedy Kent ISBN: 978-0-9961762-2-4 1. Central Asian Languages - Congresses For information on ConCALL-3 (held on 2-4 March 2018, Bloomington, Indiana), visit our website at http://www.iub.edu/~concall/ Published by Center for Languages of the Central Asian Region Eigenmann Hall 238 1900 E. Tenth Street Bloomington, IN 47406-7512 USA https://celcar.indiana.edu/ iii History of ConCALL The Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL) was founded in 2014 at Indiana University by the Center for Languages of the Central Asian Region (CeLCAR) under the leadership of Dr. Öner Özçelik, the residing director, with grants and contributions from the U.S. Department of Education (DOE) and several units at Indiana University, including the Ostrom Grant Programs, College of Arts and Humanities Center (CAHI), Inner Asian and Uralic National Resource (IAUNRC), College of Arts and Sciences, Sinor Research Institute for Inner Asian Studies (SRIFIAS), Department of Central Eurasian Studies (CEUS), and Department of Linguistics. To date, ConCALL's main sponsors are the DOE, CeLCAR, IAUNRC, and IU's Center for the Study of the Middle East (CSME). As the nation’s sole U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Proceedings of the 1St Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics
    Proceedings of the 1st Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL -1) Volume 1 Edited by 16- 17 May 2014 Dr. Öner Özçelik and Bloomington, Indiana Amber Kennedy Kent (C) ConCALL-1 2014 Proceedings copyrighted by Indiana University, CeLCAR (C) All papers copyrighted by the authors 2014 Proceedings of the 1st Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL) Volume 1/edited by Dr. Öner Özçelik and Amber Kennedy Kent ISBN: 978-0-9961762-0-0 1. Central Asian Languages - Congresses For information on ConCALL-1 (held on 16-17 May 2014, Bloomington, Indiana), visit our website at http://www.iub.edu/~celcar/ConCALL/ Published by Center for Languages of the Central Asian Region Eigenmann Hall 702 1900 E. Tenth Street Bloomington, IN 47406-7512 USA http://www.iub.edu/~celcar/ iii History of ConCALL The Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL) was founded in 2014 at Indiana University under the leadership of Dr. Öner Özçelik, the residing director of the Center for Languages of the Central Asian Region (CeLCAR), with grants and contributions from the U.S. Department of Education and several units at Indiana University, including the Ostrom Grant Programs, College of Arts and Humanities Center (CAHI), Inner Asian and Uralic National Resource, College of Arts and Sciences, School of Global and International Studies (SGIS), Sinor Research Institute for Inner Asian Studies (SRIFIAS), Department of Cen- tral Eurasian Studies (CEUS), and Department of Linguistics. As the nation’s sole U.S. Department of Education funded Language Resource Center focusing on the languages of the Central Asian Region, CeLCAR’s main mission is to strengthen and improve the nation’s capacity for teaching and learning Central Asian languages through teacher training, research, materials devel- opment projects, and dissemination.
    [Show full text]