<<

Florida State University Libraries

Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School

2014 Integrating Mainstream Criminological Theory into the Biosocial Perspective: An Empirical Analysis Joseph A. Schwartz

Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected] FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY

COLLEGE OF AND CRIMINAL

INTEGRATING MAINSTREAM CRIMINOLOGICAL THEORY INTO THE

BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

By

JOSEPH A. SCHWARTZ

A Dissertation submitted to the College of Criminology and in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Degree Awarded: Summer Semester, 2014 Joseph A. Schwartz defended this dissertation on May 22, 2014. The members of the supervisory committee were:

Kevin M. Beaver

Professor Directing Dissertation

Stephen J. Tripodi

University Representative

William B. Bales

Committee Member

Thomas G. Blomberg

Committee Member

The Graduate School has verified and approved the above-named committee members, and certifies that the dissertation has been approved in accordance with university requirements.

ii

To my beautiful and loving wife Jen, you are my best friend, greatest advocate, and the source of my happiness.

iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

First and foremost, I would like to acknowledge my mentor, Dr. Kevin Beaver. I am sure that whatever feeble attempt I make to demonstrate just how much of an impact Dr. Beaver has had on my life will be fully inadequate. Dr. Beaver’s patience, commitment, intellectual prowess, and generosity are simply unparalleled. I truly admire his endless hunger for knowledge and his fearless pursuit of it. Dr. Beaver has taught me so much more than the fundamental aspects of research and teaching, he has taught me to aggressively pursue my goals, the true value of hard work, and how to answer the questions I seek to answer. Perhaps even more importantly, he has taught me how to be a better person. Through example, he has shown me how to be a better husband, and one day, how to be a better father. I am certain that my efforts will pale in comparison, but I hope that I will be able to pass some of the knowledge and experiences he has instilled in me to my future students. Thank you so much for taking me under your wing, your efforts have not gone unnoticed and I will never forget the guidance and support you have provided.

I would also like to acknowledge the scholars who were generous enough with their time to serve on my dissertation committee: Dr. William Bales, Dean Thomas Blomberg, and Dr. Stephen Tripodi. Your support, guidance, and advice through this process has been invaluable not only in regard to the improvement of my dissertation but also to my development as a scholar and a criminologist. To receive input from scholars of this caliber is truly an honor and I am deeply grateful for the time and energy they have contributed to this project and my scholarly . I would like to especially acknowledge Dean Thomas Blomberg for his influential advice and guidance during the latter portion of my graduate career. I sincerely appreciate each and every conversation we have engaged in, and I thank you for your perpetual support.

I would also like to acknowledge the multitude of other scholars who I have had the honor of interacting with and learning from. First, I would like to thank the faculty in the College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at FSU for challenging me to reach my potential and for providing the necessary support along the way. In addition, I would like to sincerely thank the faculty of the Department of Criminal Justice at California State University, San Bernardino for sparking my interest in criminology and for encouraging me to pursue my goals.

iv

I would like to especially acknowledge the assistance of Dr. Gisela Bichler, Dr. Larry Gaines, Ms. Mary Schmidt, Dr. Pamela Schram, and Dr. Stephen Tibbetts. Without the encouragement of Dr. Schram I likely would not have pursued a graduate degree and without the guidance of Dr. Tibbetts I may still be unaware of the causal influences of biology on the underlying etiology of behavior. Thank you for all of your advice, support, and guidance during my entire career as a student, I am certain that I would not have been able to accomplish my goals without you.

I would also like to thank my fellow graduate students at Florida State. I have been fortunate enough to have forged many friendships during my time in Tallahassee. I cannot express how grateful I am for the times I was able to share with many of you. I can honestly say that I will look back fondly over my time as a graduate student at FSU, and I have plenty of stories to share with my future graduate students and colleagues (most of which revolve around conversations or experiences in the Hecht House basement). A special acknowledgement is in order for Eric Connolly and Joe Nedelec. I am certain that my time as a graduate student would have been far less productive and certainly less entertaining had I not been fortunate enough to cross paths with the two of you. Thank you both for your friendship, support, and guidance.

Words simply cannot describe the amount of support I have received from my family. I would like to sincerely thank my father, Joe Schwartz, and mother, Kim Schwartz, for their unwavering support and guidance over the course of my entire life. They both have instilled in me the importance of hard work and striving to achieve excellence in all I do. Thank you both for being such amazing parents. I would also like to thank my younger brother Allan Schwartz, whose strength, tenacity, and drive are enviable (as difficult as that is for an older brother to admit). I am excited to see what your future, with my future sister-in-law Brittany Hernandez, holds. I would also like to thank my younger sister, Shavaun Schwartz. I am certain that my trips back home would have been far less entertaining without our conversations. In addition, my fashion sense has certainly benefited from your advice (and ridicule, for that matter). I would also like to thank my grandmother, Ann West, and grandfather, Al Schwartz, both of whom have provided me with invaluable advice and support. Thank you both so much, I love you very much. Finally, I would like to thank my wife’s family who have taken me in as one of their own, and allowed me to move their daughter across the country. I am extremely grateful for their patience, understanding, and support over the past several years.

v

I have saved my final acknowledgement for my beautiful and loving wife, Jen. I simply cannot imagine going through this entire process without her love, support, and friendship. You are my rock, and I can say with certainty that I would not have been able to attempt, let alone accomplish, this achievement without you. I am fully aware of the sacrifices you have made in order for me to pursue my dreams. How you were able to tolerate all of the times I have babbled on about something I was working on, or the long nights and weekends I had to spend working, or the holidays we had to spend away from family and friends back home without even one complaint is nothing short of remarkable and exemplifies just how simply amazing you are. The fact that you were so willing to sacrifice your own goals and dreams so that I could selfishly pursue my own speaks directly to your generous and loving nature. Simply put, you are the most beautiful and amazing woman I have ever met, as long as I have your support I know that I can accomplish anything. Thank you from the bottom of my heart.

I’m sure that I have omitted several others who have guided or supported me along the way. I have been fortunate enough to have come across many, many people who have impacted my life in so many different ways and to name them all here would require far more space than has been allotted. I would like to sincerely thank each and every one of you.

This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due to Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

List of Tables ...... xi

List of Figures ...... xiii

Abstract ...... xiv

CHAPTER 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ...... 1

1.1 Research Questions ...... 4

1.2 Outline...... 7

CHAPTER 2 THE BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE ...... 10

2.1 Behavior Genetic Methodologies ...... 15

2.1.1 Twin Studies ...... 19

2.1.2 Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart (MZA) ...... 29

2.1.3 Adoption Studies ...... 31

2.1.4 Family-Based Studies ...... 33

2.1.5 Nonshared Environment Studies ...... 35

2.2 Behavior Genetic Research and Antisocial Behavior ...... 37

2.2.1 Meta-Analyses of Behavior Genetic Research on Antisocial Behavior ...... 38

2.2.2 Discussion of Findings ...... 46

2.3 Gene-Environment Interplay ...... 50

2.3.1 Gene-Environment Interaction (G × E) ...... 51

2.3.2 Gene-Environment Correlation (rGE) ...... 53

2.3.3 Gene-Environment Interplay and Antisocial Behavior ...... 56

2.4 Summary and Discussion ...... 60

CHAPTER 3 CONVENTIONAL CRIMINOLOGICAL THEORY: EMPIRICAL FINDINGS AND CONCERNS...... 64

vii

3.1 Rational Choice Theory ...... 67

3.1.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Rational Choice Theory ...... 69

3.1.2 The Empirical Status of Rational Choice Theory ...... 73

3.2 Social Learning Theory ...... 81

3.2.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Social Learning Theory ...... 83

3.2.2 The Empirical Status of Social Learning Theory ...... 87

3.3 Classic Strain Theory ...... 93

3.3.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Classic Strain Theory ...... 94

3.3.2 The Empirical Status of Classic Strain Theory ...... 98

3.4 Social Bonding Theory ...... 101

3.4.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Social Bonding Theory ...... 104

3.4.2 The Empirical Status of Social Bonding Theory ...... 106

3.5 Summary and Discussion ...... 111

CHAPTER 4 SITUATING EXISTING CRIMINOLOGY THEORIES WITHIN THE BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE ...... 114

4.1 Precursors to the Biosocial Integration Model...... 118

4.1.1 Biosocial Theories of Antisocial Behavior ...... 119

4.1.2 Preliminary Models of Biosocial Integration ...... 122

4.2 The Biosocial Integration Model ...... 126

4.3 Summary and Discussion ...... 129

CHAPTER 5 METHODS ...... 135

5.1 Data ...... 135

5.1.1 Description of the Analytic Sample ...... 140

5.2 Measures ...... 142

viii

5.2.1 Antisocial Behavior ...... 142

5.2.2 Prevalence of Illicit Drug Use...... 143

5.2.3 Alcohol Use ...... 144

5.2.4 Rational Choice Theory ...... 145

5.2.5 Social Learning Theory...... 146

5.2.6 Classic Strain Theory ...... 148

5.2.7 Social Bonding Theory ...... 151

5.2.8 Demographic Covariates ...... 155

5.3 Analytic Plan ...... 155

5.3.1 Univariate ACE Decomposition Model ...... 156

5.3.2 Multivariate Regression ...... 160

5.3.3 DeFries-Fulker Analysis ...... 160

5.3.4 Gene-Environment Interaction ...... 163

5.3.5 Research Question 1 ...... 166

5.3.6 Research Question 2 ...... 167

5.3.7 Research Question 3 ...... 167

5.3.8 Research Question 4 ...... 168

CHAPTER 6 RESULTS ...... 173

6.1 Research Question 1 ...... 173

6.1.1 Antisocial Behavior ...... 174

6.1.2 Alcohol Use ...... 176

6.1.3 Illicit Drug Use ...... 177

6.2 Research Question 2 ...... 178

6.2.1 Antisocial Behavior ...... 179

6.2.2 Alcohol Use ...... 181 ix

6.2.3 Illicit Drug Use ...... 181

6.3 Research Question 3 ...... 182

6.3.1 Antisocial Behavior ...... 183

6.3.2 Alcohol Use ...... 184

6.3.3 Illicit Drug Use ...... 184

6.4 Research Question 4 ...... 185

6.4.1 Antisocial Behavior ...... 185

6.4.2 Alcohol Use ...... 187

6.4.3 Illicit Drug Use ...... 189

CHAPTER 7 DISCUSSION ...... 203

7.1 Summary of Results ...... 203

7.2 Limitations ...... 211

7.3 Future Directions ...... 214

APPENDICES ...... 220

A. ITEMS FOR ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR MEASURES ...... 220

B. UNIVARIATE ACE AND THRESHOLD LIABILITY MODEL RESULTS ...... 222

C. IRB APPROVAL FORMS...... 237

REFERENCES ...... 240

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH ...... 266

x

LIST OF TABLES

Table 2.1: Levels of Genetic Relatedness for Family Studies ...... 34

Table 5.1: Description of the Final Analytical Sample by Level of Genetic Relatedness ...... 170

Table 5.2: Overview of the Waves at which Each Included Measure is Assessed ...... 171

Table 6.1: Descriptive Statistics for all Included Measures ...... 191

Table 6.2: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 1 ...... 192

Table 6.3: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 2 ...... 193

Table 6.4: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 3 ...... 194

Table 6.5: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 4 ...... 195

Table 6.6: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 1 ...... 196

Table 6.7: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 2 ...... 197

Table 6.8: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 3 ...... 198

Table 6.9: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 4 ...... 199

Table 6.10: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 1 ...... 200

Table 6.11: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 2 ...... 201

Table 6.12: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 3 ...... 202

Table 7.1: Tabulation of Significant Associations between Theoretical Measures and Antisocial Outcomes Organized by Theory and Model Estimated ...... 218

Table B.1: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1 and Wave 2 Antisocial Behavior Measures ...... 222

Table B.2: Threshold Liability Model Results for the Wave 3 and Wave 4 Antisocial Behavior Measures ...... 223

Table B.3: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1 and Wave 2 Alcohol Use Measures ...... 224

Table B.4: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 3 and Wave 4 Alcohol Use Measures ...... 225

Table B.5: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1, Wave 2, and Wave 3 Illicit Drug Use Measures ...... 226

xi

Table B.6: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Thoughtfully-Reflective Decision Making (TRDM) Measure...... 227

Table B.7: Univariate ACE Model Results for Peer Antisocial Behavior, Peer Illicit Drug Use, and Peer Alcohol Use Measures ...... 228

Table B.8: Univariate ACE Model Results for Classic Strain Measures ...... 229

Table B.9: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Parental Attachment Measures ...... 230

Table B.10: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 School Attachment Measures ...... 231

Table B.11: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Neighborhood Attachment Measures ...... 232

Table B.12: Threshold Liability Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Peer Attachment Measures ...... 233

Table B.13: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Parental Involvement Measures ...... 234

Table B.14: Threshold Liability Model Results for Wave 3 Community Involvement Measures ...... 235

Table B.15: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Religious Commitment Measures ...... 236

xii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 3.1: Graphical Example of a Gene-Environment Interaction ...... 63

Figure 4.1: Visual Representation of Existing Biosocial Theories ...... 132

Figure 4.2: Visual Representation of Preliminary Models of Biosocial Integration ...... 133

Figure 4.3: Visual Representation of the Biosocial Integration Model ...... 134

Figure 5.1: Path Diagram of a Univariate ACE Model ...... 172

Figure 7.1: Visual Representation of the Percentage of All Significant Associations by Model Estimated...... 219

xiii

ABSTRACT

Within the field of criminology, there is no shortage of theoretical perspectives. While these theoretical perspectives are quite diverse, they do share a common thread: an exclusive focus on social factors that contribute to criminal behavior. Despite the limited explanatory power of both classic and more recent criminological theories, an overt sociological focus persists. In direct contrast, the biosocial perspective offers a more comprehensive explanation of behavior, with a focus on both biological and environmental influences. Despite the contributions of the biosocial perspective in elucidating the underlying etiology of antisocial behavior, there is currently a paucity of theories which can be effectively situated within the biosocial perspective. In an effort to spark theoretical development within the biosocial perspective, this dissertation proposes a biosocial integration model which allows for various forms of theoretical development and integration. In addition, four mainstream criminological theories—rational choice theory, social learning theory, classic strain theory, and social bonding theory—were empirically examined using genetically sensitive research designs in an attempt to fit such theories within the biosocial perspective. The results revealed three key findings. First, nearly all (more than 80 percent) of the measures examined were significantly influenced by genes. Second, while multivariate regression models identified a large number of significant associations between key theoretical concepts and antisocial behavior, many of these associations fell from statistical significance after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. Third, even after controlling for genetic influences, some theoretical concepts were significantly associated with antisocial behavior and substance use. Additional models revealed that several theoretical concepts also significantly moderated genetic influences on the examined outcomes. The findings are contextualized within the extant literature and suggestions for future research and theoretical development are discussed.

xiv

CHAPTER 1

STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM

We are like the blind man who tries to describe the whole elephant after having only touched its trunk. Our theories and empirical explanations of as it varies over time, space, people, and the life course are woefully incomplete.

-Daniel S. Nagin (2007, pp. 260-261)

The above quote was offered by Nagin (2007) during his 2006 Sutherland Address to the

American Society of Criminology and exemplifies one of the most pervasive and concerning issues within the field of criminology. More specifically, the theories which constitute the very bedrock of criminological research tend to be limited in focus and leave the vast majority of the variation in antisocial and criminal behaviors unexplained (Bernard & Snipes, 1996; Elliott,

Ageton, & Cantor, 1979; Liska, Krohn, & Messner, 1989; Weisburd & Piquero, 2008). As with any scientific discipline, theoretical perspectives are critical in shaping both empirical research and knowledge regarding phenomena directly related to the discipline (Kuhn, 1996). Clearly, there is no shortage of criminological theories and an even greater number of refinements and reformulations of such theories (for example, see Lilly, Cullen, & Ball, 2011). Despite the numerous theories aiming to explain variation in criminal behavior, quantity cannot sufficiently supplant quality and the theoretical development within the field of criminology to date has resulted in “a million modest little studies that produce a million tiny conflicting results”

(Bernard & Snipes, 1996, p. 302).

In an attempt to contextualize this observation, Weisburd and Piquero (2008) examined all studies published in Criminology, widely regarded as the leading journal in the field of

1 criminology, between 1968 and 2005 in an effort to determine how well criminologists explain criminal outcomes. While over 1,000 articles were published in Criminology over this period of time, only 259 empirically assessed criminological theory in some way and only 169 included the information necessary to estimate the statistics included in the study. The authors then estimated the percentage of variance explained in criminal outcomes examined in each of the included articles using R2 coefficients. The study yielded two sets of findings that are directly relevant to this dissertation. First, the average percentage of variance explained by studies assessing existing criminological theories was modest at best and tended to hover between 20 and 30 percent of the variance in the examined criminological outcomes. The authors reiterate this finding by pointing out that “many models leave 80-90 percent of the variance unaccounted for” (pp. 472-473). This first set of findings provides a direct indication of the inability of existing theoretical perspectives to adequately explain criminological phenomena. Second, the findings indicated that the percentage of variance explained by criminological studies has not improved over time and may actually be declining. When summarizing their results, the authors point out that “[c]riminology does not appear to be evolving along a science-like track over time, one centered on improvement” (p. 474).

Based on this overall pattern of findings, it seems quite clear that existing theoretical perspectives, and the further refinement of such perspectives, may have taken the field of criminology as far as possible. While Weisburd and Piquero (2008) call for “[a] two-pronged approach of better theory and better and more sophisticated theory testing” (p. 494), such a call is basically a simple reiteration of the procedures the field of criminology has relied upon in the past. In this way, the root cause of the overall lack of explanatory power among existing criminological theory seems to stem directly from one of two sources: (1) either the theories

2 themselves; or (2) the paradigm from which the theories were developed. In either case, even extensive revisions to such theories are not likely to result in a noticeable change in the proportion of variance explained by concepts central to such theories. Rather, a more dramatic and sweeping change may be necessary to increase the explanatory power of the theories which drive research in the field of criminology. While existing criminological theories vary in their scope and emphasized concepts, the vast majority of these theories do share a common characteristic—they focus exclusively on the social and environmental factors that contribute to criminal behavior.

A developed and rigorous line of research in other disciplines, such as psychology, has revealed that the most powerful and effective explanations of antisocial behavior are comprised of both environmental and biological factors (Moffitt, 2005a; Pinker, 2002; Rowe, 1994).

Recently, this concerted emphasis on both environmental and biological influences on antisocial and criminal behavior has manifested within the field of criminology in the form of biosocial criminology (Beaver, 2013a; Walby & Carrier, 2010; Wright & Cullen, 2012). The biosocial perspective does not contradict the current criminological paradigm, rather, and as this aims to illustrate, the conventional criminological perspective can be situated within the larger biosocial perspective. In this way, conventional theoretical perspectives may still hold merit and can be augmented using the biosocial perspective to explain a greater portion of the variance in antisocial outcomes (Walsh, 2002). Despite the promise of the biosocial perspective and the theoretical development stemming from the incorporation of biosocial concepts into behavioral theories in other disciplines, the field of criminology has been slow to embrace the biosocial perspective (Beaver, 2013a; Wright & Boisvert, 2009; Wright & Cullen, 2012).

3

The need for a more integrated and biologically-conscious criminology is nothing new.

At least three previous ASC presidents have directly voiced the need for a greater integration of biology within the field of criminology (Cullen, 2011; Rafter, 2008; Zahn, 1999). While these calls have been largely ignored by the vast majority of criminologists, such apathy comes at the price of theoretical and empirical stagnation (as directly evidenced by Weisburd & Piquero,

2008). At the current time, the field of criminology finds itself in a peculiar situation: highly lauded theoretical perspectives only explain a menial proportion of the variance in criminal behavior; a massive amount of time and pages have been spent attempting to modify and revise existing theories with virtually no progress; and some of the most prominent criminologists continue to call for more of the same (see for example Sampson, 2013). In order to progress criminology as a scientific discipline, it is first necessary to discard the underlying assumption that all variation in human behavior is the direct result of socialization. Failing to make such a change endangers the future of the field of criminology.

1.1 Research Questions

Despite the recent traction gained by biosocial criminology, there has been virtually no formal effort made to integrate the existing criminological theories into the biosocial perspective.

What makes this revelation all the more surprising is the numerous calls for such integration over the past several decades within the field of criminology. In her address to the American Society of Criminology (ASC), Margaret Zahn (1999) openly called for the incorporation of biology into the field of criminology. More specifically, she noted “[i]t is important, if not imperative, that we become aware of the biological and biochemical bases for behavior and incorporate them, where relevant, into our theory about violent crime and its consequences” (emphasis in original,

4 p. 3). Even after such a direct call for integration, existing criminological theories remain nearly devoid of biological or biosocial factors related to criminal and delinquent behavior (for an exception see Moffitt, 1993). This dissertation attempts to directly address this gap in the literature by examining whether mainstream criminological theories fit within the biosocial perspective. More specifically, four mainstream criminological theories—rational choice theory, social learning theory, classic strain theory, and social bonding theory—will be empirically examined within the confines of genetically sensitive research designs in an effort to determine whether such theoretical perspectives fit within the larger biosocial perspective.

The current study will examine four research questions directly related to the task of fitting existing criminological theory within the biosocial perspective. These four research questions are:

Rational Choice Theory:

1. Does adequately controlling for genetic influences alter the overall association between

thoughtfully reflective decision making (TRDM) and antisocial behavior?

Social Learning Theory:

2. Does exposure to delinquent peers significantly predict subsequent antisocial behavior

even after taking genetic influences into account?

Classic Strain Theory:

3. After controlling for genetic influences, does strain, as defined by Merton (1934),

significantly predict antisocial behavior?

Social Bonding Theory:

4. Does controlling for genetic influences reduce or eliminate the association between

attachment, involvement, and commitment and antisocial behavior?

5

Each of these four Research Questions, and the existing research relevant to each, will be discussed in more detail in subsequent chapters. These four Research Questions are aimed at attempting to fit each of the examined theories within the biosocial perspective. As discussed more thoroughly in subsequent chapters, a necessary prerequisite to integrating each theory into the biosocial perspective is to isolate the effects of each theory’s primary concepts on antisocial behavior net of the effects of genetic influences. Concepts which fail to predict antisocial outcomes in such a context are likely subject to genetic confounding and may have no real influence on antisocial behavior when such confounding is properly controlled. Alternatively, theoretical concepts which significantly explain variance in a given antisocial outcome within the confines of a genetically sensitive modeling strategy should be viewed as highly robust predictors of antisocial behavior. Biosocial integration can only be carried out with theories comprised of concepts that fall into the latter category. In this way, the four Research Questions examined in the current dissertation aims to accomplish two related goals: (1) examine whether concepts central to each examined theory continue to predict a wide range of antisocial outcomes while controlling for genetic influences; and, more importantly (2) whether the examined conventional theories themselves can be integrated into the larger biosocial perspective.

Based on these goals and the proposed Research Questions, the current dissertation aims to make three distinct contributions to the extant literature. First, no previous studies have examined the predictive ability of the four examined theoretical perspectives within a genetically informed modeling strategy. In this way, the current empirical status of each perspective remains relatively unknown. While it remains fully possible that each perspective explains a significant portion of the overall variance in antisocial behavior, it also remains possible that previously examined associations are the direct result of genetic confounding. Second, the

6 current dissertation provides a novel biosocial integration strategy which provides multiple strategies aimed at incorporating existing criminological theories into the larger and more comprehensive biosocial perspective. The biosocial integration model builds on previous forms of theoretical integration and directly addresses many of the limitations present within previous efforts. Third, the current dissertation attempts to situate each of the four theoretical perspectives examined into the biosocial perspective in an effort to increase their explanatory power and scope.

1.2 Outline

Based on the overarching goals of the current dissertation, a thorough review of multiple lines of research is necessary and will be offered. Due to the large amount of information reviewed, it is necessary to provide an overview of the general organization of this dissertation.

Chapter 2 provides an overview of the biosocial perspective and is organized into four sections.

First, an overview of standard social science methodologies (SSSMs) and the methodological limitations they possess will be presented. Second, a comprehensive overview of the most common behavior genetic (BG) modeling strategies will be presented. Importantly, BG modeling strategies allow for the estimation of both genetic and environmental contributions to various behavioral outcomes. Even more importantly, these modeling strategies also allow for the isolation between environmental influences on a given outcome while adequately controlling for genetic influences. Third, the literature examining genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior will be reviewed. Fourth, the various ways in which genetic and environmental influences combine to generate variation in behavioral outcomes, a phenomenon referred to as gene-environment interplay, will be outlined.

7

Chapter 3 provides an overview of the current state of rational choice, social learning, classic strain, and social bonding theories. The chapter is divided into four sections, each of which provides an overview of the key assumptions and theoretical concepts implicated in each of the examined theories. In addition, each section provides an overview of the empirical evidence bearing on each of the examined theories. The primary purpose of Chapter 3 is to provide an overview of the empirical validity of each of the examined theories, and to provide an overview of the development of each theory over time.

Chapter 4 is divided into two sections and formally proposes the biosocial integration model. The first section provides an overview of theoretical integration and the various forms of integration that have been previously proposed. In addition, the first section of Chapter 4 provides a summary of precursors to the biosocial integration model including previous biosocial theories (Ellis, 2005; Robinson & Beaver, 2009) and preliminary models of biosocial integration

(e.g., Fishbein, 1990; Walsh, 2000; 2002). The second section of Chapter 4 presents the biosocial integration model and outlines the primary advantages of this model over previous models of biosocial integration.

Chapter 5 presents the methods used to address the four Research Questions and is divided into three subsections. The first section provides an overview of the data and analytic sample used in the current dissertation. The second section provides a detailed description of the measures used. The third and final section provides a detailed description of the analytic techniques used to answer each of the four Research Questions.

Chapter 6 presents the findings of the analyses described in the previous chapter and is divided into four sections. Each section presents the findings related to an individual theory and the related Research Question. In this way, the findings related to rational choice theory are

8 presented in the first section, social learning theory in the second section, classic strain theory in the third section, and social bonding theory in the fourth section.

The seventh and final chapter provides a discussion of the findings and is divided into three sections. First, the results of the analyses performed are summarized. In addition, the findings are also contextualized within the extant literature and the biosocial integration model proposed in Chapter 4. The second section provides an overview of the limitations of the current dissertation and how such limitations may impact the results reported. Finally, the third section discusses the implications of the findings of the current dissertation for future research and theoretical integration.

9

CHAPTER 2

THE BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE

To understand human individuality, social science must be bio-social in its outlook…Scientific advance depends on a relentless effort to reduce complex phenomena to their less complex elements and to build back and see how the elements contribute to the whole.

-David C. Rowe (2001, pp. 73-74)

The role of environmental and social influences in the etiology of criminal and delinquent behavior has been extensively examined in the extant criminological research. Nearly all major criminological theories implicate either a specific social/environmental factor or a combination of such factors in the etiology of criminal and delinquent behaviors. Influential studies have examined the manner in which a wide range of social and environmental factors, including neighborhoods (Anderson, 1999; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997), peer networks (Warr,

2002), and families (Hirschi, 1969) ultimately influence antisocial and criminal behavior. The influence of such social institutions on the development of criminal or delinquent behavior has been extensively examined using a wide range of methodologies and statistical techniques.

Studies have detected significant associations between these purely social influences and antisocial behaviors within a large number of samples, over extended periods of time, and for a wide assortment of antisocial behaviors.

While this culmination of findings is quite impressive, such findings are far from unequivocal and should still be viewed with caution. Nearly all criminological research examining the association between social factors and antisocial behaviors has relied on the use of standard social science methodologies (SSSMs; Beaver, 2013a; Harris, 2009; Walsh, 2002).

SSSMs refer to any methodology or analysis that disallows researchers from appropriately

10 accounting for genetic influences. Such methodologies involve the selection of one member per household for inclusion in the analytic sample. Information pertaining to the individual’s behavior and environment is then collected from each respondent and is then analyzed in statistical models to test various hypotheses. This procedure of collecting behavioral and environmental information from one individual per household implicitly assumes that each of the measures included in the model, including the outcome measure, is purely social and is not under any genetic influence. This underlying assumption of SSSMs supports the entire weight of these methodologies and reveals an important limitation of such modeling strategies: the presence of any genetic influence on any of the variables analyzed will violate this assumption and result in significant limitations including biased parameter estimates and standard errors.

While SSSMs have been used extensively in criminology and a number of other social science disciplines, such methodologies may suffer from a serious methodological limitation that is commonly overlooked. Since SSSMs are unable to estimate and partial out the influence of genetic factors on observed associations, it remains possible that uncontrolled genetic influences may confound any observed association between social/environmental factors and antisocial behaviors. More specifically, the potential presence of genetic influences on both socialization variables and antisocial behaviors effectively prevents SSSMs from adequately isolating the influence of a given social influence on antisocial behavior, resulting in a spurious association

(Johnson et al., 2009; McGue et al., 2010). In this way, studies that employ SSSMs inherently assume that genetic factors have no influence on the independent and dependent variables of interest. As long as this assumption is not violated, any observed association will not be confounded by uncontrolled genetic influences and the use of an SSSM is appropriate. However, mounting evidence indicates that this assumption is becoming increasingly untenable, regardless

11 of the combination of independent and dependent variables examined (Ferguson, 2010; Mason &

Frick, 1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Moffitt, 2005b; Rhee & Waldman, 2002). In this way, existing research employing SSSMs has likely produced findings that are biased due to the inability to adequately partial out the confounding influence of genetic factors. To state the problem as clearly as possible: there is a substantial amount of evidence which indicates that many of the previously identified associations between concepts central to conventional criminological theories and antisocial behaviors are spurious.

Despite a significant amount of theoretical evidence suggesting that genetic confounding is a widespread methodological limitation plaguing the vast majority of the extant criminological literature, it is currently unclear how and to what extent such limitations may bias criminological research. The primary goal of this dissertation is to clarify this significant gap in the existing literature by examining whether several of the most influential and prominent theories of criminal behavior can be empirically integrated into the biosocial perspective. In other words, do conventional theoretical explanations of criminal behavior hold after accounting for genetic confounding? In order to address this question, along with the previously specified Research

Questions, it is necessary to employ methodologies which properly account for genetic influences and effectively isolate the influence of theoretical concepts on the outcome of interest.

Collectively, such methodologies are referred to as behavior genetic (BG) research methodologies and are becoming increasingly popular in a number of social science disciplines including criminology (Barnes & Boutwell, 2013; Jaffee, Price, & Reyes, 2013).

Prior to providing an overview of the various methodological techniques which adequately control for genetic confounding and result in fully-specified models, it is first necessary to clarify one particular issue: biosocial criminology represents an overarching

12 paradigm or perspective, not a single theory of human behavior. This point is stated explicitly since even the most recent editions of some of the most widely read and required textbooks on criminological theory made the mistake of narrowly classifying biosocial research and findings as theories as opposed to an overarching perspective which can encompass many theories. For example, in the sixth edition of their widely read textbook Criminological Theories:

Introduction, Evaluation, and Application, Akers and Sellers (2013) repeatedly refer to central issues within the biosocial perspective—such as genetic influences or neuropsychological functioning—as aspects of a theory. However, this particular view is far too narrow, as these particular findings and concepts stem not only from their own theories, but also from their own disciplines in some instances. Behavior is a subfield of psychology with several universities offering degrees (including doctoral degrees) in the field of behavior genetics.

Neuroscience and molecular genetics are similar. These are not theoretical concepts that fit cleanly within the overarching theory of biosocial criminology. Rather, these are perspectives in their own right—which include theories, concepts, and other organizing ideas—which fall under the much larger perspective of biosocial criminology. In other words, the biosocial perspective borrows concepts, theories, and methodologies from other perspectives in an effort to better understand individual variation in human behavior.

This distinction is an important one since paradigms—at least in the Kuhnian sense— often organize multiple theories and it is often times impossible to present a single theory which adequately represents an entire perspective or paradigm (Kuhn, 1996). While some biosocial criminologists have attempted to develop a biosocial theory of criminal behavior (Ellis, 2005;

Robinson & Beaver, 2009), such efforts have been largely unsuccessful with the resulting theories being overly complex in an effort to take so many sets of influences into account (both

13 biological and environmental). In addition, the resulting theories consist of multiple intricate facets which, even individually, would be quite difficult or even impossible to test empirically.

While such efforts should be lauded, they are likely far too ambitious. Antisocial behavior is a highly complex phenotype that is likely the result of a complex combination of environmental and biological influences. Attempting to capture the gradations of these influences (and the combinations between them) in a single theory is a task that is likely impossible. Rather, there are likely multiple theoretical perspectives that fit nicely within the biosocial perspective and collectively specify the underlying etiology of antisocial and criminal behavior.

In order for theoretical explanations of criminal behavior to effectively fit within the biosocial perspective, the concepts contained within such theories must have a causal influence on antisocial phenotypes after controlling for genetic influences. Theories which emphasize concepts that do not significantly explain variance in antisocial outcomes after taking genetic influences into account do not fit within the biosocial perspective. The primary goal of this dissertation is to empirically test whether concepts specified within conventional criminological theories fit within the biosocial perspective. In order to perform this particular task, it will be necessary to utilize BG modeling strategies to effectively remove genetic influences and isolate the potential association between each theoretical concept and the outcome of interest. In this way, it is necessary to provide an overview of BG modeling strategies and the logic underlying such methodologies. The remainder of this chapter will be devoted to providing an overview of

BG methodologies and the findings from studies employing such methodologies to better specify the extent to which genetic and environmental influences explain individual level differences in antisocial phenotypes.

14

2.1 Behavior Genetic Methodologies

In an attempt to further integrate the biosocial perspective within the field of criminology,

Walsh (2000) suggested “it is time for mainstream criminology to at least pull back its blinders and peek at what behavior genetics has to offer” (p. 1077). Unfortunately, based on the limited number of criminologists who actively and consistently employ genetically sensitive research designs, few have followed Walsh’s suggestion (Wright et al., 2008; Wright & Cullen, 2012). In fact, since Walsh’s suggestion was published in 20001, only 1.4% (five total) 2 of all articles published in Criminology, the flagship journal of the American Society of Criminology, employ research designs that effectively estimate and partial out the influence of genetic factors. A closer examination reveals that a total of eight criminologists have collectively contributed to the five articles published in Criminology in a period of time that spans over a decade. This preliminary investigation further solidifies the observation that the vast majority of criminologists are unfamiliar with BG methods and the assumptions and procedures that accompany such methodologies. For this reason, it is necessary to provide an overview of BG methods.

BG methods, at their most fundamental level, are specialized analytic techniques that allow for the estimation of both genetic and environmental influences on virtually any phenotype or measureable characteristic. More specifically, BG techniques partition the variance within a given phenotype (e.g., height, weight, delinquency, or self-control) into three latent components:

(1) (symbolized as h2), (2) shared environmental influences (symbolized as c2), and

1 A total of 357 articles contained within 51 issues of Criminology have been published in the timeframe mentioned. The five articles which employ behavior genetic methodologies comprise only 1.4% of this overall total. 2 Importantly, a closer inspection of one of the studies included in this count (Haynie & McHugh, 2003) reveals that while behavior genetic methods were employed (i.e., DeFries-Fulker analysis), such methods were not executed properly and the resulting findings are rendered uninterruptable due to the authors’ misunderstanding of the analytic strategy. Despite these problems, the authors did indeed attempt to utilize a genetically informed analysis and the study is retained in the count presented. 15

(3) nonshared environmental influences (symbolized as e2). The heritability component of a BG analytic model provides an estimate of the proportion of the variance in the examined phenotype that can be explained by additive genetic influences. Additive genetic influences refer to the sum of the average independent effects of all genes that influence the examined phenotype (Neale,

2009). This type of genetic influence is juxtaposed against dominance genetic contributions and epistasis. Dominance effects take into account the influence of interactions between alternative copies of a single gene (which are referred to alleles) on the examined phenotype (Beaver,

2013a). Epistasis takes into account the influence of interactions between alleles of two or more different genes on the examined phenotype (Neale, 2009). Additive genetic influences operate under the assumption that each of the genes that collectively contribute to the examined phenotype operate independently of one another. Alternatively, dominance and epistasis assume nonadditive effects on the examined phenotype in that multiple alleles and genes are working interactively with one another. Dominance and epistasis are hypothesized to have relatively small effects on most human phenotypes and are therefore typically omitted from most BG models. Rather, the proportion (or percentage) of overall variance in the examined phenotype that can be explained by additive genetic influence is typically estimated and referred to as heritability.

BG modeling strategies also estimate the proportion of overall variance in a given phenotype that can be explained by environmental influences. However, as alluded to above,

BG modeling strategies distinguish between two separate types of environmental influences.

First, shared environmental influences are comprised of environmental experiences that are the same between siblings from the same household and work to make siblings more closely resemble one another across the examined phenotype. The underlying logic assumes that if a

16 particular environment explains a significant portion of the variance in the examined phenotype and this environment is experienced by both (or all) siblings, then the environment should have a similar impact on both siblings making them more closely resemble one another. For example, growing up in poverty would impact both siblings from a given household and would thereby be considered a shared environmental influence. Other examples include experiencing a divorce, family abuse or neglect, and neighborhood-level factors that are experienced by both siblings. In direct contrast to shared environmental influences are nonshared environmental influences which are comprised of environmental experiences that are differentially experienced between two siblings from the same household. In this way, any environment that is different between siblings and differentiates one sibling from the other is considered a nonshared environment.

Common examples of the nonshared environment include differential peer groups, different school environments, and different parenting strategies (such as parents treating their children different from one another). Importantly, nonshared environments can also arise when siblings subjectively interpret an objective event (Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). For example, siblings may interpret and react differently to an event that they both experience such as their parents divorcing. In this way, it is often difficult to positively differentiate shared environments from nonshared environments, with some behavior geneticists even going as far as to question whether shared environments even exist in the purest sense (Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000).

BG modeling strategies are designed to partition 100 percent of the variance in a given phenotype into these three categories (h2, c2, and e2) as latent factors. In other words, each of the components estimated indicate the collective influence of all genetic and environmental influences that culminate into the phenotype of interest. This characteristic of BG modeling strategies is highly attractive in that such models account for all environmental and genetic

17 influences that significantly influence variation in the phenotype examined are included in the model, providing a distinct advantage over SSSMs. In this way, BG modeling strategies effectively partial out genetic influences on the examined phenotype and isolate the association between independent and dependent variables of interest, resulting in a much more conservative quasi-experimental design (Johnson et al., 2009). SSSMs, on the other hand, are incapable of removing genetic influences and make it impossible to effectively isolate any potential association. The fundamental difference between BG modeling strategies and SSSMs which provides the former with distinct advantages over the latter, is that BG modeling strategies analyze samples which include more than one child per household, while SSSMs rely on sample with only one child per household. This inclusion of more than one child per household in the analytic sample allows for the decomposition of the overall variance in a given phenotype into the components described above. Since SSSMs do not analyze samples with more than one child per household they cannot differentiate between genetic and environmental influences making it impossible to adequately account for genetic confounding and preventing any causal associations from being detected.

Below, a more detailed description of the procedures that are used to effectively partition phenotypic variance into each of the aforementioned categories will be provided. As mentioned above, the majority of these procedures require analytic samples that include more than one sibling per household and cannot be performed on samples which only include one sibling per household. The reasoning and logic underlying this particular data requirement will also be discussed. Finally, an overview of the most common BG modeling strategies, along with the advantages and limitations of each, will be provided. This discussion will commence with the most common modeling strategy—the .

18

2.1.1 Twin Studies

BG modeling strategies require analytic samples that possess two specific characteristics.

First, as discussed previously, the analytic sample must include more than one child from the same household. Second, the level of genetic relatedness between children from the same household must be accurately recorded. In other words, most BG modeling strategies require that the amount of genetic material that the household members share with one another is known and recorded. Importantly, samples of twin pairs satisfy both of these requirements. By definition, samples that consist of twin pairs include multiple children from the same household.

In addition, there are two differing types of twins in the general population: monozygotic (MZ; commonly referred to as identical twins) and dizygotic (DZ; commonly referred to as fraternal twins) twins. MZ twins share 100 percent of their genetic material and occur in approximately 1 in every 250 births. The MZ twinning process is quite similar to non-twin pregnancies in which a single sperm fertilizes a single egg, but then, for reasons that remain unknown currently, the fertilized egg splits to form two identical embryos which eventually develop into two identical fetuses. In contrast, DZ twins only share 50 percent of their genetic material, which is the same amount of genetic material that is shared by non-twin full siblings. DZ twins occur in approximately 1 in every 125 births and are the result of two separate sperm fertilizing two separate eggs. Since the fertilization occurs at roughly the same time, both fertilized eggs eventually develop into fetuses that are carried during the same pregnancy. Since twins are typically reared in the same household by the same parents and the procedures involved with assessing the level of genetic relatedness between twins is fairly simple and straightforward, samples of twin pairs have become a staple in BG research.

Twin studies analyze samples that consist of MZ and DZ twins to estimate the heritability

(h2), shared environmental influences (c2), and nonshared environmental influences (e2) on a 19 phenotype of interest by examining how similar MZ twins are to one another relative to DZ twins across the phenotype of interest. Since MZ twins share twice as much genetic material as

DZ twins but both types of twins’ rearing environments are identical, the only factor that would result in MZ twins more closely resembling one another than DZ twins are genetic influences.

For example, if a research team were interested in determining whether genetic factors have a significant effect on body mass index (BMI), they could simply collect BMI information from a sample that consisted of both MZ and DZ twin pairs. There would be preliminary evidence that genetic factors have a significant influence on BMI if each MZ twins’ BMI scores more closely resemble their co-twins’ BMI score relative to the similarity between each DZ twin and their co- twin.

While the underlying logic of twin studies is fairly straightforward, there are additional details regarding the procedures used to compare twins from the same pair with one another.

The most common procedure involves the estimation of an intraclass correlation coefficient. An intraclass correlation coefficient expresses the direction and the magnitude of the association between one twin’s score on the measure of interest (e.g., BMI) with their co-twin’s score on the same measure. While traditional correlation coefficients focus on the association between two different variables, intraclass correlation coefficients estimate the association between the same measure but for two different members of the same household (e.g., a twin and their co-twin).

MZ intraclass correlation coefficients can then be compared DZ intraclass correlation coefficients to garner estimates of h2, c2, and e2 on the examined phenotype. Keep in mind that the majority of twins included in these samples are raised in the same household by the same parents, grow up in the same neighborhoods, and often attend the same schools as their co-twin.

In this way, environments between MZ and DZ twin pairs should be relatively similar and the

20 only reason that MZ twins would more closely resemble one another than DZ twins would be because MZ twins share twice as much genetic material with one another than DZ twins.

MZ and DZ intraclass correlations can be used to calculate direct estimates of the proportion of overall phenotypic variance explained by genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences (Neale & Maes, 1992). The proportion of variance that can be explained by genetic influences can be estimated as:

h2 = 2(rMZ – rDZ) (2.1) where rMZ refers to the intraclass correlation for MZ twins, and rDZ refers to the intraclass correlation for DZ twins. As is indicated in Equation 2.1, the intraclass correlation for DZ twins is subtracted from the intraclass correlation for MZ twins which effectively removes all of the variance in the phenotype that is due to shared environmental factors. In addition, the difference term included in Equation 2.1 also removes half of the variance in the examined phenotype that is due to genetic influences since MZ twins share twice as much genetic material as DZ twins.

In order to correct for this underestimation, the resulting difference term is doubled. The resulting coefficient is a direct estimate of the proportion of overall phenotypic variance that can be explained by genetic influences (Neale & Maes, 1992; Plomin, 1990; Walsh, 2002).

The same intraclass correlations for MZ and DZ twins can be used to estimate the proportion of variance that is explained by shared environmental influences. The coefficient for c2 can be estimated as:

c2 = 2(rDZ) – rMZ (2.2) where rDZ still refers to the intraclass correlation for DZ twins and rMZ still refers to the intraclass correlation for MZ twins. However, there are two noticeable differences regarding

Equation 2.2 relative to Equation 2.1. First, only the rDZ term is doubled. Doubling rDZ fixes

21 the proportion of variance explained by genetic influences to be equal for MZ and DZ twins.

Second, the rMZ term is subtracted from the rDZ term (after it is doubled). The resulting coefficient provides a direct estimate of the proportion of overall phenotypic variance that can be explained by shared environmental influences.

Finally, a direct estimate of the proportion of phenotypic variance that is explained by nonshared environmental influences can be calculated as:

e2 = 1 – (h2 + c2) (2.3) where h2 refers to the heritability estimate obtained from Equation 2.1 and c2 refers to the estimate of shared environmental influence obtained from Equation 2.2. Both terms are summed and then subtracted from 1 to estimate the residual variance in the examined phenotype that is not explained by genetic or shared environmental influences. Importantly, since e2 is estimated as the residual variance that is not explained by h2 and c2, measurement error is also included in the e2 estimate.

Two additional points regarding the equations presented above and their resulting coefficients warrant additional attention. First, the sum of h2, c2, and e2 will always be equal to

1.00, indicating that all three components collectively explain 100 percent of the overall variance in the phenotype examined. Second, the equations presented above are somewhat antiquated and have been updated to increase accuracy and to be used with samples that include a wider range of kinship pairs other than twins. While the inclusion of additional types of kinship pairs increases the versatility of BG modeling strategies, the modified equations become far more computationally advanced. Despite these recent developments, the underlying logic of twin- based studies remains the same: if MZ twins more closely resemble one another than DZ twins

22 across a variable of interest, genetic influences explain a significant portion of the variance in the examined variable.

While the logic underlying twin studies is fairly straightforward, there are three assumptions that must be met for the estimates obtained from twin samples to be considered unbiased. The first assumption is referred to as the equal-environment assumption (EEA) and refers to the assumption that twins from the same MZ pair experience environments that are no more similar than twins from the same DZ pair. In other words, if MZ twins experience environments that are significantly more similar than environments that are experienced by DZ twins, estimates from BG models may be biased. Critics of twin studies have argued that a violation of the EEA would result in upwardly biased heritability coefficients. More specifically, critics have argued that MZ twins more closely resemble one another relative to DZ twins since they experience environments that are more similar. For example, it is not uncommon for parents to dress MZ twins similarly or for MZ twins to be mistaken for one another (Loehlin & Nichols, 1976). While these observations may seem to provide preliminary evidence which suggests that the EEA is commonly violated, it is important to point out that the only violations of the EEA that would potentially result in biased heritability estimates are those that increase the similarity of MZ twins on the measures of interest. For example, the fact that

MZ twins have a more similar physical appearance than DZ twins may violate the EEA, but such a violation would not upwardly bias the influence of genetic factors on a measure of delinquency. This is primarily due to the fact that there is no empirical evidence suggesting that the physical appearance of a given individual is related to their propensity to engage in delinquency, to argue otherwise would be to argue in favor of Lombroso’s (2006 [1876]) theory

23 of the criminal man. Regardless, violations of the EEA remain a significant point of contention between critics and advocates of twin-based research designs.

Fortunately, there has been a significant amount of research examining how often the

EEA is violated and whether violations of the EEA result in biased heritability estimates (Cronk et al., 2002; Eaves et al., 2003). Providing an empirical examination of the potential repercussions of violating the EEA is not a simple task to carry out and researchers have had to rely on innovative approaches. A number of studies have examined samples in which a subsample of MZ and DZ twin pairs has been incorrectly classified wherein MZ twins were mistakenly classified as DZ twins and vice versa (Conley et al., 2013; Gunderson et al., 2006;

Kendler, 1983; Kendler et al., 1993). This type of misclassification typically occurs when researchers are forced to rely on physical similarities to assess zygosity as opposed to genotyping. Such cases provide the opportunity to examine the potential effect of violating the

EEA on heritability coefficients. More specifically, if the EEA is violated, MZ twins who were mistakenly classified as DZ twins should be less similar to one another than MZ twins who were correctly classified. Along the same lines, DZ twins who were misclassified as MZ twins should be more similar to one another than DZ twins who were correctly classified.

Collectively, the results of these studies found no evidence in favor of upwardly biased heritability coefficients. In addition, the results of a recent study (Conley et al., 2013) which analyzed misclassified twins from three separate samples found that violation of the EEA actually resulted in a significant reduction in heritability coefficients indicating that such estimates are actually downwardly biased. Importantly, Conley and colleagues’ findings extended to a wide range of phenotypes including height, weight, BMI, ADHD, depression, and even delinquency. Taken as a whole, the results of these studies seem to indicate that violation

24 of the EEA does not make MZ twins more similar to one another across a wide range of behavioral phenotypes, many of which are considered important causes or correlates of antisocial behavior.

The second assumption underlying twin studies is an absence of assortative mating. The concept of assortative mating refers to the phenomenon of human mate selection practices that result in mating couples that closely resemble one another across a wide range of physical, psychological, and behavioral characteristics (Boutwell et al., 2012; Krueger et al., 1998; Lykken

& Tellegen, 1993; Shackelford et al., 2005). For example, studies have revealed that mates tend to possess similar demographic characteristics (Mare, 1991; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007), similar levels of educational attainment (Lefgren & McIntyre, 2006) and intelligence (Mascie-

Taylor & Vandenberg, 1988), and even political affiliation (Kandler, Bleidorn, & Riemann,

2012). In addition, a corresponding line of literature has revealed that mates tend to display similar levels of antisocial and delinquent behaviors, wherein delinquent individuals are significantly more likely to select a delinquent mate than a nondelinquent individual (Beaver,

2013b; Boutwell et al., 2012; Capaldi et al., 2008; Krueger et al., 1998; Rhule-Louie &

McMahon, 2007). For example, in a recent study, Boutwell and colleagues (2012) analyzed a nationally representative sample of mates from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth

Cohort (ECLS-B) and found that the propensity for criminality (measured as antisocial behavior and substance use) between mates was strongly correlated, with a correlation coefficient of .54.

Taken together, the results of this line of research provide overwhelming evidence that individuals tend to select mates that are similar to themselves.

The presence of assortative mating may potentially bias results from BG models because this process results in a systematic increase in the genetic similarity of all siblings that possess

25 any dissenting genetic material (i.e., all siblings other than MZ twins). Since mates tend to more closely resemble one another across phenotypes that have been found to be influenced by genetic factors than individuals selected at random, there is also a greater probability that such mates will also have genotypes that are more similar than two individuals chosen at random. For example, delinquency has been found to be significantly influenced by genetic factors (Mason & Frick,

1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Rhee & Waldman, 2002; Ferguson, 2010) and mates tend to engage in similar rates of offending (Beaver, 2013b). In this way, there is a high probability that mates will share many of the polymorphisms that are related to delinquency. This, in turn, may result in an increase in the amount of genetic material that their non-MZ twin offspring possess. Based on the research outlined above, there is good reason to believe that the assumption of no assortative mating is consistently violated in studies employing BG methods.

While the violation of this assumption does result in biased parameter estimates, studies have revealed that the presence of assortative mating actually results in significantly underestimated heritability estimates (Keller et al., 2013; Vinkhuyzen, van der Sluis, Maes, &

Posthuma, 2012). For example, Beauchamp and colleagues (2011) found that specific BG models are highly sensitive to violations of the assortative mating assumption and models which adequately account for assortative mating resulted in a dramatic increase in the estimated heritability estimate (from .31 to .91) and a significant decrease in the shared environmental estimate (from .59 to .00). Taken together, it seems that the assortative mating assumption is consistently violated in studies employing BG modeling strategies. However, violation of this assumption actually results in more conservative heritability estimates.

The third and final assumption underlying twin studies is that the phenotype of interest must be measured accurately and without error. Since the nonshared environment coefficient

26 estimates the proportion of the phenotypic variance that is explained by the nonshared environment and measurement error, it is possible that measures with higher amounts of error may yield less reliable estimates. In other words, measures that are less reliably measured may result in inflated e2 estimates, which, in turn, may artificially deflate estimates of h2 and c2. Once again, a violation of this assumption would result in an artificially deflated heritability coefficient.

It is worth noting that these three assumptions are commonly violated in studies employing BG research. In many instances there is simply no way to avoid a violation of at least one of the assumptions. Critics of BG research may point to this fact in an effort to discredit the findings garnered from such studies and claim that violations of these assumptions result in biased heritability estimates which calls into question the finding that genetic factors significantly influence examined phenotypes. Such critics are simply misinformed and do not fully understand the repercussions that accompany the violation of each of these assumptions.

While studies have revealed that a violation of any of the assumptions does result in biased heritability coefficients, the vast majority of the studies that have examined the extent to which heritability estimates are biased have indicated that these estimates are downwardly biased, indicating that heritability estimates are more conservative than they should be (Beauchamp et al., 2011; Conley et al., 2013). In other words, BG models are designed to err on the side of caution and consistently produce highly conservative heritability estimates and inflated estimates of environmental influence. In this way, it is far more likely that genetic factors explain a significantly greater proportion of the variance in phenotypes related to antisocial behavior than previous studies have reported.

27

In addition to violations of the underlying assumptions of BG models, critics have also pointed out that BG modeling strategies rely on samples of twins and that such samples may not be representative of samples of singletons, making it impossible to extrapolate findings from genetically sensitive research designs to any larger population. A recent study by Barnes and

Boutwell (2013) explored this critique by comparing the effects of a wide range of covariates on antisocial behavior (which included delinquency, drug use, and victimization) between twins and non-twins from a nationally representative sample. Some of the covariates included in the analysis included: low self-control, depression, delinquent peers, popularity, physical attractiveness, GPA, and a host of parenting measures. The results revealed that twins did not significantly differ from non-twins across levels of antisocial behavior and each of the examined covariates. In addition, the association between each of the covariates and the antisocial behaviors examined did not significantly differ between twins and non-twins. The results of this study provide strong evidence indicating that twin samples are representative of non-twin samples, effectively dampening any argument to the contrary.

Despite the overwhelming amount of evidence suggesting that the above concerns regarding BG modeling strategies are not warranted, additional research strategies have been devised to directly address such limitations. While all of these strategies are distinct from one another, they do share a common difference from traditional twin studies in that they do not compare common phenotypes across MZ and DZ twin pairs. Rather, these techniques rely on alternative means to partial out genetic confounding and isolate the association of interest. Since these alternative modeling strategies do not rely on comparisons within twin-based samples, they are not subject to the assumptions outlined above. In this way, a violation of such assumptions

28 should not result in biased (either upwardly or downwardly) parameter estimates. Below, each of these alternative modeling strategies will be outlined.

2.1.2 Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart (MZA)

In an effort to eliminate any bias resulting from a violation of the EEA, researchers have obtained samples of MZ twins who were separated at birth and reared in different homes by different parents. These twins, symbolized as MZA, are unique in that they are genetically identical but have been raised in completely different environments. In this way, MZAs provide researchers with the unique opportunity to effectively separate genetic and environmental influences on any phenotype. Since MZAs share 100 percent of their genes, but experience very different environments, any similarities between them have to be solely the result of genetic influences and, conversely, any differences between them are solely the result of nonshared environmental influences. Since the twins were reared in unique environments, shared environmental influences cannot play a role. Based on this logic, we would expect that phenotypes that are significantly impacted by the environment would vary significantly between

MZAs, while phenotypes that are under significant genetic influence would be highly similar between MZAs.

One of the primary limitations of this modeling strategy is that MZAs, as one would expect, are exceedingly rare, making it difficult to generate samples with enough statistical power to generate meaningful results. However, a team of researchers at the University of

Minnesota have established the Minnesota Study of Identical Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) in an effort to generate a functional sample of MZAs. To date, MISTRA has identified and interviewed over 100 MZA pairs. Members of the MISTRA research team have been able to analyze the data collected from the MZA pairs they have identified in an effort to examine

29 genetic and environmental influences on a wide range of phenotypes including many that are direct correlates of criminal and antisocial behavior. The results of these studies have revealed that genetic factors significantly influence nearly every measureable human phenotype, with some phenotypes being highly heritable (for a comprehensive overview see Segal, 2012). For example, Tellegen and colleagues (1988) examined a wide range of psychological and behavioral phenotypes related to criminal behavior including aggression, self-control, negative emotionality, and constraint and found that all of the examined phenotypes had heritability estimates hovering around .50. Importantly, these estimates directly converge with studies which employ the more traditional twin-based BG design. Along the same lines, additional studies have revealed that MZ twins who are reared in different homes are just as similar to one another as MZ twins who are reared in the same home (Bouchard et al., 1990; Bouchard &

McGue, 1990). This finding provides further evidence suggesting that genetic influences play a pivotal role in the development of virtually all phenotypes. Importantly, these findings are free from any bias that may result from a violation of the EEA since these studies focused on samples of MZ twins and all of the twins included in the analytic models were reared in separate homes.

While the MZA design addresses some of the most notable limitations of the traditional twin-based design, the MZA design is not without its own limitations. The most notable limitation of the MZA design is the possibility that the adoptive environments that MZ twins experience are correlated. In other words, if two MZ twins are separated at birth but are placed in two separate homes that are situated in very similar neighborhoods, attend similar schools, and have very similar parents these similar environmental experiences may work to make both twins more similar to one another and result in artificially inflated heritability estimates. To further condense this argument, critiques of the MZA approach argue that unmeasured shared

30 environmental influences may be working to make twins more closely resemble one another.

There is a fundamental flaw with this particular argument: the shared environment typically accounts for a minimal amount of the overall variance in antisocial phenotypes (Mason & Frick,

1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Rhee & Waldman, 2002; Ferguson, 2010). The evidence regarding minimal shared environmental influence is so overwhelming that leading behavior geneticists have openly questioned whether shared environmental influences even exist (see Turkheimer &

Waldron, 2000 for an overview of this argument). Regardless of these findings, another research design—the adoption study—has been developed to take this particular limitation of the MZA design into account.

2.1.3 Adoption Studies

Just as twin pairs provide a unique situation allowing for the estimation of genetic and environmental influences on a given phenotype, adopted children also present a unique situation, in which, it is possible to tease genetic and environmental influences from one another. In addition, adoption studies are not reliant upon the EEA, alleviating concerns regarding differential treatment of siblings from the same household. The most powerful aspect of the adoption design is that since adoptees do not share any genetic material with their adoptive parents, this particular design isolates the effects of environmental influences on the phenotype of interest. More specifically, adoptees, just like any other child, share 50 percent of their genetic material with their biological father and the remaining 50 percent of their genetic material with their biological mother. However, children that are adopted shortly after birth do not share the environment with their biological parents. Any similarities that are detected between biological parents and their adopted children must be directly influenced by genetic factors. While adoptive children do not share any genetic material with their adoptive parents,

31 they do share the environment, making any similarities between adoptive parents and adoptees the result of environmental influences. Based on these unique conditions, the adoption-based design is a powerful methodology that allows for the estimation of environmental effects on the phenotype of interest net of the potentially confounding effects of genetic influences (Beaver et al., 2011; Natsuaki et al., 2013; Rutter, 2006; Rutter et al., 2001).

This somewhat elementary logic has been developed into what has been referred to as “a conservative and effective approach to examining the interplay of nature and nurture” (Natsuaki et al., 2013, p. 1753) and has been used extensively by some of the leading developmental researchers (Deater-Deckard & Plomin, 1999; Moffitt, 2005a; Rutter, 2006; Rutter et al., 2001).

The basic logic of this methodology rests on the statistical comparison between adoptees’ phenotypes to the same (or closely related) phenotypes in both their biological and adoptive parents. If the adoptee more closely resembles their biological parents then genetic influences have a greater impact on the examined trait. Conversely, if the adoptee more closely resembles their adoptive parents then environmental influences have a greater impact on the examined trait.

While adoption studies are fairly uncommon in criminology, there are a few notable examples.

For example, in one of the most well-known adoption studies, Mednick and colleagues (1984) compared the prevalence of arrest between adoptees and their biological and adoptive parents.

The results indicated that adoptees who experience both genetic and environmental risk factors

(i.e., biological and adoptive parents had been arrested) had the greatest odds of being arrested.

Adoptees that experienced only genetic risk (i.e., only their biological parents had been arrested) had the second greatest chance of being arrested. This same pattern of results has been echoed in several subsequent studies also (see for example Beaver, 2011). A comprehensive review of adoption studies examining antisocial phenotypes revealed a consistent and sizable genetic effect

32 on a wide range of antisocial behaviors (Raine, 1993). Collectively, adoption studies examining antisocial phenotypes have yielded heritability estimates that are slightly lower, but not significantly different, from estimates obtained from twin studies (Beaver, 2013a).

2.1.4 Family-Based Studies

Despite the conservative nature of the adoption-based design, some critics still argue that this particular methodology suffers from damaging limitations. Most notably, critics have previously argued that adoptees (and twins) represent “special” populations and that adoptees differ from non-adoptees limiting the generalizability of the results from such studies to any larger population. These same criticisms have been leveled against twin-based studies, and subsequently resolved (see Barnes & Boutwell, 2013). Despite empirical findings which suggest that finings garnered from twin-based and adoptee-based samples can be extrapolated to larger populations, additional BG methodologies have been developed to alleviate any lingering concerns. More specifically, family-based studies extend the logic and equations used in twin- based studies to additional non-twin kin pairs. While twin studies are limited to MZ and DZ twin pairs, family-based studies may be used to include a much larger range of kinship pairs including MZ twins, DZ twins, full-siblings, half-siblings, cousins, and even nonrelated individuals reared in the same household (e.g., stepsiblings). Due to this inclusion of a much wider range of kinship pairs, the results garnered from family-based studies are more generalizable than twin-based or adoption-based studies.

In addition to greater generalizability, another attractive feature of family-based studies is the ease in which traditional twin-based BG equations can be modified to include non-twin kinship pairs. The previously presented equations (Equations 2.1 through 2.3) can be easily modified to include varying levels of genetic relatedness. Table 3.1 displays the differing levels

33 of genetic relatedness across various kinship pairs ranging from MZ twins (r = 1.00) to unrelated or stepsiblings (r = 0.00). Despite the inclusion of a wider range of kinship groups included in the equations, the underlying logic of family-based studies is very similar to the underlying logic of twin-based studies. Namely, if kinship pairs that have a greater proportion of genetic material in common (e.g., full siblings) more closely resemble one another across the phenotype of interest than kinship pairs with less genetic material in common (e.g., half siblings), the examined phenotype is influenced by genetic factors.

Table 2.1: Levels of Genetic Relatedness for Family Studies Kinship Pair Level of Genetic Relatedness (r) Monozygotic (MZ) Twins 1.00 Dizygotic (DZ) Twins 0.50 Full Siblings 0.50 Half Siblings 0.25 Cousins 0.125 Unrelated (e.g., step) Siblings 0.00

While the family-based design presents distinct advantages over alternative BG methods

(namely greater external validity), it also presents distinct limitations that must be addressed to obtain valid and reliable parameter estimates. For example, since family-based studies include non-twin kinship pairs it is all but certain that distinct differences in age and sex may exist between individuals included in a given pair. This limitation can be easily corrected statistically by regressing the phenotype of interest on each of the potential demographic confounders and preserving the residuals prior to calculating heritability, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental estimates. This procedure would effectively remove any of the variance in the

34 examined phenotype that would be attributed to differences in age or sex, leaving the remaining variance to be decomposed into the common h2, c2, and e2 components. Research has consistently revealed that heritability estimates garnered from family-based studies which effectively account for demographic differences with pairs are highly similar to heritability estimates obtained from traditional twin-based studies (Plomin et al., 2013).

2.1.5 Nonshared Environment Studies

The BG methodologies that have been discussed thus far are primarily focused on providing an unbiased and reliable estimate of genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences. Importantly, all three of these estimates are latent in the sense that they take into account all genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences that contribute to the examined phenotype. While these estimates are useful and directly relevant to a wide range of both theoretical and empirical issues within criminology and other disciplines, such estimates do not identify the specific genes and environments that contribute to the phenotype being examined. A logical “next step” would be to extend traditional

BG models in both a theoretical and analytical sense to identify the specific genetic and environmental influences that contribute to particular phenotypes. In an effort to provide a more detailed description of the specific genes and environments that comprise the latent estimates of h2, c2, and e2 two additional research designs have been developed. Molecular genetic studies are aimed at identifying the specific genes that comprise the h2 estimate, while nonshared environment studies have been developed to identify the specific environmental influences which compromise e2.3 In order to isolate specific aspects of the nonshared environment, it is

3 A developing line of literature has focused on the identification of specific environments that comprise c2 (Baker, Maes, & Kendler, 2012; Walden, McGue, Iacono, Burt, & Elkins, 2004). However, the studies comprising this line of research are limited in number and have only examined a limited number of phenotypes. This limited attention in the extant literature is likely a direct result of the limited influence of the shared environment on most phenotypes. 35 first necessary to calculate the latent genetic and shared environmental influences on the phenotype of interest. This particular aspect of nonshared environment studies provides a distinct and considerable advantage over traditional SSSMs in that any statistically significant coefficient can be interpreted as an effect of the specific nonshared environmental influence on the examined phenotype net of the effect of genetic and shared environmental influences

(Beaver, 2013a; Plomin & Daniels, 1987; Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). In this way, coefficients estimated in nonshared environment studies are immune to some of the most concerning methodological shortcomings of SSSMs such as genetic confounding. In addition, since shared environmental influences are modeled as latent factors, the probability of omitting some other potentially confounding environmental influence from the model is also greatly diminished. Taken together, nonshared environment studies make use of some of the most powerful and conservative modeling strategies currently available in the social sciences.

Perhaps the most common modeling strategy used in nonshared environment studies is the MZ difference score approach (Asbury, Dunn, Pike, Plomin, 2003; Burt, McGue, Iacono, &

Krueger, 2006; Pike, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1996). While this particular approach is fairly straightforward, it is widely considered the “gold standard” for identifying measured nonshared environmental influences (Beaver, 2008) and has been regarded as one of the most rigorous quasi-experimental designs available (Vitaro, Brendgen, & Arsenault, 2009). The MZ difference score approach is situated on the observation that MZ twins share 100 percent of their genetic material. Since MZ twins are genetically identical to one another, any observed difference between them cannot be the result of genetic influences and must be due to nonshared environmental influences. In addition, since MZ twins are reared in the same household by the

Based on the limited nature of the existing literature, and the limited explanatory power of shared environmental influences, a more detailed explanation of the methodologies used to identify specific shared environments will not be elaborated upon. 36 same parents, the MZ difference score approach also takes shared environmental influences into account. In this way, the MZ difference score approach effectively controls for genetic and shared environmental influences and isolates observed nonshared environmental influences on the examined phenotype. Since the MZ difference score approach does not compare MZ and DZ twins (as in traditional twin studies), a violation of the EEA is not a concern (Rovine, 1994).

The MZ difference score approach is carried out by subtracting one twin’s score on a given measure from their co-twin’s score on the same measure. This same procedure is also carried out for the outcome measure of interest. For example, in order to examine whether differences in maternal attachment predict differences in delinquency during adolescence, one twin’s score on the maternal attachment measure would be subtracted from their co-twin’s score on the same maternal attachment measure (this procedure would be repeated for the delinquency measure). After both difference scores have been calculated they can then be inserted into a statistical equation and making it possible to test whether the observed nonshared environmental influence (e.g., differences in maternal attachment) has a significant effect on differences in the examined outcome (e.g., adolescent delinquency).

2.2 Behavior Genetic Research and Antisocial Behavior

Despite the overall dearth of studies which employ genetically research designs within the leading criminology journals, other disciplines have produced an overwhelming number of studies which directly examine both genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior.

In addition, the majority of such studies were published several decades ago, indicating that information regarding the genetic and environmental contributions to antisocial behavior has been readily available for quite some time. In a comprehensive review of the potential benefits of integrating behavior genetic methods into developmental research that is now nearly a decade

37 old, Moffitt (2005a) notes that “[m]ore than 100 studies have addressed the question of genetic influence on antisocial behavior” (p. 535). In the years since Moffitt’s review was published, there are surely dozens of additional studies that have been published. Based on the vast number of studies that have examined the genetic and environmental contributions to antisocial behavior, this section will provide a general overview of this line of research as opposed to a comprehensive review. This decision was made for two distinct reasons. First, comprehensive reviews of this line of research have been completed relatively recently (see Moffitt, 2005b), making and additional review redundant. Second, four meta-analyses reviewing this literature are currently available (Mason & Frick, 1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Rhee & Waldman, 2002;

Ferguson, 2010), which again makes a more comprehensive review redundant. Based on the breadth and robustness of findings that accompany meta-analysis (Cooper, 2010), the majority of this section will be devoted to summarizing the content and findings of these four meta-analyses.

2.2.1 Meta-Analyses of Behavior Genetic Research on Antisocial Behavior

Meta-analysis is an analytic technique which allows for the empirical accumulation of a body of studies which may differ in various aspects, but are all aimed at addressing a similar topic of interest. More specifically, while studies that comprise a given body of research may employ different measurement strategies, analyze different samples, and employ differing analytic techniques, such studies are all aimed at gaining a better understanding of a given relationship or topic. In such situations, conflicting results may, and oftentimes do, arise making it difficult to determine whether any consensus among the findings of the studies can be reached.

Additionally, it becomes more and more difficult to determine the underlying source of conflicting findings in such instances. For example, studies with conflicting results may employ methodologies which differ in various ways and may employ dramatically different analytic

38 samples making in nearly impossible to determine whether any detected difference is a result of methodological differences between the studies or actual discrepancies in findings.

Meta-analyses are invaluable in situations wherein a voluminous literature has been produced and provide at least three important advantages. First, meta-analyses perform a systematic review of the literature and an empirical summary of the findings gleaned from such a review. In this way, it becomes possible to synthesize the findings from an overwhelming number of studies down into a single set of findings, referred to as the average effect size, which provide an overall representation of all of the individual studies in a given area of research.

Second, meta-analyses allow researchers to detect the signal through the noise in that this procedure allows for the identification of the precise methodological characteristics that may contribute to discrepancies in findings between studies. This particular characteristic of studies becomes highly valuable when attempting to draw a consensus from a body of research that is wrought with conflicting findings. In addition, meta-analyses allow for the weighting of studies with superior or rigorous methodologies which can increase the accuracy of the average effect size and provide meaningful suggestions for future research. Third, meta-analyses make it possible to detect small effect sizes and inconsistent effects. Since meta-analyses make use of studies as the primary unit of analysis (as opposed to data points within a sample), the average effect size is the result of all of the individual data points from each of the analyzed studies which results in a dramatic increase in statistical power. For example, the results of an individual study may reveal that the association between two variables is nonsignificant; however, when the results from this single study are combined with many others what appears to be an inconsistent association between the two variables may actually result in a relatively consistent association. Despite these distinct advantages of meta-analyses, such studies should

39 not be seen as a panacea. Meta-analyses are only as good as the studies that they summarize. In this way, a body of research which consistently employs careless methodologies and reports inaccurate findings will result in a meta-analysis which produces biased findings as well.

As mentioned previously, there are currently four meta-analyses which examine genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior (Mason & Frick, 1994; Miles & Carey,

1997; Ferguson, 2010; Rhee & Waldman, 2002). These meta-analyses synthesize findings from studies which employ various behavior genetic research methodologies to estimate the proportion of variance in antisocial behavior that be explained by genetic (h2), shared environmental (c2) and nonshared environmental (e2) influences. In this way, the average effect size of interest in each of these meta-analyses refers to each of the three components that are estimated in a behavior genetic study, h2, c2, and e2. In this way, each meta-analysis will provide an average effect size indicating the average influence of genetic influences, the shared environment, and the nonshared environment on antisocial behavior across all of the individual studies analyzed.

Prior to discussing each meta-analysis in more detail, there are two additional characteristics of these four studies that should be considered. First, the amount of time that passed between each of the studies indicates that each subsequent meta-analysis almost certainly contains novel findings that were not included in the studies that preceded it. This is an important characteristic of these studies in that the findings collectively represent a sizable segment of the literature that spans a significant period of time. In addition, due to the limited overlap in the studies included in each meta-analysis, an overall concordance in findings would seem to indicate that the overall pattern of results is highly robust. Second, the procedures used in each study were subsequently refined based on criticisms and limitations of each prior study,

40 indicating that each subsequent meta-analysis was more rigorous than the previous one.

Collectively, these characteristics likely increase the validity of the findings, particularly in the most recent studies.

Mason and Frick (1994). The first meta-analysis to synthesize results from studies examining the influences of genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior was performed by Mason and Frick (1994). As with any meta-analysis, Mason and Frick developed and specified explicit criteria regarding the characteristics of studies that would be included in their meta-analysis. Studies published between 1975 and 1991 which either compared MZ and

DZ twins in a classic twin-based study or analyzed a sample of adoptees (using the adoption- based methodology specified above) were considered for inclusion in the study. In regard to antisocial behavior, only studies which included various externalizing behavioral issues related to antisocial behavior (such as aggression, antisocial personality, or criminal behavior) were considered. Importantly, the authors decided to exclude studies which focused solely on substance abuse, but included studies that used substance abuse as a singular indicator of antisocial behavior more broadly. In addition, Mason and Frick (1994) only included studies in which all respondents were compared across the same measure of antisocial behavior.4 Finally, the authors excluded any studies with overlapping analytic samples and any studies in which the results could not be used to estimate an average effect size. Based on these inclusion criteria, the final sample consisted of 15 studies which included over 4,000 twin and sibling pairs.

The results of the meta-analysis revealed that the average heritability estimate across all

15 of the studies included in the analysis was .48, indicating that approximately 48 percent of the variance in antisocial behavior was explained by genetic influences. In a series of sensitivity

4 This is particularly important in regard to adoption studies, as the same measure of antisocial behavior would have to be collected for adoptees, biological parents, and adoptive parents. 41 tests, the authors also examined whether their results differed based on three different criteria.

First, the authors compared genetic influences on severe antisocial behavior in relation to nonsevere antisocial behavior. The results revealed that the heritability coefficient for severe antisocial behavior (h2 = .45) was greater than the heritability coefficient for nonsevere antisocial behavior (h2 = .00). Second, the authors examined whether demographic characteristics such as age, sex, or country of origin significantly moderated the average heritability effect size. The results indicated that these various characteristics did not moderate the results. Third, the authors examined whether the population from which the analytic sample was drawn significantly moderated the resulting heritability coefficient. The results indicated that studies which analyze samples drawn from clinical populations tend to report greater h2 estimates (h2 = .53) than studies that analyze samples drawn from the general population (h2 = .20).

Miles and Carey (1997). The second meta-analysis examining genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior was performed by Miles and Carey (1997). The authors limited their analysis to studies which included a measure of aggression, hostility, antisocial behavior, or . In concordance with Mason & Frick (1994), Miles and Carey

(1997) also included both twin and adoption studies in their meta-analysis. These inclusion criteria resulted in a total of 24 effect sizes gleaned from 17 studies (n = 15 twin studies and n =

2 adoption studies). The results of the meta-analysis produced an average heritability coefficient of .50, indicating that approximately 50 percent of the variance in antisocial behavior can be explained by genetic influences. As in the previous study, the authors also performed a sensitivity analysis in an effort to identify potential study characteristics which may moderate heritability estimates. The results revealed that the sex of respondents significantly moderated heritability coefficients, wherein samples comprised of males produced greater estimates of h2

42 relative to samples of females. Similarly, samples with older respondents resulted in greater heritability estimates relative to samples of younger respondents. The authors also examined whether different measurement strategies (i.e., observational measures vs. self-report measures) moderated heritability estimates. The results revealed that self-report measures of antisocial behavior produced greater estimates of h2 relative to observational measures of antisocial behavior. While estimates of h2 varied as a function of these three factors (age, sex, and measurement strategy), it is important to note that h2 estimates remained at least moderate in size across all models and ranged between .22 and .78.

Rhee and Waldman (2002). The third meta-analysis to examine genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior was conducted by Rhee and Waldman in 2002.

Based on the explosion of behavior genetic research focusing on antisocial behavior since the previous meta-analytic review, the authors had a larger sample of studies to choose from and were able to utilize more stringent selection criteria. The authors attempted to limit the included studies to those which included a direct measure of antisocial behavior and excluded studies which analyzed proxy measures of antisocial behavior like hostility. For example, included studies had to operationalize antisocial behavior as a clinical diagnosis, criminal behavior as measured by official records, or observations/self-reports of aggression. Studies which analyzed overlapping or nonindependent samples or reported incomplete findings were excluded from the final analytic sample. These selection criteria resulted in a total of 42 twin studies and 10 adoption studies which included over 55,000 pairs of respondents in total.

The results indicated that 41 percent of the variance in antisocial behavior was the result of genetic influences, shared environmental influences explained 16 percent of the variance, and the nonshared environment explained the remaining 43 percent of the variance. As in the

43 previous meta-analysis, Rhee and Waldman (2002) also performed a series of sensitivity analyses aimed at identifying factors which may moderate genetic influences. More specifically, the authors examined the potentially moderating role of age, sex, assessment method, zygosity determination, and concept operationalization. While heritability estimates did differ based on the technique used to determine zygosity, the difference between measured obtained from blood testing (h2 = .47) versus self-report (h2 = .43) were markedly similar. The results also revealed that self-report measures of antisocial behavior, relative to observational measures, result in smaller heritability estimates. In addition, older samples tend to result in smaller heritability estimates relative to younger samples.

Ferguson (2010). The most recent meta-analysis examining the genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior was conducted by Ferguson (2010). This particular meta-analysis was distinct from those previously presented in at least two ways. First,

Ferguson (2010) focused on more recent research and included studies that were published between 1996 and 2006. This particular feature of this meta-analysis is critical in that more recent studies are expected to be of higher empirical rigor in that they are more likely to employ more advanced statistical methods, draw from modern samples, and directly address limitation of studies previously published. Second, Ferguson (2010) directly attempts to bridge “the chasm between and behavioral genetics” by examining more closely the proximate (i.e., genetic influences) and ultimate causes (i.e., ) of antisocial behavior (p. 163). This is another important contribution to the literature since behavior genetics and evolutionary psychology5 are directly compatible and can collectively contribute to a more

5 Evolutionary psychology refers to a field of study concerned with the application of evolutionary theory to human behavior (Barkow, 2006; Mealey, 2000). More specifically, this particular paradigm focuses on the specific processes and , operating through natural and sexual selection, which directly contribute to , and in turn, phenotypic variation (for a more detailed overview see Wright, 1994). Importantly, 44 comprehensive understanding of the underlying etiology of nearly any human phenotype, including antisocial behavior. Despite these common interests and overlapping goals, both disciplines tend to distance themselves from one another. In addition to these two distinct contributions to the extant literature, Ferguson (2010) also employed stringent inclusion criteria in an effort to perform the most methodologically rigorous meta-analysis examining the genetic and environmental contributions to antisocial behavior to date. For example, Ferguson (2010) used the same stringent definition of antisocial behavior employed by Rhee and Waldman

(2002). In addition, only studies which employed twin-based and adoption research designs were included in the meta-analysis. These criteria ultimately resulted in a final analytic sample of 53 separate effects nested within 38 studies. Collectively, these studies analyzed nearly

100,000 twin pairs/adoptees (N = 96,918). Similar to previous meta-analyses, Ferguson (2010) also examined the potential moderating effects of sex, age, and the measurement strategy used to operationalize antisocial behavior (self-report vs. observational measures).

The results indicated that the average heritability of antisocial behavior across all of the examined studies was .56, indicating that 56 percent of the overall variance in antisocial behavior was explained by genetic influences. While genetic influences accounted for the largest proportion of the overall variance in antisocial behavior, shared environmental influences accounted for approximately 11 percent of the variance, and the remaining 31 percent of the variance was explained by nonshared environmental influences. Further, the results indicated that standardized measures of antisocial behavior (i.e., antisocial personality disorder as defined

evolutionary psychology is not a theory of individual-level behavior (Daly & Wilson, 1988), but rather a separate paradigm aimed at situating the explanations of human phenotypes within the principles of biological evolution. While evolutionary psychology can be considered a stand-alone paradigm or perspective, Barnes (2014) has previously suggested that the biosocial perspective encompasses several other perspectives including evolutionary psychology. In this way, evolutionary psychology can be viewed as one of the many explanatory tools that the biosocial perspective brings to the table all in the interest of providing a more comprehensive explanation of phenotypic variation. 45 by the DSM-IV) yield lower heritability estimates relative to studies which employ broader measures of antisocial behavior. The operationalization of antisocial behavior was also found to significantly moderate nonshared environmental influences, but not shared environmental influences. As in the previous meta-analysis performed by Rhee and Waldman (2002), the results also indicated that age significantly moderated genetic influences wherein samples comprised of older respondents tended to yield smaller heritability coefficients relative to samples of younger respondents. Relatedly, age was also found to significantly moderate nonshared environmental influences wherein older samples yielded significantly higher estimates of e2 relative to younger samples. While age did not significantly moderate the shared environment, such influences did trend downwardly across various age categories. Sex did not significantly moderate genetic, shared environmental, or nonshared environmental influences.

2.2.2 Discussion of Findings

Taken together, the results of these four meta-analyses produce at least three key findings. First, all four of the studies indicate that approximately half of the overall variance in antisocial behavior (Mason & Frick, 1994: h2 = .48; Miles & Carey, 1997: h2 = .50; Rhee &

Waldman, 2002: h2 = .41; Ferguson, 2010: h2 = .56) is the direct result of additive genetic influences. Second, nonshared environmental influences explained the second greatest proportion of the overall variance in each study. Third, the shared environment accounted for the smallest portion of the overall variance in each study. Importantly, each of these three findings directly coincide with Turkheimer’s (2000) three laws of behavior genetics. Prior to broadly contextualizing these findings within current criminological research, it is first important to reiterate a few points regarding each of the meta-analyses reviewed here. As previously mentioned, all four of these meta-analyses were conducted over an extended period of time and

46 therefore have only a limited amount of overlap in regard to the studies and effects they synthesize. In addition, each meta-analysis makes a unique contribution to the literature and effectively builds on the previous one in terms of methodological rigor. Finally, the collective findings from these meta-analyses span several decades (1975 to 2006). In this way, a consensus in findings among these studies provides a strong indication that the collective findings are highly robust.

Based on these considerations, the findings gleaned from these four studies are directly relevant to the field of criminology in at least three ways. First, the overall findings indicate that, by any reasonable standards, genes and environments significantly influence antisocial behavior.

In other words, the overall pattern of results favor a biosocial explanation of antisocial behavior, which in turn, indicates that a purely biological or sociological explanation of antisocial behavior is highly unlikely and fails to comport with a large segment of empirically-rigorous research spanning several decades. Based on these findings, the most advantageous and efficient approach to understanding the underlying etiology of antisocial and criminal behaviors would be to employ a biosocial approach.

While the studies, and the discussion of them presented above, tend to provide a heavy emphasis on the genetic contributions to antisocial behavior, this is simply a result of the overall scarcity of research and theoretical development devoted to the genetic influences on such behaviors in the field of criminology and other social science disciplines. In addition, genetic influences consistently accounted for the largest proportion of the overall variance in antisocial behavior in each of the studies. As outlined previously, most statistical models (which include multiple independent variables) presented in criminological studies typically only account for approximately 10 to 20 percent of the overall variance in the examined outcome (Weisburd &

47

Piquero, 2008). In light of these findings, it is difficult to imagine that a single variable—genetic influences—can account for 50 percent of the overall variance in such outcomes.6 Currently, no other variable known to criminologists can effectively and consistently account for one-half of the overall variance in antisocial or criminal behaviors. As a comparison, the results of the now classic meta-analysis performed by Pratt and Cullen (2000) revealed that self-control, widely considered one of the most consistent and “strongest correlates of crime” (p. 952), resulted in an average effect size that was consistently greater than .20 but less than .30 (range: .213-.278).

Based on these findings, the overall emphasis on the genetic influences on antisocial behavior becomes a bit more understandable; however, as its name implies, the biosocial perspective is not solely concerned with biological or genetic influences. While genetic influences account for approximately one-half of the overall variance in antisocial behavior, the remaining half of the variance is explained by environmental influences. Based on these findings, failing to account for either set of influences would result in suboptimal theories and statistical models which are, even under the most ideal situation, only able to account for a fraction of the overall variance in the outcomes of interest. In this way, thoroughly accounting for both sets of influences would ultimately result in more effective theories and statistical models.

Second, the results also indicate that failing to employ genetically-sensitive research designs may result in model misspecification and any resulting significant associations may be spurious due to genetic confounding (Johnson et al., 2009; McGue et al., 2010). Put differently, models which do not effectively account for genetic influences on the outcome of interest cannot effectively isolate the association between the independent variable and the dependent variable.

This particular finding, coupled with the overall lack of genetically-informed research within the

6 It is important to reiterate the explanation of the h2 estimate garnered from behavior genetic studies. While the h2 coefficient represents a single latent variable, this variable is directly informed by the complete set of additive influences from all genes within the entire genome. 48 field of criminology, raises serious concerns regarding the empirical status of the vast majority of theoretical perspectives within the field of criminology. Since such theoretical perspectives are built upon empirical foundations that are potentially wrought with spurious associations, the potential ramifications for these theories are not currently clear. This is primarily a result of the overall lack of research within the field of criminology directly aimed at integrating such theories within the biosocial perspective. While more research is needed before direct theoretical suggestions can be offered, there are potentially serious implications that may arise out of such methodological limitations.

The third way in which the findings gleaned from these four meta-analyses have direct application to criminological research is in regard to the importance of differentiating between shared and nonshared environmental influences. More specifically, the results of the four meta- analyses collectively indicate that nonshared environmental influences have far more influence on antisocial behavior relative to shared environmental influences. This particular finding indicates that the environmental influences that criminological theory and research should focus on are nonshared environmental influences. In this way, theories and research that provide an increased focus on nonshared environments (relative to shared environments) would likely result in greater explanatory power and an overall better understanding of the underlying etiology of antisocial phenotypes. In addition, this finding also reiterates the importance of the further integration of genetically-sensitive research designs into the field of criminology and other social sciences, since such methodologies present the only feasible analytic strategy allowing for the partitioning of shared environmental influences from nonshared environmental influences.

49

2.3 Gene-Environment Interplay

The study designs and research discussed above primarily focuses on decomposing the variance in a given measure into thee latent components: h2, c2, and e2. Each of these three components has been described in detail above. While a limited discussion was devoted to the different ways in which genetic effects may interact with one another (i.e., epistasis and dominance), the majority of the discussion regarding genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior has facetiously implied that these two sets of influences exclusively work in isolation from one another. In actuality, this is an oversimplification of reality since genetic and environmental influences frequently work in tandem to influence a given phenotype. According to Anthony Walsh (2009), “[g]enes and environments are separate entities as are hydrogen and oxygen, and can be analyzed as such, but when they have worked their magic and produced a phenotype they are not more separable than hydrogen and oxygen are when talking about the water they produce” (p. 33). To further extend this metaphor, Walsh (2011) states that “any trait or behavior of any living thing is always the result of biological factors interacting with environmental factors” (emphasis in original, p. xi). In other words, examining the independent influences of genes and the environment is an empirical endeavor that will likely result in theories and statistical models that oversimplify reality and human behavior. An alternative would be to examine how genetic and environmental influences work together in the development of antisocial behavior. This particular line of reasoning has come to be the prevailing consensus among biosocial criminologists, who now recognize the importance of specifying the processes in which genetic and environmental influences work together collectively to influence behavioral phenotypes. This culmination of genetic and environmental influences has come to be known as gene-environment interplay. The remainder of this chapter will be devoted to describing the two most common forms of gene-environment interplay—gene 50

X environment interactions (G × E) and gene-environment correlation (rGE)—and the research which applies these concepts to antisocial behavior.

2.3.1 Gene-Environment Interaction (G × E)

Criminological theories, particularly theories that focus exclusively on macro-level socializing agents (e.g., social disorganization theory), typically propose that various aspects of environment in which an individual is reared in have measurable and distinct influences on subsequent behavioral outcomes. For example, Sampson and colleagues (1997) argue neighborhoods with overall lower levels of are more likely to experience disproportionately high levels of violence, even after controlling for other social and organizational characteristics of neighborhoods. While studies have found support for the theoretical concept of collective efficacy (Morenoff, Sampson, & Raudenbush, 2001; Sampson,

Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Sampson Morenoff, Earls, 1999), this seemingly powerful theoretical concept still fails to explain the overall low prevalence of criminal behavior among individuals who reside in neighborhoods with exceedingly high levels of disadvantage, disorganization, and inefficacy.7 This observation serves as a powerful example of the wide range of heterogeneous responses of individuals who are exposed to a similar set of environmental influences.

The concept of G × E can provide a better understanding of the overall variability in response to highly similar environmental influences. Simply put, each individual has a unique composition of genes (typically referred to as a “genome”) which result in differential genetic predisposition toward certain phenotypes (as evidenced in the previous section). This genetic

7 Interestingly, Sampson and collegues (1997) do not offer descriptive statistics for their measures of violence, victimization, and homicide rates in their study. However, additional analysis of the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) data indicate that the majority of youth included in the sample (55.5%) refrained from any delinquent behavior over the study period, and an even greater proportion of youth refrained from any violent criminal behavior (67%) during the same time period (Fagan & Wright, 2011). 51 predisposition toward a given phenotype may also differ from one person to another based on differential exposure to environmental risk factors. The reverse holds true also: the influence of environmental risk factors on a given phenotype may vary as a function of different levels of genetic predisposition. In this way, a G × E may be interpreted much in the same way as a traditional multiplicative interaction term in SSSMs, the effect of one independent variable on the dependent variable is moderated by another independent variable. In other words, individuals who are most susceptible to certain phenotypes are those who exhibit the highest levels of genetic predisposition toward such phenotypes and experience a higher concentration of environmental influences that have been found to influence such phenotypes.

An example will likely provide a further clarification of the concept of a G × E. In this example, suppose that the results of a BG model examining violent criminal behavior identifies two distinct groups within a sample, one with a relatively high h2 estimate (h2 = .70) and the other with a lower h2 estimate (h2 = .30). In addition, further analyses reveal that levels of parental maltreatment during childhood significantly moderate genetic influences on violent behavior. The results of this hypothetical example are presented in Figure 3.1. The predicted probability of engaging in violent behavior is plotted on the y-axis and the x-axis presents the level of parental maltreatment each respondent experienced during childhood. The solid line represents respondents with overall higher levels of genetic risk (h2 = .70) and the dashed line represents respondents with overall lower levels of genetic risk (h2 = .30). Three specific patterns in the resulting figure warrant additional attention. First, individuals with higher levels of genetic risk seem to be at a higher risk for violent behavior than individuals with lower levels of genetic risk. In other words, across all levels of parental maltreatment, individuals with higher levels of genetic risk had a higher predicted probability of engaging in violent criminal behavior.

52

Second, parental maltreatment also seems to have a significant effect on the probability of engaging in violent behavior. In both groups (high vs. low genetic risk), the probability of engaging in violent behavior increased as levels of parental maltreatment increased. Third, while parental maltreatment has a significant effect on violent behavior across both sets of respondents, respondents with higher levels of genetic risk had a much steeper slope relative to individuals with lower levels of genetic risk. This final pattern is highly indicative of a G × E, in that we see independent effects from the genetic risk and the environmental risk measures, but the greatest effect occurs when both sets of factors are present. Keep in mind that individuals who experienced only high environmental risk or only high genetic risk are far less likely than individuals who experience both sets of risk factors to engage in violent behaviors. This is similar to the heterogeneity in behavioral response to purely social risk factors observed in the extant literature discussed above.

2.3.2 Gene-Environment Correlation (rGE)

The second type of gene-environment interplay is referred to as gene-environment correlation (rGE). Where G × Es illustrate the process in which genetic and environmental processes interact with one another to produce phenotype variance, rGEs highlight the processes wherein genetic influences can produce variation in exposure to different environments (Beaver,

2013a). The idea that an environment may be influenced by one’s genome may be somewhat difficult to grasp when viewed through the traditional criminological lens. More specifically, virtually all criminological theories and research assume that the causal arrow is drawn from the socializing agent to the individual, not the other way around. In addition, environmental influences are typically viewed a “purely social” and are free from genetic influence since

53 environments do not possess genomes and are not under genetic influence. While the idea that environments may actually be influenced by genetic influences may seem to be implausible on its face, there is a significant amount of empirical research supporting this claim (Jaffee & Price,

2007; Kendler & Baker, 2007; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). A line of research has revealed that individual-level variation in exposure to various environments is directly and significantly influenced by genetic factors. In one of the first and most comprehensive conceptualizations of rGEs, Scarr and McCartney (1983) reiterated this point directly when they observed that “the genome, in both its species specificity and its individual variability, largely determines environmental effects on development, because the genotype determines the organism’s responsiveness to environmental opportunities” (p. 424). In an effort to provide a better understanding of the underlying logic of rGEs, it may be beneficial to provide an explanation of the three types of rGEs that have been identified in previous studies: passive rGEs, active rGEs, and evocative rGEs.

The first type of rGE is referred to as a passive rGEs and is a direct result of parents providing their offspring with both their genotype and environment. Since both sets of influences emanate from a common source (i.e., parents), they will be positively correlated with one another. For example, a child with two parents who are both highly aggressive and antisocial is at an increased risk for being raised in a physically abusive or neglectful environment. In addition, this child’s parents will also pass whatever genetic predispositions toward aggression and antisocial behaviors they possess. In this way, the child’s environment

(i.e., abuse and neglect) is positively correlated with their genome (which includes a genetic predisposition toward aggressive and antisocial behavior).

54

The second type of rGE is referred to as an evocative rGE and is the direct result of one’s genome eliciting responses from an environment which are positively correlated with genetic predisposition. The concept of genes eliciting responses from the environment may seem inherently unintuitive initially since genes are not active organisms that earnestly work toward some particular goal (the sole purpose of any given gene is far more mundane: to produce proteins). However, the concept of an evocative rGE becomes far more clear when the processes which mediate the relationship between the genome and environmental responses are specified more directly. Genes to not directly elicit responses from the environment, instead, genes influence various phenotypes (e.g., personality and behavioral traits) which, in turn, elicit different responses from the environment. For example, children who display high levels of antisocial behavior, a phenotype that is directly influenced by genetic factors, may elicit more frequent and harsh from their parents. In this particular example, the phenotype of antisocial behavior is influenced by genes, which ultimately are correlated with the elicited parental response of more frequent and harsh punishment.

The third and final type of rGE is referred to as an active rGE and takes into account the important role that the genome plays in the selection into specific environments that allow for optimal genetic expression. In other words, individuals will seek out specific environments (and actively avoid other environments) which most consistently align with their individual-level genetic predispositions (Plomin et al., 2013). Directly in line with the first law of behavior genetics (Turkheimer, 2000), it can be expected that selection into various environments, so called “niche-seeking,” is at least partially influenced by genetic factors. For example, more athletically-inclined youth are typically drawn to more physical activities and spend more time engaging in such activities relative to youth who are less athletically-inclined.

55

2.3.3 Gene-Environment Interplay and Antisocial Behavior

Now that the basic principles of gene-environment interplay have been introduced, it is possible to discuss the application of such concepts to the study of antisocial behavior. While the overall number of criminological studies employing these techniques and concepts may be lacking, there has been a significant amount of research outside of criminology which indirectly ties gene-environment interplay to theoretical concepts that are highly relevant within criminology. The purpose of this section is only to provide a cursory overview of this line of research, since Chapter 4 will provide a more detailed discussion of the extant literature linking the biosocial perspective to theoretical concepts pertinent within the field of criminology.

Early studies attempting to examine G × Es on antisocial phenotypes utilized adoption samples due to the ability to model both genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior (Cadoret, Cain, & Crowe, 1983; Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Woodworth, & Stewart,

1995; Mednick et al., 1984). These studies operationalize genetic risk as each adoptee’s biological parents score on the antisocial behavior measure and environmental risk as each adoptee’s adoptive parents score on the same antisocial behavior measure. The results of these studies have provided preliminary support the presence of a G × E in the development of antisocial behavior, wherein adoptees who have a biological parent and an adoptive parent who possess high scores on the employed measure of antisocial behavior are most likely to display higher levels of antisocial behavior themselves. More recently, studies have extended the traditional BG modeling strategies in an attempt to determine whether genetic influences are significantly moderated by specific environmental contexts (Carlson, Mendle, & Harden, 2014).

The results of these studies have revealed specific environmental contexts that significantly moderate genetic influences on antisocial behavior such as classroom context (Lamb,

Middledorp, Van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2012), pubertal timing (Harden & Mendle, 2012), 56 preschool attendance (Tucker-Drob & Harden, 2012), and parental maltreatment (Jaffee, Caspi,

Moffitt, Dodge, Rutter, Taylor, & Tully, 2005).

The main drawback in utilizing a BG modeling strategy to study whether specific environments significantly moderate genetic influences on a given phenotype is that such methodologies model genetic influences as a latent factor and do not provide information regarding the specific genes involved in the G × E. Over the past decade, a developing line of research, typically referred to as molecular genetic research, has focused on identifying specific genes and environments that interact to directly contribute to the underlying etiology of antisocial behavior. Briefly, molecular genetics8 is a field of study which is focused on examining how genetic variation contributes to various phenotypes including antisocial behavior.

More specifically, a small percentage of genes, between 1 and 10 percent, can differ between individuals within the general population (Frazer, Murray, Schork, & Topol, 2009). These variations are referred to as alleles or polymorphisms and, at least in some cases, have been found to directly contribute to different phenotypes. A significant number of genes have been theoretically and empirically linked to antisocial phenotypes, with the majority of these polymorphisms linked to the process of neurotransmission9 and the specific neurotransmitters dopamine and serotonin (for an overview see Morley & Hall, 2003). In addition, polymorphisms implicated in the production of enzymes that metabolize neurotransmitters from the synapse after

8 This discussion of molecular genetics is highly cursory since this dissertation will not employ a molecular genetic research design. However, findings from studies employing a molecular genetic research design are directly relevant to the overall focus of the current study (i.e., situating existing theoretical perspectives within the biosocial perspective), particularly in regard to extending the concept of G × Es to phenotypes related to antisocial behavior. For a more detailed overview of molecular genetics and their application to criminological research see Beaver (2009; 2013a). 9 Neurotransmission refers to the electrical and chemical processes in which neurons (nerve cells in the brain) communicate with one another. Neurons are separated by a small gap, referred to as the synapse, requiring the use of chemical messengers (referred to as neurotransmitters) to bridge the synapse and pass the message to an adjacent neuron. After the process of neurotransmission, excess neurotransmitters remain in the synapse and must be removed. One of the primary ways excess neurotransmitters are removed from the synapse is through the release of a specialized protein called an enzyme which effectively degrades the excess neurotransmitters. 57 neurotransmission have also been linked to antisocial phenotypes (Beaver, DeLisi, Vaughn, &

Barnes, 2010; Brunner et al., 1993; Volvaka, Bilder, & Nolan, 2004; Rujescu, Giegling, Gietl,

Hartmann, & Moller, 2003).

Molecular genetic research offers the unique opportunity to examine the potential association between a measured genetic and a given antisocial phenotype, but also to examine whether measured environmental influences significantly moderate such associations. The majority of the studies examining whether G × Es with specific genetic polymorphisms significantly contribute to the development of antisocial phenotypes have focused on the enzymatic breakdown gene monoamine oxidase A (MAOA; Caspi et al., 2002;

Foley et al., 2004; Haberstick et al., 2005; Schwartz & Beaver, 2011; Widom & Brzustowicz,

2006). Perhaps the most commonly examined moderating environmental influence examined in these studies is parental maltreatment, which the results of a meta-analysis have indicated significantly moderates the effect of MAOA on antisocial behavior (Kim-Cohen et al., 2006).

Additional combinations of measured polymorphisms and environments have also been found to significantly contribute to antisocial behaviors. For example, the influence of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) has been found to be significantly moderated by exposure to delinquent peers (Vaughn, DeLisi, Beaver, & Wright, 2009); family risk has been found to significantly interact with a dopamine receptor gene (DRD2) to produce early-onset of offending (DeLisi,

Beaver, Wright, & Vaughn, 2008); the serotonergic transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) has been found to significantly interact with adverse childhood events to produce antisocial personality disorder (Douglas et al., 2011).

A significant number of BG studies have examined rGEs on a wide range of environments, many of which are directly related to theoretical perspectives that are highly

58 influential within the field of criminology. While there are far too many individual studies examining the heritability of specific environments directly relevant to criminological theories, a recent meta-analysis by Kendler and Baker (2007) provides a thorough overview of the findings that can be culled from this line of research. This meta-analysis included a total of 55 studies which directly examined the genetic contributions to a wide variety of environments.

Importantly, four of the general environmental influences that Kendler and Baker examined are directly related to theoretical perspectives within criminology, including those that will be examined in the current study: parenting practices, family environment, peer interactions, and stressful life events.

First, the meta-analysis included 19 studies which examined various aspects of the parent-child relationship including: maternal/paternal warmth, maternal/paternal negativity, maternal/paternal control, and maternal/paternal monitoring. Importantly, as will be discussed in detail in the following chapter, parental socialization plays a central role in several influential criminological theories including social bonding theory (Hirschi, 1969) and Gottfredson and

Hirschi’s (1990) general theory of crime. The results of the meta-analysis revealed that environments related to the parent-child relationship are under moderate levels of genetic influence, with heritability estimates ranging between .12 and .37. Second, Kendler and Baker also included studies that examined other family environments that extended beyond parenting behaviors including family cohesion, family conflict, and family control. The results of the meta-analysis revealed that family environments are also moderately influenced by genetic influences with average heritability estimates ranging between .18 and .30.

Third, six studies examining the heritability of peer interactions were also included in the meta-analysis. Peer interactions also play a prominent role in various explanations of criminal

59 behavior, with perhaps the most influential and well-known example being Akers’ (1998) social learning theory. The results of the meta-analysis revealed that the average genetic effect size on peer interactions was .21. However, it is important to point out that while Kendler and Baker examined various aspects of peer interactions, they did not directly examine interactions with delinquent peers. Fortunately, a number of studies have directly examined genetic influences on interaction with delinquent peers, with most studies reporting heritability coefficients of .40 or higher (Cleveland, Wiebe, Rowe, 2005; Fowler et al., 2007; Harden, Hill, Turkheimer, & Emery,

2008; Iervolino, Pike, Manke, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 2002; Kendler, Jacobson,

Gardner, Gillespie, Aggen, & Prescott, 2007). Fourth, Kendler and Baker included 10 studies which examined the heritability of stressful life events. Stressful, or strain-inducing, life events have been found to be positively associated with various forms of antisocial behavior and serve as the primary causal mechanism in some of the most influential and well-known criminological theories including Merton’s (1938, 1957) classic strain/ theory and Agnew’s (1992) general strain theory. The results of the meta-analysis revealed heritability estimates that ranged between .24 and .47, with an average weighted heritability coefficient of .28.

2.4 Summary and Discussion

This chapter presented an overview of the basic assumptions, concepts, and methodologies that underlie the biosocial perspective. In addition, the findings from a chronologically, methodologically, and theoretically mature literature focused on the underlying etiological development of antisocial behavior were discussed. Based on this summary of the literature, four key points with direct application to existing criminological theoretical perspectives emerge and warrant additional attention. First, there is now undisputable evidence that antisocial behavior (which inherently includes criminal behavior) is the direct result of a

60 culmination of environmental and genetic influences. More specifically, genetic influences explain approximately 50 percent of the overall variance in antisocial behavior and the remaining

50 percent of the variance is explained by environmental influences, with nonshared environmental influences being far more potent than shared environmental influences. Second, the culmination of genetic and environmental influences, through G × Es and rGEs, tend to have larger effects on antisocial behavior than the independent effects of each set of influences.

Third, studies performed under the biosocial perspective, but in disciplines other than criminology, have provided evidence that any studies attempting to better specify the underlying etiological development of any phenotype but do not effectively account for genetic influences are likely misspecified and yield statistical coefficients that are essentially meaningless. Further, the theoretical implications of this particular observation are currently unknown, but there is good reason to believe that attempting to situate existing criminological theories within the biosocial perspective may significantly attenuate previously observed associations between some of the most widely-recognized theoretical concepts and antisocial behavior. For example, peer interactions, a central concept to social learning theory (Akers, 1998), have been found to be significantly influenced by genetic factors (Cleveland et al., 2005; Fowler et al., 2007; Harden et al., 2008; Iervolino et al., 2002; Kendler et al., 2007). Along the same lines, some studies indicate that specific genetic polymorphisms (i.e., a dopamine receptor gene [DRD2]) cluster within peer groups, suggesting that youth tend to select peers with genetic predispositions similar to their own (Fowler, Settle, & Christakis, 2011). A sizable literature has also indicated that the association between parental socialization, a key theoretical concept in self-control and social control theories, and various antisocial outcomes becomes highly attenuated or nonsignificant after taking shared genetic influences between parents and their children into account (Harris,

61

1995, 2009; Jaffee et al., 2004; Lytton, 1990; Wright & Beaver, 2005). In addition, based on the first law of behavior genetics (Turkheimer, 2000), all other key theoretical concepts would be under at least some genetic influence.

While such observations provide some preliminary evidence which suggests that any previously observed association between theoretical concepts and antisocial outcomes will be at least attenuated when genetic influences are properly controlled, the direct results of tests examining these associations within the confines of a genetically informed model remain unknown. This is a direct result of the overall lack of criminological research that has been done under the biosocial perspective. The current project aims to provide a more thorough understanding of the predictive ability of theoretical concepts central to rational choice theory, social learning theory, classic strain theory, and social bonding theory after properly accounting for genetic influences. Once this information has been gathered, it will then be possible to determine how to situate each of these theoretical perspectives within the biosocial perspective.

62

0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Predicted Probability Violenceof 1 2 3 4 Level of Parental Maltreatment

High Genetic Risk Low Genetic Risk

Figure 3.1: Graphical Example of a Gene-Environment Interaction.

63

CHAPTER 3

CONVENTIONAL CRIMINOLOGICAL THEORY: EMPIRICAL FINDINGS AND CONCERNS

The biosocial perspective is rapidly developing, such that results from hundreds of studies now indicate that both biological and environmental influences work—both independently and interactively—to generate variation in antisocial phenotypes (Ferguson, 2010;

Mason & Frick, 1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Rhee & Waldman, 2002). While a small number criminologists remain openly and vehemently opposed to a more “biological” criminology, this underlying blind opposition is becoming increasingly indefensible and unpopular. Despite a few flamboyant examples (see Carrier & Walby, 2014; Walby & Carrier, 2010), criminologists now seem to be more accepting of biosocial explanations of antisocial behavior than ever before.

While the overall presence of biosocial research may be lacking in many of the leading journals within the field of criminology, the number of presentations at professional conferences, the number of freshly minted PhDs specializing in biosocial criminology, and the number of criminology programs that offer some sort of biosocial criminology course continues to grow each year. Whether this growth is the result of a fundamental shift in the underlying ideology within the field of criminology or is driven by demand from curious students, practitioners, and researchers remains an open question. Regardless of the source of the shift, the present represents an exciting time within the field of criminology.

Despite the substantial gains in the underlying understanding of the contributions of biological and environmental influences on antisocial behavior made over the past decade or two, the processes that eventually culminate into antisocial and criminal behaviors are still relatively unknown. In other words, while a large swath of research has indicated that genetic

64 and environmental influences directly contribute to antisocial phenotypes, the underlying processes that specify the manner in which both sets of influences eventually lead to antisocial behavior have not been explicitly detailed. This lack of specificity is likely a direct result of the overall absence of any theoretical progress within the biosocial perspective. As previously mentioned, the few attempts to specify a biosocial theory of criminal behavior (Ellis, 2005;

Robinson & Beaver, 2009) have been largely unsuccessful. However, these particular theories were concerned with distilling the entire biosocial perspective down into a single theory as opposed to fitting a single theory within the confines of the biosocial perspective. For example,

Ellis (2005) attempts to specify a biosocial theory of antisocial behavior by drawing on concepts from multiple disciplines including evolutionary psychology, neuroscience, endocrinology, psychiatry, and psychology while also attempting to incorporate known environmental factors that have previously been shown to increase predisposition toward antisocial behavior such as exposure to cigarette smoke in utero. Ellis also attempts to incorporate aspects of genetic risk and concentrations of various neurochemicals including cortisol and various neurotransmitters.

Even this cursory overview illustrates the primary limitation of the theory: it is overly complex.

In an effort to integrate all of the findings from biosocial research, Ellis proposes a theory that collapses under its own weight.

An alternative approach has been embraced by a handful of biosocial criminologists.

More specifically, both Walsh (2000, 2002) and Fishbein (1990, 2001) have argued that formal and novel biosocial theories of criminal behavior are unnecessary and instead propose that biosocial concepts can be integrated into existing criminological theory. In perhaps the most direct example of this form of integration, Walsh (2000) attempted to integrate findings from the field of behavior genetics within Merton’s (1938) strain theory. Walsh (2000) explained that the

65 purpose of his inquiry was to “explore how behavior genetics can complement and expand traditional social science understanding of criminal behavior in the context of anomie/strain theory, a respected and long-lived criminological theory that focuses on cultural and structural causes of crime” (p. 1075). While this particular suggestion has greatly influenced the current dissertation, there is a fundamental flaw in the logic extended by Walsh and others calling for the integration of biosocial concepts within existing criminological theory. More specifically, this observation assumes that the direction of integration should point from biosocial concepts to existing criminological theory, which inherently assumes that concepts central to conventional theories have causal influences on antisocial behavior even after taking biological influences

(including genetics) into account. While this may be the case, the overall lack of studies examining associations between concepts central to such theories and antisocial outcomes within the confines of genetically informed models leaves this question open. Alternatively, if after controlling for genetic influences, concepts central to mainstream criminological theories are no longer associated with antisocial outcomes, the integration of biosocial concepts into such theories would be counterproductive, effectively diminishing the explanatory power of such theories even further. Based on these observations, the current dissertation proposes that the direction of integration should be reversed and should point from existing criminological theory to the biosocial perspective. This model of integration will be more thoroughly specified in

Chapter 4, but the underlying logic is presented here in an effort to effectively situate the discussion of the following theoretical perspectives within this context.

Prior to providing a more thorough discussion of the manner in which conventional criminological perspectives can be situated within the biosocial perspective, it is first necessary to provide an overview of the theories that will be examined in the current dissertation. The

66 remainder of this chapter will be devoted to providing an overview of each of the following theories: rational choice theory; social learning theory; classic strain theory; and social bonding theory. In this way, the current chapter has two primary objectives. First, a detailed discussion of each theory, and its central concepts, will be provided. While logical and empirical limitations noted in the literature will be noted and discussed, the primary purpose of the current chapter is to provide a detailed overview of each theoretical perspective. In this way, critiques of each perspective will be limited and focused only on criticisms noted by the author(s) of each perspective or those that have been noted in the literature. Second, an overview of the current empirical status of each theory will be provided. Importantly, this overview will be limited to only the most influential, comprehensive, and empirically-robust studies examining each particular theory. The theories examined in the current dissertation are some of the most influential theories of antisocial behavior within the social sciences and have resulted in literally thousands of studies. In this way, an exhaustive review of the empirical research surrounding each theory would result in hundreds of pages and would detract from the primary objectives of the current project.

3.1 Rational Choice Theory

Many of the conventional criminological theoretical perspectives that have received the most attention over the past several decades tend to shift focus away from processes that occur within the individual and, instead, attempt to specify the mechanisms that lead from various aspects of socialization or environmental influences to antisocial behavior. Many of these theories focus on concepts that exert some sort of influence on the agent in some way, primarily through socialization. Therefore, the underlying focus of such perspectives is on the

67 environmental influences as opposed to the individual. While some notable exceptions exist10, most theoretical perspectives tend to treat the internal responses to specific environmental influences as an afterthought. For example, while Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) general theory of crime focuses on individual-level factors that predict variation in antisocial behavior, this perspective does not detail the specific internal mechanisms that bridge the gap between parental socialization, the development of self-control, and antisocial behavior. What processes sparked by inadequate parental socialization ultimately result in overall lower levels of self- control? What internal mechanisms lead from possessing lower levels of self-control and ultimately engaging in criminal or antisocial behavior? These questions remain largely unanswered, not just in regard to self-control theory, but also within the majority of the most commonly studied criminological theories. Simply establishing an association between theoretical concepts and antisocial behavior does not sufficiently capture the complex internal processes that ultimately contribute to variation in antisocial behavior.

Deterrence theory, and other theories falling under the rational choice perspective, attempts to provide a more thorough specification of the internal process that ultimately results in antisocial behavior. In other words, rational choice theories attempt to better specify the mediating processes that connect external influences and variation in antisocial behavior. More specifically, the rational choice perspective attempts to specify the role of choice in the decision- making process that underlies all behavioral outcomes, including antisocial behavior. Why do some individuals eventually engage in criminal behavior while others, facing the same or similar

10 For example, (Becker, 1963; Cooley, 1902; Lemert, 1951; Mead, 1934) specifies the process from which external events (typically contact with the criminal justice system) results in internal modifications of one’s perception of self. The labeling perspective is unique in that it details both the external and internal processes that eventually result in antisocial behavior. Another notable example, would be Agnew’s (1992; Agnew et al., 2002) general strain theory (GST), which details the process in which external strain-inducing experiences trigger negative emotional states, which in turn, ultimately result in antisocial coping mechanisms. Another notable and relevant example was offered in the now classic work of Matza (1969), who specified the process by which individuals “become deviant.” 68 circumstances, refrain? Proponents of rational choice theories argue that in order to adequately answer this question, criminologists must take into account human agency, or the process in which human beings make decisions. In addition to providing a more robust and thorough understanding of criminal behavior, proponents also argue that providing a greater emphasis on this decision-making process will result in at least one other benefit. Criminal law is built upon the fundamental notion that individuals play a central role in the behaviors they ultimate engage in. In this way, individuals are punished for misconduct and rewarded for prosocial behavior.

This foundational concept operates under the assumption that humans actively choose their own actions and are therefore responsible for such actions. In this way, a failure to consider the decision-making process that underlies criminal behavior drives a wedge between current explanations of criminal behavior and the legal code (Nagin, 2007). In addition, this disconnect between theoretical explanations of criminal behavior and criminal law likely has negative repercussions for the extent to which criminological research can inform public policy. Below, a discussion of the key theoretical assumptions and concepts that comprise rational choice theories will be provided. In addition, findings from the empirical literature focusing on the rational choice perspective will be discussed.

3.1.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Rational Choice Theory

The basic assumption that human behavior is directly influenced by the expected consequences, whether they be good or bad, of such behaviors is a commonly held belief that has spilled over into criminological theory. Based on the pervasiveness of this assumption, it is not all that surprising that some of the first formally proposed criminological theories focused on the role of expected consequences in the decision-making process that ultimately resulted in behavior. The majority of the guiding ideas and logic underlying the rational choice perspective

69 were initially proposed throughout the 18th century by Cesare Beccaria (1963 [1764]) and Jeremy

Bentham (1948). Many of the underlying ideas of the rational choice perspective can even be traced back to Thomas Hobbes proposal of a “social contract” during the 16th century. During these periods of rapid social change, scholars began to recognize the utility of introducing proportional, yet adequately harsh, for criminal behavior in an effort to deter potential offenders from committing such acts. The organizing idea underlying the concept of was that prior to committing a given act, human beings weigh the potential costs and benefits that accompany engaging in (or failing to engage in) the act in question. In this way, many scholars hypothesized that it would be possible to deter potentially harmful and violent behaviors by attaching consequences to these behaviors that fit specific criteria. More specifically, punishments that accompany a specific behavior must be carried out as quickly as possible (celerity), have a high probability of being carried out (certainty), and must be proportional to the behavior committed (more severe behaviors should be responded to with more severe sanctions; certainty). Punishments that adequately balance these three characteristics were lauded by Beccaria, Bentham, and others as being the most likely to deter potentially harmful behaviors.

In order for deterrence to be possible, three basic assumptions regarding human nature must be present. First, human behavior must be the direct result of free will. In other words, humans possess the ability to freely choose whether they ultimately engage in a given behavior.

In direct opposition to free will is the classic assumption of determinism—the probability of the occurrence of a given event is directly related to and dependent upon the occurrence of other antecedent events (Walsh, 2014). Anything more strongly worded would constitute fatalism, not

70 determinism.11 In an attempt to further differentiate determinism from fatalism, Walsh (2014) stated that “[s]cientific determinism does not state that X will lead to Y absolutely and unerringly; rather, it says that given the presence of X, there is a certain probability that Y will occur” (p. 5). Regardless, the concept of deterrence rests on the assumption that humans possess free will, at least to some extent. Scholars have argued that free will and determinism (or fatalism) do not reflect a natural dichotomy, instead various gradients of each perspective are possible. For example, a number of scholars have argued in favor of constrained agency or bounded free will (for an overview see Swidler, 1986), wherein the potential menu of behavioral choices available to an individual is directly related to the environmental circumstances to which the individual is exposed to. In other words, individuals are only able to choose from the options that are available to them, and some individuals simply have more potential options to choose from than others. In this way, we cannot assume that humans have free will in the purest sense, but, humans still possess the ability to make decisions based on the (limited) available options.

The second assumption underlying the concept of deterrence, and the rational choice perspective, is that humans are hedonistic in that they tend to favor pleasure over pain. All else equal, humans will choose options that increase pleasure and minimize pain. Deterrence directly exploits this particular aspect of human nature by actively manipulating the potential pain and pleasure that accompanies various behaviors. For example, attaching a harsher penalty to a given criminal act increases the potential pain that an offender may experience if they are caught engaging in said act. This particular aspect of deterrence is directly related to the third assumption of the rational choice perspective and deterrence: humans are rational beings and

11 This is a common misconception among criminologists who assume, and often argue, that determinism dictates that “the offender’s behavior is ‘determined’ by something other than his or her free-willed choice” (Brown, Esbensen, & Geis, 2013; p. 218). This description would more closely reflect fatalism as opposed to determinism, which Walsh (2014) argues underlies any form of science and is a position held by all scientists. 71 weigh the potential costs and benefits of a given act before acting. More specifically, Bentham

(1948), and later others, argued that prior to the commission of a given act, humans undergo a

“hedonistic calculus” in which they weigh the probability of pleasure maximization against the pain of punishment. This particular process, which eventually culminates into a choice, is one of the defining features of the rational choice perspective and details the internal processes that eventually culminate into antisocial behavior. The assumption that humans are rational beings stems primarily from the field of economics and the principle of expected utility (for an overview see Schoemaker, 1982) which asserts that individuals will make rational decisions with the intent to maximize profits and minimize costs or losses. According to the rational choice perspective, this same rationale can be directly applied to all other behavioral outcomes including criminal behavior. More specifically, offenders will engage in criminal behavior if they perceive the potential benefits of such behavior outweigh the potential costs.

The underlying assumption that humans are rational and hedonistic beings who freely choose whether to participate or refrain from criminal activity directly aligns with the concept of deterrence. More specifically, deterrence asserts that the proper manipulation of the costs and benefits which underlie various behaviors will significantly increase or decrease the likelihood that individuals will engage in such behaviors. Sliding the weights of celerity, certainty, and severity of punishment alters the information that is processed by potential offenders and ultimately results in a decrease in the likelihood of engaging in antisocial or criminal behaviors.

Based on these observations, it is no surprise that the concept of deterrence was highly influential in the development and evolution of the American justice system. This form of deterrence— typically referred to as formal deterrence—posits that formal social controls (e.g., police, judges, legal codes) are responsible for any potential deterrent effect. Alternatively, informal deterrence

72 focuses on the potential social (as opposed to legal) consequences that accompany misbehavior.

Importantly, a findings from a number of studies have revealed that informal deterrent effects are far more influential than formal deterrent effects (Green, 1989; Grasmick & Bursik, 1990; Pratt,

Cullen, Blevins, Daigle, & Madensen, 2006; Williams & Hawkins, 1989; Zimring & Hawkins,

1973). However, these findings are far from unequivocal and a number of additional studies have found that the formal deterrent effects outweigh informal deterrent effects (Nagin &

Paternoster, 1991; Nagin & Pogarsky, 2001). Importantly, some early studies have suggested that any deterrent effect stemming from formal punishment is likely indirectly influenced by the informal or social consequences that also stem from formal interventions (Williams & Hawkins,

1989; Zimring & Hawkins, 1973). In either case, the rational choice perspective, and subsequently the concept of deterrence, has resulted in a significant number of empirical studies.

The section below will provide an overview of the empirical literature examining the rational choice perspective within the context of antisocial and criminal behavior.

3.1.2 The Empirical Status of Rational Choice Theory

The rational choice perspective has a storied past within the field of criminology. Most criminologists would agree that the fundamental building blocks for the field of criminology were directly connected with the rational choice perspective (Brown et al., 2013). The primary and most influential organizing principles of the Classical School of Criminology directly aligned with the main premises and assumptions of the rational choice perspective and deterrence theory (Akers & Sellers, 2013). Needless to say, the rational choice perspective—and deterrence theory specifically—was quite influential during criminology’s infancy. While the rational choice perspective was eventually replaced by the positivist perspective in the early

1900s, and then later by purely sociological theories of criminal behavior beginning in the 1920s,

73 a renewed interest in the rational choice perspective was sparked by advances in the field of economics, specifically in regard to the principle of expected utility described above during the late 1960s (Pratt et al., 2006; Lilly et al., 2011; Nagin, 1998). Since this time, the rational choice perspective has remained a mainstay within the literature. While the rational choice perspective no longer carries the same level of influence on criminological theory as it once did, this perspective still actively influences modern criminological research. The longevity of this perspective, along with the steady level of interest associated with it, has generated an impressive number of studies. To put the overall size of the literature in perspective, there have been at least five different systematic reviews of the literature related to the rational choice perspective within criminology (Blumstein, Cohen, & Nagin, 1978; Nagin, 1998, 2013; Paternoster, 1987; Pratt et al., 2006). In their meta-analytic review of the literature, Pratt and colleagues (2006) argue that deterrence theory “is one of the most extensively tested theories of crime” (p. 374). Below, a summary of this expansive literature will be offered. This summary will focus on four distinct areas of the existing literature: (1) the rationality of offenders; (2) the effectiveness of deterrence;

(3) the effectiveness of formal and informal deterrence; and (4) modern developments in the rational choice perspective.

Rationality of Offenders. Despite the pervasive influence of the rational choice perspective on the field of criminology and the legal code, there has been little research actually focusing on the rationality of offenders. This particular observation is somewhat surprising since the assumption of rationality is one of the primary, and inherently necessary, assumptions of the rational choice perspective. In addition, one of the primary advantages of the rational choice perspective is the identification of the mechanisms within the individual which ultimately explain variation in offending. The majority of the studies that have attempted to specify the decision

74 making process that underlies criminal behavior analyze qualitative data, typically in the form of interviews with successful offenders. Tunnell (1990, 1992) interviewed repeat property offenders in an effort to gain a better understanding of the decision making process that eventually culminated into the choice of criminal behavior. Offenders tended to believe that their overall chances of being caught were relatively low, and that if they were caught, they would receive a lenient punishment. Importantly, this particular finding directly aligns with more recent studies which indicate that offenders with higher levels of contact with the criminal justice system (or are in direct contact with others who have higher levels of contact) are significantly more likely to engage in subsequent criminal behavior compared to their counterparts (Paternoster & Piquero, 1995; Piquero & Paternoster, 1998; Piquero & Pogarsky,

2002). This somewhat counterintuitive finding has been referred to as the experiential effect

(Paternoster, 1987) or the resetting effect (Pogarsky & Piquero, 2003).

Despite this congruence in findings, this particular pattern of behavior does not necessarily constitute rational decision making, at least not in the purest sense. While the offenders interviewed by Tunnell (1990, 1992) described the measures they would take to avoid detection, their overall assessment of the risk of detection was highly unrealistic. A closer examination revealed that these offenders, did not accurately assess the probability of arrest, performed only menial levels of planning prior to the commission of a particular crime, and were uninformed of the most likely legal penalties for the they committed. A similar set of findings emerged from an ethnographic study of burglars performed by Cromwell and colleagues

(1991), who concluded that opportunity and other situational factors played the most important role in previous burglaries. More recently, De Haan & Vos (2003) interviewed a sample of offenders who have previously committed street robberies, and argued that the potential rewards

75 stemming from these criminal behaviors extend beyond the money or goods they obtain in such crimes. Rather, additional factors figured into the decision making process include emotional and psychological factors such as the release of tension and desperation. Another recent study

(Dugan, LaFree, & Piquero, 2005), focused on the potential role that recent increases in the certainty and severity of punishment played in overall decreases in airplane hijackings. These modifications to the potential punishments which accompany hijackings were found to be more effectively deter offenders, providing support for the rational choice perspective.

Previous studies have attempted to account for this discrepancy in findings by arguing for models of partial or bounded rationality (De Haan & Vos, 2003; Matsueda, Kreager, & Huizinga,

2006). This explanation takes into account variability in the potential choices available to offenders, which stem from various social influences. Rather than assuming that individuals are perfectly rational, more modern theorists have argued that the underlying mechanisms driving human behavior are far more complex and subject to many moderating and mediating processes.

In an effort to better capture this notion of partial rationality, Paternoster and Pogarsky (2009;

Paternoster, Pogarsky, & Zimmerman, 2011) introduced the concept of thoughtfully reflective decision making (TRDM), which refers to “intelligent or ‘competent’ decision making because it is the best way to sift among alternative courses of action for the most effect way to reach one’s desired goals” (Paternoster et al., 2011, p. 2). An empirical examination of TRDM revealed that adolescents who scored higher on the TRDM measure were more likely to be in better health, were less likely to use drugs and alcohol, were engaged in more community involvement, and were less likely to engage in criminal behavior (Paternoster et al., 2011). The results of this study provide support for the rational choice perspective since respondents who were more likely

76 to thoughtfully assess the potential options available were less likely to engage in delinquent or risky behaviors and more likely to engage in prosocial behaviors.

Effectiveness of Deterrence. In direct contrast to the minimal amount of criminological research examining the rationality of offenders, a sizable number of studies have directly examined the effectiveness of various aspects of deterrence or deterrence theory. Fortunately, a relatively recent meta-analysis performed by Pratt and colleagues (2006) directly attempted to empirically synthesize this line of research in an effort to directly examine the current state of deterrence theory. Pratt and colleagues examined all studies published before 2003 and utilized stringent inclusion criteria which ultimately resulted in the inclusion of 40 studies and a total of

200 effect sizes. A total of four “predictor domains” directly related to deterrence theory were included in the study and included: certainty of punishment, severity of punishment, deterrence theory composites, and the threat of non-legal sanctions. Importantly, the authors of the study included a number of statistical procedures in an effort to estimate unbiased effect sizes including weighting estimates by sample size, correction for multiple estimates from each study, and correction for multivariate estimates.

The results of the meta-analysis produced five specific sets of findings directly related to deterrence theory. First, the overall mean effect sizes of the examined predictor domains on outcomes related to crime and/or are relatively weak, ranging between .00 and -.20.

The authors observe that these effect sizes are weaker than previous meta-analyses examining the potential association between alternative theoretical concepts and antisocial outcomes

(Andrews & Bonta, 1998; Pratt & Cullen, 2000). Second, even when significant mean effect sizes were observed, such effects become significantly attenuated, oftentimes to zero, in multivariate models. Third, studies which employed more rigorous methodological approaches

77 were significantly more likely to detect attenuated or nonsigificant effects. In addition, many of the strongest and most consistent effects were detected using bivariate modeling strategies, making such findings highly susceptible to confounding. Fourth, the certainty of punishment was among the strongest predictors of deterrence, particularly when predicting white-collar offenses such as fraud and tax violations. Fifth, non-legal sanctions were also among the most robust predictors of deterrence and severity of punishment was among the weakest. Pratt and colleagues (2006) interpret these results as direct evidence of the limited explanatory power of deterrence theory, even going as far as to state “[d]eterrence theory is likely to persist in much the same way despite the relatively poor predictive capacity of the variable specified in the theory” (p. 386). While the results of this study may be somewhat dated, a recent systematic review of the literature by Nagin (2013) seems to echo the findings. While Nagin praises the utility of the deterrence theory, he also recognizes the limitations of the theory, particularly in regard to punishment severity. While Nagin does not perform a meta-analysis, the overall conclusions drawn from his review directly coincide with the results reached by Pratt et al.

(2006). Taken together, these results suggest that empirical evidence illustrating the predictive ability of deterrence theory is—at best—mixed.

Formal and Informal Deterrence. Directly in line with one of the primary findings of the meta-analysis performed by Pratt and colleagues (2006), a parallel line of research has compared the effectiveness of formal versus informal sanction costs. In other words, deterrence theory— and, more broadly, the rational choice perspective—is concerned with the “pains of punishment” and the resulting influences such pains (whether real or perceived) have on the overall decision making process. However, the vast majority of early studies failed to recognize the importance of informal sanction costs and focused nearly exclusively on costs associated with formal

78 sanctions. During the 1970s and 1980s, when the rational choice perspective and deterrence theory received renewed interest within criminology, theorists began to recognize the importance of informal sanction costs (Anderson, Chiricos, & Waldo, 1977; Paternoster & Iovanni, 1986;

Salem & Bowers, 1970; Tittle & Rowe, 1973; Waldo & Chiricos, 1972; Williams & Hawkins,

1989; Zimring & Hawkins, 1973). In addition, several theorists recognized that any association between formal sanctions and deterrent effects is likely mediated by informal sanctions, wherein formal punishment activates specific informal ramifications which ultimately results in a deterrent effect (Salem & Bowers, 1970; Zimring & Hawkins, 1973). Findings from more recent studies seem to fall directly in line with this observation. For example, a developed and methodologically sophisticated line of research has revealed that individuals with greater stakes in conformity are most likely to be deterred by informal sanction costs (Nagin & Paternoster,

1993, 1994; Nagin & Pogarsky, 2001, 2003). Subsequent studies have attempted to identify additional individual characteristics which result in variability in deterrent effects (for an overview see Piquero, Paternoster, Pogarsky, & Loughran, 2011). Some of the more salient individual characteristics that have been identified include self-control (Piquero & Tibbetts,

1996; Pogarsky, 2007), moral inhibition (Gallupe & Baron, 2014; Paternoster & Simpson, 1996), levels of intoxication and anger (Exum, 2002), and religiosity (Spivak, Fukushima, Kelley, &

Jenson, 2011).

A related line of research indicates that informal deterrent effects are not limited to tangible social relationships and costs. Rather, internalized ramifications, such as feelings of shame and guilt, have also been found to have deterrent effects (Grasmick & Bursik, 1990;

Grasmick, Bursik, & Arneklev, 1993; Klepper & Nagin, 1989; Nagin & Paternoster, 1993;

Tibbetts & Meyers, 1999). Importantly, studies have found that the anticipation of shame has a

79 significant mediating effect on deterrence across both males and females, but females seem to be more susceptible relative to males (Tibbetts & Herz, 1996). The results of a recent study revealed that anticipated feelings of shame completely mediated the association between certainty of punishment and the intent to engage in delinquent behavior (Rebellon, Piquero,

Piquero, & Tibbetts, 2010). The results of this line of research seem to indicate that informal social costs seem to be driving the majority of any detectable deterrent effects. Furthermore, a complex process in which external and internal (e.g., feelings of shame) informal social costs differentially influence the underlying decision makings processes which culminate into behavioral outcomes including antisocial behavior.

Modern Developments. Due to the limited empirical evidence favoring deterrence theory and the rational choice perspective, several theorists have advocated for theoretical integration between deterrence theory and other popular theoretical perspectives (Gray, Ward, Stafford, &

Menke, 1985; Nagin & Paternoster, 1994; Paternoster et al., 2011; Piquero & Tibbetts, 1996;

Stafford & Warr, 1993).12 Two particular instances of theoretical integration in regard to deterrence theory have received a significant amount of empirical attention. First, Stafford and

Warr (1993) attempted to apply concepts from social learning theory (Akers, 1998) to deterrence theory. More specifically, offenders who have been detected and punished—or who are aware of others who have been—are significantly less likely to offend again in the future. However, offenders who engage in delinquent or criminal acts but are not detected or punished—or have knowledge of others who were failed to be detected—undergo “punishment avoidance” which

12 Importantly, the majority of these studies simply advocate for the “cross-fostering” of theoretical concepts among deterrence theory and additional theoretical perspectives as opposed to specifying an explicit integration model. For example, several theorists have recognized that various individual characteristics which are directly tied to additional theoretical perspectives (e.g., self-control) significantly predict variance in deterrent effects, but make no effort to specify the process in which integration between these two perspectives could be carried out. The majority of these studies do not even specify the direction of integration (whether deterrence theory should be integrated into other perspectives or vice versa), resulting in an incomplete explanation of the nature of integration proposed. 80 acts as a negative reinforcer which, in turn, results in a significant increase in the probability of future offending. In this way, the decision making process underlying antisocial behavior is shaped by the presence of punishment, the lack of punishment, and the modeling of others’ behavior (based on information regarding the detection or lack of detection passed on to the offender). Importantly, this reconceptualization of deterrence theory takes into account the direct experiences of the offender, but also accounts for information the offender has regarding others’ experiences. Importantly, subsequent studies have revealed that others’ experiences play an important role in the decision making process underlying antisocial behavioral outcomes (Anwar

& Loughran, 2011; Matsueda et al., 2006; Pogarsky, Piquero, & Paternoster, 2004).

Second, Piquero and Tibbetts (1996) integrated concepts from self-control and deterrence theories. More specifically, individual’s perceptions of the potential costs and benefits that accompany criminal behavior, in addition to their perception of being sanctioned, were significantly associated with self-control. In this way, individual characteristics significantly predict variation in the assessment of risk, but also in the probability of detection and both formal and informal sanction costs. A substantial number of additional studies have observed findings which illustrate the potentially important role of the trait of self-control within deterrence theory (Nagin & Pogarsky, 2001, 2003; Poarsky, 2002, 2007).

3.2 Social Learning Theory

As noted previously, many criminological theories simply identify associations between various sets of social influences and antisocial behavior, but fail to identify the underlying process and specific mechanisms driving the observed associations. Obviously, the rational choice perspective is one notable example, but so are learning theories. While many texts and studies errantly operationalize social learning theory as “a theory of bad companions or a

81

‘cultural deviance’ theory” (Akers & Jensen, 2006, p. 38), such a view is likely far too narrow, as social learning theory was proposed as a general theory of crime. According to Akers and

Jensen (2006), social learning theory was proposed as “a general theory that offers an explanation of the acquisition, maintenance, and change in criminal and deviant behavior that embraces social, nonsocial, and cultural factors operating both to motivate and control criminal behavior and both to promote and undermine conformity” (p. 38). Based on this explanation, social learning theory as a theoretical perspective has much more to offer than an explanation of the role that “bad companions” play in the development of antisocial behavior. Rather, social learning theory seeks to specify, explicitly, the processes which ultimately result in all antisocial and criminal behaviors.

Directly in line with this particular goal, social learning theory operates under the assumption that all behaviors are the direct result of a learning process. More directly, all behavior—including antisocial and criminal behavior—is learned just like anything else (Akers

& Sellers, 2013). However, social learning theory does not simply identify “learning” as the process in which criminal behaviors are passed from one person to another, the theory also specifies the processes which both promote and control criminal behavior. In addition, and perhaps most importantly, social learning theory also specifies the process in which criminal behavior is learned. This is an important departure from previous perspectives in that the social learning perspective explicitly identifies the manner in which external socializing processes are internalized by the individual and eventually influence behavioral patterns. In this way, social learning theory provides a greater level of detail in the description of the underlying etiology of antisocial and criminal behavior. For a number of reasons, social learning theory is one of the most frequently tested criminological perspectives (Akers & Sellers, 2013; Stitt & Giacopassi,

82

1992) and the criminological theory that is most favored among criminologists (Cooper, Walsh,

& Ellis, 2010; Ellis & Walsh, 1999). Below, a discussion of the main theoretical concepts which comprise social learning theory will be provided. The actual learning process which eventually culminates into behavior will also be specified.

3.2.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Social Learning Theory

While modern social learning theory is considered a distinct theoretical perspective, the overarching concepts and processes specified in the theory are rooted in Edwin Sutherland’s

(1947) theory of , which is widely recognized as one of the first purely sociological explanations of criminal behavior (Brown et al., 2013). Differential association operated under the assumption that all criminal behavior is learned in the same way anything else is learned. Based on this assumption Sutherland argued that differential association theory was a general theory of criminal behavior and possessed the ability to explain both street and white- collar crimes. While Sutherland’s explanations of criminal behavior focused on explaining variation at the individual level, the theory also indirectly accounted for macro-level socializing agents such as neighborhoods. Sutherland argued that individuals who reside within disorganized neighborhoods are more likely to engage in criminal behavior due to the socialization processes that are more common in such neighborhoods. In this way, differential socialization does not run counter to more macro-level theories of criminal behavior, rather, this theoretical perspective is more concerned with specifying the micro-level processes that occur in such neighborhoods or other contexts.

Sutherland (1947) presented differential association theory as nine interconnected principles, with each principle building on the previous one. First, the foundation of the entire perspective is that criminal behavior is learned. This principle effectively rules out any process

83 internalized within the offender such as biological influences or human nature (Brown et al.,

2013). The second and third principles specify that criminal behavior is learned through interaction with others, with intimate personal groups—such as family and friends—providing the most potent influence. The fourth principle specifies that learning the motives, drives, rationalizations, and attitudes that accompany crime are just as important as learning the actual techniques of criminal behavior. In this way, learning the motivations that underlie criminal behavior is just as important as learning how to physically engage in such behaviors. The fifth principle is concerned with the driving force of the drives and motives specified in the fourth principle. More specifically, drives and motives are the direct result of either favorable or unfavorable definitions of legal codes which are acquired during the socialization process specified in the second and third principles.

The sixth principle is typically referred to as the principle of differential association

(Akers & Sellers, 2013) and indicates that criminal or delinquent behavior is a result of an excess of definitions favorable toward such behaviors over definitions unfavorable to the violation of laws. In other words, when definitions favorable toward antisocial behavior outweigh definitions unfavorable toward antisocial behavior individuals will engage in delinquent or criminal behaviors. The seventh principle specifies that differential associations that occur earlier

(priority), last longer and take up more time (duration), occur more often (frequency), and involve others with whom the individual more closely identifies with (intensity) will be more influential (Akers & Jensen, 2006). The eighth and ninth principles indicate that the process which ultimately results in criminal behavior involves the same mechanisms that are involved in any other learning, and that the same general needs and values underlie both criminal and non- criminal behavior.

84

While Sutherland’s (1947) theory of differential association is largely regarded as a standalone theoretical perspective and was lauded as the most thorough and plausible explanation of criminal behavior of the time, some theorists argued that this perspective is highly vague, particularly in regard to the actual learning mechanisms that result in behavior. Burgess and Akers (1966) attempted to better specify such mechanisms in their “differential association- reinforcement” theory. Rather than offer a competing theoretical perspective, Burgess and Akers offered a thorough revision and modernization of differential association theory. More specifically, the reformulated theory attempted to reframe differential association within the learning principles of operant and respondent conditioning popular in behavioral psychology at the time. In light of various critiques and empirical findings, Akers (1973, 1977, 1985, 1998) modified the theory numerous times eventually resulting in what is now commonly referred to as social learning theory. Importantly, social learning theory is fully complementary with

Sutherland’s theory of differential association, but instead offers a “broader theory that retains all of the differential association process in Sutherland’s theory (albeit clarified and somewhat modified) and integrates it with differential reinforcement and other principles of behavioral acquisition, continuation, and cessation” (Akers & Sellers, 2013, pp. 80-81).

Social learning theory includes four major concepts which detail the learning process which eventually results in criminal or antisocial behavior: differential association; definitions; differential reinforcement; and imitation. Differential association refers to the process specified by Sutherland (1947), including the social interactions which develop definitions that are either favorable or unfavorable toward the commission of criminal behavior. In addition, the modalities of differential association (Akers & Sellers, 2013) which refer to the situations in which differential associations are most influential (i.e., priority, duration, frequency, and

85 intensity) are also retained. Definitions refer to internal attitudes and the meanings that are attached to specific behaviors. In this way, definitions comprise the internal orientations, rationalizations, justifications, and attitudes that indicate whether a given act is viewed as right or wrong (Akers & Jensen, 2006). General definitions include moral, religious, or other conventional values and norms that encourage prosocial behavior and discourage antisocial behaviors. Specific definitions are unique to the individual and define whether certain situations provide opportunities for antisocial behavior.

Differential reinforcement refers to the balance of anticipated or actual rewards and punishments that are the direct result of a given act or set of acts. When balancing the potential rewards and punishments that accompany a particular act, individuals consider the past, present, and even the anticipated future ramifications that accompany the act. Similar to the rational choice perspective, acts which are expected to result in the greatest reward and least amount of punishment are most likely to be carried out and persist over time (Akers & Sellers, 2013).

Rewards may be nonsocial (e.g., the physical effects of drugs), but social rewards are considered to be the most powerful. Finally, imitation refers to the process in which an individual will engage in a given act after witnessing similar behavior in others. Several factors play an important role in the imitation process including the characteristics of the model, the behavior observed, and the observed consequences (or lack thereof) of the act (Akers & Jensen, 2006).

Imitation plays a more important role during the initialization of a given act, but is expected to continue to have some influence on the maintenance of behavioral patterns.

Akers (1998; Akers & Sellers, 2013) also specifies the manner in which these different concepts culminate into the actual process of learning. Importantly, this process is highly complex and follows a distinct sequence but also features feedback effects which differentially

86 emphasize various concepts (Akers & Sellers, 2013). The initiation of criminal behavior is a direct result of the organization of learned definitions, imitation of deviant models, and the expected balance of reinforcement at that current moment (Akers & Jensen, 2006). The actual or perceived rewards and punishments which accompany the act directly influence whether the act will be committed again. In addition, all four of the concepts central to social learning theory influence both the initiation and the maintenance of antisocial behavior, however, it is expected that imitation will play less of a role in the latter process. As previously mentioned, this process features prominent feedback effects wherein specific environmental factors may influence definitions (or some other aspect of the process), which in turn, results in an overall modification of the other aspects of the process. In addition, Akers’ (1998) most recent modification to social learning theory also recognizes the potential role that both meso-level and macro-level factors play in the learning process. In this social structure and social learning (SSSL) model, Akers argues that structural factors have an indirect influence on behavior via the influence of such factors on the learning process. More specifically, structural factors influence differential associations, definitions, differential reinforcement, and imitation, which in turn, influence behavior.

3.2.2 The Empirical Status of Social Learning Theory

A significant portion of the literature has been devoted to empirically assessing the primary concepts and processes that are directly implicated in social learning theory. As previously mentioned, the popularity of social learning theory has consistently increased since its initial proposition (Burgess & Akers, 1966), with recent inquiries revealing that the popularity of the theory has increased (Cooper et al., 2010; Ellis & Walsh, 1999). Based its long history and the overall empirical interest in social learning theory within the field of criminology, it is not all

87 that surprising that an immense literature related to this particular theory has taken shape.

Fortunately, two recent and comprehensive reviews of the literature have been completed (Akers

& Jensen, 2006; Pratt et al., 2010). Rather than focus on the hundreds of independent studies that comprise this literature (e.g., the trees), the most pertinent and informative findings gleaned from both comprehensive reviews of the literature will be offered below (e.g., the forest).

The first comprehensive review was offered by Akers and Jensen (2006), and was aimed at providing an overall summary of the empirical literature bearing on social learning theory.

Importantly, Akers and Jensen provide a comprehensive literature review and extrapolate findings from the existing literature, but they do not quantitatively synthesize the findings from the literature with meta-analytic techniques. While Akers and Jensen (2006) caution that even the richest survey data and most advanced modeling strategies “cannot fully reproduce the behavioral process envisioned in social learning theory,” they also acknowledge that “[i]f the theory is correct, then empirical findings…derived from or consistent with the theory, that approximate or provide a snapshot of the underlying process, should be supported by the data when subjected to proper statistical analysis” (p. 43). Akers and Jensen (and later Akers and

Sellers, 2013) categorize findings from the extant literature into three distinct categories: (1) general findings from studies examining the association between social learning variables and delinquent or antisocial behavior; (2) research on social learning within a family context; (3) research on social learning within peer groups or other group context. Empirical findings from each of these categories will be outlined below.

Perhaps the largest literature stemming from social learning theory is comprised of studies examining associations between the conceptual variables that comprise the theory and various delinquent, criminal, and antisocial behaviors. While this line of research exhibits

88 significant levels of variability in regard to a number of pertinent characteristics including the operationalization of key theoretical concepts, the population from which the analytic sample was drawn, and the outcome or dependent variables examined, Akers and Jensen (2006) conclude that this extensive line of research provides overwhelming evidence in favor of social learning theory. More specifically, the authors cite numerous studies from the 1970s, 1980s,

1990s, and the early 2000s, which provide direct support for the concepts and underlying processes specified in the theory. This same pattern of results persisted even when controlling for competing theoretical concepts, with social learning variables typically exerting stronger and more consistent effects on the outcomes of interest relative to competing theoretical concepts

(Catalano et al., 1996; Jang, 2002; Kaplan, 1996; Thornberry et al., 1994; but see Pratt & Cullen,

2000). Importantly, the majority of these findings have been garnered from studies examining samples from the United States, but a limited number of studies have also revealed similar findings in international samples (Bruinsma, 1992; Junger-Tas, 1992; Lopez et al., 1989; Wang

& Jensen, 2003; Zhang & Messner, 1995). A significant number of studies have also examined the underlying processes specified by Akers (1998) by which delinquent and criminal behavior is learned (Akers & Lee, 1996; Andrews & Kandel, 1979; Elliott & Menard, 1996; Empey &

Stafford, 1991; Esbensen & Deschenes, 1998; Esbensen & Huizinga, 1993; Sellers & Winfree,

1990; Warr, 1993). The overall pattern of findings flowing from this line of research has provided support for the learning process specified in social learning theory.

Akers (1998) has previously argued that the family is a key socializing agent with which individuals are differentially associated, wherein families provide individuals with definitions either favorable or unfavorable toward antisocial behavior, provide differential reinforcement for or against such behaviors, and also act as models for imitation. Albeit indirectly, a limited line

89 of research has also provided support for the importance of families within the context of social learning theory. Previous studies have revealed that antisocial behavior concentrates within families (Fagan & Wexler, 1987; McCord, 1991) with parents modeling antisocial behaviors for their children to imitate. In addition, parent-child interactions have been previously shown to have a direct influence on both prosocial and antisocial behavior in children (Snyder &

Patterson, 1995; Wiesner et al., 2003). Finally, previous studies have also revealed that younger siblings are at an increased risk for delinquent behaviors when they have older delinquent siblings (Lauritsen, 1993; Rowe & Farrington, 1997; Rowe & Gulley, 1992). Taken together, the findings flowing from this line of literature suggest that learning processes occurring within a family context have a significant influence on subsequent behavioral outcomes.

The influence of peer associations is so closely tied to social learning theory that many have mistakenly classified the theory as only a theory of peer influence. While social learning theory openly recognizes the peer group socialization as a critically important group context within which the social learning process occurs, the theory is not limited to socialization among peers. A substantial number of studies have reported significant associations between the behavior of one’s peers and their own behavioral patterns (Huizinga et al., 1991; Loeber &

Dishion, 1987; Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986; Warr, 2002). The evidence is so overwhelming that Akers and Jensen (2006) observed that “[o]ther than one’s own prior deviant behavior, the best single predictor of the onset, continuance, or desistance of crime and delinquency is differential association with conforming and law-violating peers” (p. 51). A related line of research has revealed that the influence of gang membership, affiliation, and participation is also highly associated with antisocial behavior, even when taking into account peer behavior (Battin et al., 1998; Curry et al., 2002; Winfree et al., 1994). Taken together, the

90 social learning processes that occur within various social contexts, particularly in regard to peer groups, have a significant and consistent influence on subsequent behavioral patterns.

The second, and most recent, comprehensive review of the social learning literature was performed by Pratt and colleagues (2010) and consisted of a meta-analytic review of the literature. As previously discussed, meta-analyses are powerful quantitative tools which allow for the synthesis of a large number of research findings into a more digestible set of average effect sizes which make overall patterns more discernable. Pratt and colleagues limited their meta-analysis to empirical articles published after the initial proposal of social learning theory

(Burgess & Akers, 1966), studies which deliberately intended to test social learning theory as proposed by Akers (1998; Burgess & Akers, 1966), estimated the independent effect of at least one of the concepts included in the theory, and examined an outcome measure that represented some form of delinquency or criminal behavior. These inclusion criteria yielded 133 studies published between 1974 and 2003, with 246 statistical models resulting in a total of 704 effect sizes and representing 118,403 individual cases (Pratt et al., 2010, pp. 773-775). Importantly, studies were included if they examined social learning theory in isolation, in competition with competing theoretical perspectives, or within an integrated theoretical perspective. The analysis also took into account various sample characteristics including the sampling frame used (general vs. school sample) in addition to the demographic composition of the sample. Finally, information on a number of additional factors related to the model specification used in each study was also collected. These characteristics include whether variables from competing perspectives were included in the models, whether the study used a cross-sectional or longitudinal research design, and the dependent variable used in the study.

91

The meta-analysis performed by Pratt and colleagues (2010) yielded three main findings directly relevant to social learning theory. First, the explanatory power of social learning theory seems to be directly in line—if not greater than—with previous theoretical perspectives that have been subjected to meta-analytic review. More specifically, the mean effect size of both differential association (Mz = .225) and definitions (Mz = .218) seem to be directly in line with the average effect sizes garnered from Pratt and Cullen’s (2000) meta-analysis of the self-control literature, and significantly greater than the average effect sizes stemming from Pratt and colleagues’ (2006) meta-analytic review of deterrence theory. In addition, while some of the methodological and sample characteristics examined in the meta-analysis did significantly moderate these effect sizes somewhat, the overall pattern of results revealed that both differential association and definitions had consistent and moderate to large effects on the examined outcomes.

The second key finding garnered from the meta-analysis revealed that some aspects and concepts related to social learning theory have received more empirical attention than others.

More specifically, differential association and definitions have received the greatest amount of attention, while differential reinforcement and imitation have received the least amount of attention. Directly in line with this finding, the third key finding to emerge was that some components of social learning theory are more strongly associated with antisocial outcomes than others. Differential association and definitions exert the strongest average effect sizes, while differential reinforcement (Mz = .097) and imitation (Mz = .103) seem to exert the weakest average effect sizes. Importantly, differential reinforcement and imitation are the two theoretical concepts that Akers (1998) added to Sutherland’s (1947) original conceptualization of differential association. Despite these relatively weak average effect sizes, Pratt and colleagues

92 conclude that the overall pattern of results stemming from their analysis provided strong support in favor of social learning theory. This same overall conclusion was more recently echoed by

Akers and Sellers (2013) in their summary of the empirical literature pertaining to social learning theory when they noted that “[t]he relationships between social learning variables and delinquent, criminal, and deviant behavior found in the research are typically strong to moderate and there has been very little negative evidence reported in the literature” (p. 89).

3.3 Classic Strain Theory

As the fields of and criminology transitioned from early biological explanations of criminal and antisocial behavior in the early decades of the 20th century, theoretical perspectives focusing on macro-level influences, such as the neighborhood (Shaw &

McKay, 1942), became the leading explanations of criminal behavior. While these perspectives were highly influential in the development of the field of criminology—and remain influential at the current time—they fail to account for variability in behavioral response to macro-level influences. As previously mentioned, in even the most disorganized or criminogenic neighborhoods a significant portion of the residents within each neighborhood refrain from criminal behavior. For example, in his now classic ethnographic study of neighborhoods in

Philadelphia, Anderson (1999) noted that most of the families he observed could be classified as

“decent” families as opposed to “street” families. Failing to account for this differential response to macro-level socializing agents and processes is a significant shortcoming of many of these theoretical perspectives, and one that is commonly glossed over.

One of the earliest, and perhaps the most well-known, attempts to account for the variability in response to macro-level social conditions and experiences was offered by Robert

Merton (1938). Drawing from concepts originally proposed by Emile Durkheim (1951 [1857]),

93

Merton offered an explanation of criminal behavior that focused on the process that was originated by experienced social conditions which differentially impact individuals based on their current standing within the class structure. Various macro-level conditions, which Merton attempted to specify in subsequent modifications of his theory (1957, 1959, 1968), ultimately resulted in feelings of strain or pressures to engage in antisocial or criminal behaviors.

Individual-level responses to these feelings or pressures can vary between individuals, effectively resulting in measurable differences in resulting behavioral responses. The theoretical perspective offered by Merton quickly became extremely popular within the field of criminology, particularly during the 1960s (Lilly et al., 2011). The overall influence of Merton’s strain theory continues within the contemporary literature and is largely regarded as the point of departure for two modern theoretical perspectives in Agnew’s (1992) general strain theory (GST) and Messer and Rosenfeld’s (1994) institutional anomie theory (IAT). In short, there are few theoretical perspectives that have had a greater influence on the theoretical and empirical landscape of the field of criminology than Merton’s strain theory. Below, a summary of the primary assumptions and concepts that comprise classic strain theory and the current empirical status of the theory will be provided.

3.3.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Classic Strain Theory

Merton’s (1938, 1957) strain theory is rooted in assumptions and concepts first proposed by Durkheim (1951 [1857]) in his influential work “Suicide.” More specifically, Durkheim proposed that human beings possess unlimited appetites or desires which must be socially controlled in order for a society to function properly. Societies which do not properly socialize individuals to place reasonable limits on desires will suffer from higher rates of deviant and criminal behaviors due to the overall pressure to fulfill unlimited desires and the lack of

94 legitimate opportunities or means to fulfill such desires. The result of the disjunction between desires and the ability to achieve goals ultimately culminates into what Durkheim referred to as a state of normlessness or anomie. Anomie is present when societies fail to properly implement the proper social regulation of appetites through the emphasis of the use of legitimate means to achieve goals. Durkheim also recognized other social circumstances which may ultimately result in anomie, such as wars or political revolutions, but the disjunction between aspirations and realized goals is the most pertinent to criminological inquiry.

Merton (1938) applied the concept of anomie to the industrialized societies which were modern at the time, and the United States in particular. Merton recognized American culture places a significant amount of emphasis on economic success and achievement across all levels of society. In this way, the richest of the rich and the poorest of the poor are equally socialized to strive toward the “American Dream” of wealth and success. While individuals are primed to emphasize the overall goal of wealth and success, far less emphasis is placed on the approved social means necessary to achieve these goals (e.g., college education, hard work, etc.). In other words, American culture places an overwhelming amount of emphasis on the goals that people should strive toward, but little to no emphasis on the appropriate means that are required to achieve these goals. Merton argued that this particular situation is highly problematic since an integrated society would equally emphasize goals and socially-approved means. This problem becomes even more salient within members of the lower class who experience even more limited access to socially-acceptable means due to the current nature of the social structure. Despite limited access to legitimate means, the goal of wealth and success is equally salient at all levels of American society. This combination of an unlimited desire for wealth and success coupled with an overall lack of the desire or opportunity to utilize legitimate means results in a

95 suboptimal society. More specifically, Merton argued that societies which overemphasized goals at the expense of socially-acceptable means would experience higher rates of anomie, and in turn, higher levels of criminal and antisocial behavior.

Merton (1938) also recognized the important role that the disconnect between expectation and payoff in the development of criminal behavior. American culture emphasizes the already unlimited desires of humans to achieve wealth and success without providing adequate levels of opportunity to achieve such lofty goals. Merton argued that this disconnect, which is even more emphasized at lower social classes, ultimately resulted in an anomic society which produces feelings of strain or pressure to achieve socially universal goals through alternative means. Since

American society does not heavily emphasize the importance of utilizing legitimate means to achieve universal social goals, individuals are primed to explore alternative avenues to realize such goals. In this way, some individuals may resort to illegitimate means—including deviant or criminal behaviors—in an effort to effectively close the gap between expectations and payoff.

Directly in line with his theory, Merton argued that societies which equally emphasize means and goals, or societies which place less overall emphasis on the accumulation of wealth, are more integrated, do not experience anomie resulting in diminished levels of strain, and have overall lower levels of crime.

While a significant portion of Merton’s (1938) strain theory was devoted to specifying the structural processes which ultimately culminate into criminal behavior, he also specified the processes which result in individual-level variation in response to these structural influences (see

Baumer, 2007). More specifically, Merton (1968) proposed that individuals responded to and attempted to alleviate feelings of strain with one of five adaptations. The first , conformity, refers to continuing to believe in the goals of wealth and success while also

96 continuing to endorse the use of conventional means to achieve such goals. Merton argued that the default response to feelings of strain was to conform and to continually value both socially- accepted goals and legitimate means. While the majority of members within society are conformists, many others ultimately find the feelings of strain stemming from the disconnect between expectation and payoff to be overwhelming and attempt to alleviate their feelings of strain in other ways.

The second adaptation, innovation, is the most common criminal response and refers to individuals who continue to believe in the goals of wealth and success, but abandon conventional means in favor of unconventional means. Most offenders utilize this adaptation by substituting criminal behavior for conventional means in an effort to achieve highly valued goals. Third, ritualists simply adjust of the overall goal in an effort to fit the conventional means that are available. By scaling down their expectations, these individuals do not have to turn to illegitimate means and also alleviate feelings of strain. Fourth, retreatists, abandon the goal of economic success and the conventional means to achieve such goals. Merton hypothesized that individuals who utilize this adaptation typically suffer from alcoholism or drug addiction.

Finally, rebellion refers to a complete rejection of the current system in favor of an alternate one with newly defined means and goals. While Merton goes to great lengths to describe each particular adaptation to strain in an effort to explain variation in response to structural influences, no attention is given to the process which influences variability in the adaptation utilized to alleviate feelings of strain. This is a significant oversight, particularly due to the significant differences between each of the identified adaptations. Surprisingly, this is a criticism that has not been addressed in subsequent presentations of strain theory and has not been addressed within the extant literature.

97

3.3.2 The Empirical Status of Classic Strain Theory

Despite the resounding presence strain theory holds in the history of both sociology and criminology, the concepts and processes presented by Merton (1938, 1967) have resulted in a surprisingly limited line of research. This observation is particularly surprising after taking into account the dramatic influence of classic strain theory on the historical and contemporary theoretical landscape of the field of criminology. The roots of classic strain theory extend into

Cohen’s (1955) of “delinquent boys,” Cloward and Ohlin’s (1960) theory of opportunity, Agnew’s (1992) general strain theory (GST), and Messner and Rosenfeld’s (1994) institutional anomie theory (IAT). Needless to say, the underlying concepts and ideas proposed by Merton in his conceptualization of classic strain theory have remained relevant throughout the theoretical evolution of the field of criminology and remain today. In this way, the limited amount of empirical attention researchers have devoted to testing the key aspects of classic strain theory is surprising. Regardless, the limited literature that has been devoted to classic strain theory will be reviewed below.

In his now classic study, Hirschi (1969) analyzed a sample of junior and senior high school students in an early test of Merton’s strain theory. Based on the assumptions of classic strain theory, Hirschi expected that a disjunction in educational aspirations and expectations would result in higher levels of strain and, in turn, higher levels of delinquency. The results of the analyses provided three sets of findings directly relevant for classic strain theory. First, only a small proportion of the sample experienced a disjunction between aspirations and expectations, indicating that this issue may not be as salient as Merton initially indicated. Second, respondents who reported higher aspirations than expectations were no more likely to engage in delinquency than individuals who reported identical levels of aspirations and expectations indicating that the disjunction between aspirations and expectations—a central concept of the theory—did not 98 significantly predict delinquency. Third, only a limited number of respondents reported aspirations which exceeded their expectations—the situation that would most likely result in feelings of strain. Based on these findings, Hirschi concluded that classic strain theory does not significantly predict delinquency.

Another study performed by Farnworth and Leiber (1989) attempted to address one of the more notable limitations of Hirschi’s (1969) study by focusing on economic (as opposed to educational) aspirations and educational expectations. The results of the study revealed that a disjunction between economic aspirations and educational expectations was strongly associated with both serious and minor delinquency, providing direct support for classic strain theory.

However, this overall pattern of findings has not been supported in the overall literature examining Merton’s strain theory. Burton and Cullen (1992) reviewed approximately 50 studies published between 1963 and 1991 testing classic strain theory. The majority of these studies operationalized strain as a disjunction between aspirations and expectations, but also failed to find a significant association between strain (operationalized in this way) and delinquency. In this way, the majority of the extant literature—at least during the time period examined—failed to provide support for classic strain theory. However, Burton and Cullen recognize that an alternative operationalization of strain relates more directly to individual perceptions of blocked opportunities, a definition that more closely resembles Merton’s original conceptualization of strain (Kubrin, Stucky, & Krohn, 2009). While the majority of studies included in the review did not operationalize strain in this way, approximately 60 percent of the studies that did report significant associations between strain and delinquency operationalized strain as the presence of blocked opportunities.

99

Subsequent studies have also advocated for the operationalization of strain as relative deprivation, which refers to comparisons between one’s own standing and others (Burton &

Dunaway, 1994). Findings flowing from this limited line of research have revealed significant and positive associations between relative deprivation and various forms of criminal behavior.

In an effort to more directly examine the direct implications of various operationalizations of strain, Burton and colleagues (1994) utilized three distinct measurement strategies: perceptions of blocked opportunities; a disjunction between aspirations and expectations; and relative deprivation. The results of the study revealed significant bivariate associations between all three operationalizations of strain. However, subsequent analyses involved the inclusion of variables related to competing theoretical perspectives including social control and social learning theories into the multivariate equations. The results of this step in the analysis failed to identify any significant associations between any of the measures of strain and criminal behavior, indicating that competing theoretical perspectives are better suited for explaining variability in criminal behavior than classic strain theory.

A handful of subsequent studies have attempted to offer a more refined measurement strategy aimed at tapping strain as originally proposed by Merton (for example see Agnew,

Cullen, Burton, Evans, & Dunaway, 1996; Wright, Cullen, Agnew, & Brezina, 2001). In an effort to examine the current empirical status of macro-level theories and predictors of criminal behavior, Pratt and Cullen (2005) performed a meta-analysis of over 214 articles containing over

500 statistical models and nearly 2,000 effect size estimates. The authors identified a total of seven macro-level theoretical perspectives, one of which was classic strain theory (referred to as anomie/strain theory), which have been subjected to empirical investigation in the extant literature. Despite previous emphasis on both the macro- and individual-level aspects of

100

Merton’s strain theory (Messner, 1988; see also Baumer, 2007), Pratt and Cullen note that “[f]ew direct tests of anomie/strain theory, however, exist at the macro level” (p. 392). While the authors conclude that the results of the meta-analysis provide moderate support for classic strain theory, they also acknowledge that “anomie/strain theory has not been adequately tested to confirm its empirical status” (p. 410).

Taken as a whole, the current empirical status of classic strain theory is surprisingly underdeveloped and mixed at best. Studies have yet to attempt to test both the individual and structural aspects of the theory within a single study, despite Baumer’s (2007) comprehensive proposal of classic strain theory as a multilevel theory. In addition, with only one exception

(Menard, 1995), no studies consider the potential role that adaptations specified by Merton play in the development of criminal behavior. Adaptations to strain occupy a critically important feature of classic strain theory, making it quite surprising that the vast majority of studies examining the perspective have completely overlooked this particular facet of the theory. Also related to the adaptation to strain, Merton failed to specify the mechanisms and/or processes which account for variability in adaptation selection. In other words, the processes that ultimately result in conformity over innovation are not specified or even alluded to. Subsequent research has yet to offer any additional insight in this regard either.

3.4 Social Bonding Theory

Perhaps the most influential, in terms of both empirical and theoretical attention, theoretical perspective within the field of criminology is the social control perspective. While competing assumptions may underlie the two perspectives, both social control theories and strain theories stem from Durkheim’s (1951 [1857]) argument that human beings possess unlimited appetites or desires. Strain theories—such as Merton’s (1938) classic strain theory—build upon

101 this argument by cautioning against the potentially anomic consequences which accompany over-emphasizing goals over conventional means within modern industrialized societies. Social control theories emphasize the importance of social controls aimed at limiting individual’s appetites through socialization practices all in the better interest of both the social as a whole and the individuals who comprise the social. These overall goals stem directly from Durkheim’s

(1951 [1857]) original propositions which indicate that more integrated societies require some form of control in an effort to prevent the development of anomie. While social control and strain theories interpret Durkheim’s observations quite differently and concentrate such observations into vastly different theoretical perspectives, both sets of theories stem from

Durkheim’s assumptions regarding human nature.

Based on these underlying assumptions regarding human nature, social control theories operate under the assumption that a loosening of social controls will result in increases in criminal and antisocial behavior. In other words, human beings are motivated to offend and will do so unless the proper social controls are implemented and enforced. This particular assumption runs directly counter to many of the other mainstream theoretical perspectives within the field of criminology including social learning theories and strain theories (Brown et al.,

2013). In conjunction with these assumptions, some of the main proponents of the social control perspective have advocated for a fundamental shift in the way criminologists examine offenders.

More specifically, rather than attempting to understand the underlying mechanisms which ultimately result in criminal and delinquent behavior, advocates of the social control perspective argue that understanding the mechanisms and processes that prevent criminal and delinquent behaviors is far more pertinent. For example, Hirschi (1969) noted that “[t]he question ‘Why do they do it?’ is simply not the question the theory is designed to answer. The question is ‘Why

102 don’t we do it?’” (p. 34). Since all humans are believed to be motivated to engage in criminal behavior under the proper social conditions, Hirschi—and his predecessors—argue that attempting to specify the processes that underlie such behaviors is a meaningless endeavor.

Rather, attempting to understand why some individuals actually refrain from criminal or delinquent behaviors is a far more interesting question.

Perhaps the most influential and well-known was proposed by

Travis Hirschi (1969) in his now classic and noteworthy book “Causes of Delinquency.” While several notable control theories were proposed during the 1950s and 1960s (Matza, 1964; Nye,

1958; Reckless, 1961; Reiss, 1951; Sykes & Matza, 1957; Toby, 1957), Hirschi’s social bonding theory quickly became the most popular and, arguably, the most impactful social control theory.

The overwhelming popularity of social bonding theory is likely the direct result of two contributing factors. First, Hirschi proposed a concise, parsimonious, and coherent theory which incorporated elements from previous control theories. In this way, social bonding theory was a digestible and easily comprehended theory which summarized and built upon the theoretical foundation set by earlier theories. Second, Hirschi not only specified the assumptions and concepts of social bonding theory, he also offered an empirical operationalization for each concept. In addition, he also performed an empirical test of social bonding theory using a sample of adolescents from California. The manner in which Hirschi initially proposed social bonding theory not only made a convincing argument in favor of the theory, but also set the standard for criminological research in the subsequent decades (Akers & Sellers, 2013). Below, a discussion of social bonding theory will be offered in addition to a summary of the overall findings from the empirical literature focused on testing the theory.

103

3.4.1 Key Assumptions and Theoretical Concepts of Social Bonding Theory

As previously mentioned, some of the more attractive aspects of social bonding theory are the clarity with which the theory is proposed and the underlying simplicity of the concepts and processes included. Social bonding theory proposes that individuals who refrain from criminal and delinquent behaviors are more strongly bonded to society relative to individuals who give into their natural tendency to offend. When social bonds are weakened or broken, individuals are significantly more likely to engage in delinquent or criminal behaviors. Hirschi

(1969) further specified that individuals are not bonded to some larger latent entity known collectively as the social, but rather, individuals are bonded to specific socializing agents within the social such as peers, parents, teachers, and family members. The greater the extent to which social bonds between the individual other socializing agents are weakened, the greater the likelihood that the individual will engage in criminal or antisocial behavior. Hirschi proposed that the social bond was comprised of four separate, yet related, elements: attachment; commitment; involvement; and belief. While each of the four elements of the social bond are viewed separately, they are also dependent upon one another wherein the weakening of one element of the social bond will likely result in the weakening of other elements.

The first element of the social bond, attachment, refers to the affection that characterizes a relationship between people and is viewed as the most important social bond. Higher levels of attachment are inversely related to delinquency since this element of the social bond takes into account the admiration and identification with others who hold specific behavioral expectations and opinions. Lower levels of attachment would constitute a greater level of insensitivity regarding other’s opinions, which in turn decreases the constraints on behaviors that are viewed as unconventional or delinquent (Akers & Sellers, 2013). Hirschi (1969) also argued that concepts previously implicated in earlier control theories—such as self-control, internal control, 104 and indirect control—are actually encompassed under the concept of attachment preventing the need for further conceptualization. While Hirschi emphasized parental attachment in his proposition of social bonding theory, he also acknowledges the importance of attachment with other important socializing agents including peers. Along the same lines, Hirschi argued that the specific socializing agents to whom one is attached to does not really matter. The most important aspect is that the relationship is characterized by a strong level of attachment, which ultimately contributes to the overall social bond and diminishes the likelihood of offending.

Subsequently, strong levels of attachment to delinquent peers or even antisocial parents are expected to result in decreases in the likelihood of offending since individuals are attached to the person not necessarily their character (Akers & Sellers, 2013).

The second element of the social bond is commitment and is directly related to Toby’s

(1957) stakes in conformity. More specifically, commitment refers to the investment in conventionality that is potentially at risk by engaging in criminal or delinquent behaviors. The more committed an individual is to conventionality, the more they have to lose by engaging in delinquent behaviors. For example, individuals may lose their job, damage their relationship with a significant other, or diminish their overall social standing by engaging in criminal or delinquent behaviors. The potential risk of losing previously acquired social investments is much greater for individuals with greater levels of such investments, resulting in greater levels of commitment to conformity. Alternatively, individuals with lower overall levels of social investment have far less to lose and, in turn, are far less committed to conformity. Importantly, this particular aspect of social bonding theory implicates the principles of the rational choice perspective in that human beings are largely rational and will be more likely to refrain from

105 criminal activity when the potential costs far outweigh the potential benefits (Akers & Sellers,

2013).

Involvement is the third element of the social bond, and simply refers to being too occupied with conventional activities to engage in unconventional activities. For example, conventional activities such as spending time with family, studying, and participation in community activities may comprise the majority of one’s attention, leaving little time for delinquent or criminal behavior. The fourth and final element of the social bond is belief, which

Hirschi (1969) described as the endorsement of conventional rules, values, and beliefs, with the most important being the belief that laws and social rules represent moral standards and should be obeyed. Importantly, individuals do not have to necessarily agree with each and every law or rule implemented within a society, but rather, they should generally accept the underlying belief that laws and rules in general are meaningful and valid. In this way, individuals who no longer hold such beliefs are expected to be more loosely bonded to society and are more likely to engage in criminal activity.

3.4.2 The Empirical Status of Social Bonding Theory

Since its formal introduction in 1969, Hirschi’s social bonding theory “has been one of the most, if not the most, tested theories in criminology” (Lilly et al., 2011, p. 119; emphasis in original). There are likely two specific factors accounting for the overwhelming amount of empirical attention that social bonding theory has received. First, Hirschi deliberately proposed a theory that could be easily tested with secondary data by proposing concepts and processes that were clearly specified and explicit. Hirschi (1969) recognized the overly complex nature of many of the existing theoretical perspectives and the problems that accompany attempting to empirically test such complex theories. Based on these assessments, Hirschi sought to propose a

106 concise and parsimonious theory that was amenable to empirical assessment. Second, Hirschi offered a direct operationalization of the central concepts in social bonding theory in his initial proposal of the theory. This particular practice offered researchers a unique opportunity to test various aspects of social bonding theory using the proper operationalization of the primary theoretical concepts as intended by Hirschi. Taken together, these aspects of social bonding theory, and the manner in which it was proposed, have culminated into an impressive body of research.

The majority of the studies examining social bonding theory have attempted to directly assess the predictive ability of each of the four elements of the social bond (i.e., attachment, commitment, involvement, and belief). Several early tests of social bonding theory attempted to create a general measure of the social bond comprised of individual items tapping each of the four elements of the bond (see Krohn & Massey, 1980). The overall pattern of findings stemming from this line of research indicates that the social bond is significantly and negatively associated with delinquency, but the effect sizes were largest when predicting minor forms of delinquency. Subsequent studies have advocated for the examination of each individual element of the social bond in an effort to provide a more thorough understanding of the mechanisms which underlie the association between the social bond and subsequent criminal or delinquent behavior (see Wiatrowski, Griswold, & Roberts, 1981). Studies have found significant associations between various combinations of the elements of the social bond and a wide range of antisocial and criminal outcomes including white collar crime (Lasley, 1988), marijuana use in adolescence (Akers & Cochran, 1985), and other forms of delinquency (Rankin & Kern,

1994).

107

In a systematic review of the literature, Kempf (1993) analyzed a total of 71 studies conducted between 1970 and 1991 examining social bonding theory. Unfortunately, the review did not yield an easily interpreted pattern of results. While Kempf concluded that the overall findings from the analysis “professed at least conditional support for control theory” (1993, p.

167), much of the literature suffered from significant methodological limitations making it difficult to assess the overall pattern of findings more directly. The results revealed a diverse set of measurement strategies aimed at tapping the four elements of the social bond as specified by

Hirschi (1969). This variability in the operationalization of the social bond makes it difficult to generalize findings across studies, particularly since some studies offer much stronger measures while other studies offer much weaker measures. In addition, the majority of studies failed to include measures that tapped all four elements of the bond making it difficult to draw conclusions regarding the predictive ability of the theory as a whole. Studies conducted after

Kempf’s review have provided additional support for social bonding theory (Costello & Vowell,

1999; Rankin & Kern, 1994; Wright, Caspi, Moffitt, & Silva, 1999). In addition, a recent meta- analysis revealed a significant and negative association between parental attachment and delinquency, with an overall mean effect size of r = .18 (Hoeve et al., 2012).

The vast majority of the early studies examining various aspects of social bonding theory were cross-sectional, only examining associations between measures of the elements of the social bond and antisocial outcomes at the same time. Some critics argued that longitudinal studies of social bonding theory were necessary to effectively establish causal order (see Brown et al., 2012). Agnew (1985) was one of the first criminologists to test social bonding theory within the confines of a longitudinal study. Prior to performing the longitudinal analysis, Agnew examined cross-sectional association between eight items used to tap the social bond and

108 delinquency. The results revealed significant and moderately sized effects on the delinquency measures examined. Agnew then examined the potential effect of the social bond measures on the change in delinquency between two time points. The results of the longitudinal models yielded dramatically different results with only a limited number of significant associations between the included social bond measures and delinquency. In addition, the overall effect sizes had dropped significantly wherein the included social control measures only explained between 1 and 2 percent of the variance in changes in delinquency between the two time points.

Subsequent longitudinal analyses have yielded similar results suggesting that the association between the social bond and subsequent delinquency may not be causal (Agnew, 1991; Burkett

& Warren, 1987; Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985; Massey & Krohn, 1986; McCarthy & Hoge,

1984; Paternoster & Iovanni, 1986). Based on the pattern of findings stemming from his analysis, Agnew (1985) concluded that “cross-sectional studies have greatly exaggerated the importance of Hirschi’s social control theory” (p. 58).

A complementary line of research has also attempted to better specify the mechanisms which ultimately result in each of the four elements of the bond. Arguably, the potential role of peers in the development and maintenance of social bonds has received the most attention within the extant literature (see Kubrin et al., 2009 for a more comprehensive overview). Recall that

Hirschi argued that stronger social bonds should be inversely related to delinquency. In this way, individuals who are strongly bonded to peers should be less likely to engage in criminal or delinquent behavior, even if their peers are themselves delinquent. This overall pattern has not been borne out in the literature. Rather, a substantial line of research has consistently revealed the exact opposite pattern of findings: individuals who are more strongly attached to delinquent peers have been found to be significantly more likely to engage in delinquent and criminal

109 behavior (Krohn, Thornberry, Rivera, & LeBlanc, 2001; Warr, 2002). In addition, after accounting for peer influence, studies tend to report attenuated or nonsignificant associations between social bond measures and delinquency (Agnew, 1991; Marcos, Bahr, & Johnson, 1986;

Massey & Krohn, 1986). Taken together, this line of research indicates that Hirschi’s (1969) original conceptualization of social bonding theory is, at the very least, incomplete and requires modification in order to effectively explain variation in delinquent and criminal behavior.

Directly related to studies examining the potential role of peers within social bonding theory, several studies have attempted to identify the specific sources to which one becomes bonded. Several studies have implicated factors directly related to parenting such as being attached to both parents (Rankin & Kern, 1994) and parental supervision (Simons, Simons, Burt,

Brody, & Cutrona, 2005). Additional studies have also found that higher levels of school attachment and commitment result in lower levels of antisocial behavior (Cernkovich &

Giordano, 1992; Stewart, 2003). Akers and Sellers (2013) make the argument that “[a]dherence to religious practices clearly indicates commitment to conventionality, involvement in conventional activities, and attachment to others” (p. 121). A significant number of studies have identified a negative association between attachment to religious beliefs and delinquency, providing evidence in favor of religion as a potential source of attachment (Cochran & Akers,

1989; Evans, Cullen, Dunaway, & Burton, 1995; Welch, Tittle, & Grasmick, 2006). In addition, the results of a meta-analysis conducted by Baier and Wright (2001) revealed moderate and negative associations between religiosity and delinquency. A series of more recent reviews of nearly 300 studies revealed similar findings (Johnson, De Li, Larson, & McCullough, 2000;

Johnson & Jang, 2010; Johnson, 2011).

110

3.5 Summary and Discussion

The four theoretical perspectives presented in this chapter constitute some of the most influential and well-known explanations of criminal and delinquent behavior within the field of criminology. With that said, each of these perspectives have encouraged the development of a vast and impressive literature aimed at empirically analyzing the predictive ability of each theory or attempting to further specify aspects of each perspective. Each individual perspective has been subjected to a remarkable number of empirical tests with some studies finding support for each perspective and other studies failing to find support. Throughout all of the iterations of statistical models, the swapping of independent and dependent variables, the innovation in measurement strategies, and the analysis of new and innovative samples, the overall findings stemming from the literature are decidedly mixed. While some of the reviewed perspectives have received much more empirical support (i.e., social learning theory) than others (i.e., rational choice and classic strain theories), all of the perspectives have received at least some support and remain relevant within the field of criminology currently. In addition, each of the presented theoretical perspectives remain staples in the most popular criminological theory textbooks and are taught in virtually every criminology course to date.

This overall pattern of findings raises two important questions regarding the current landscape of the field of criminology. First, what is the primary source of the overall lack of consensus regarding each of the reviewed theories? In other words, what factors account for the lack of consensus in findings stemming from the voluminous literature surrounding each perspective? Despite the impressive number of studies examining each of these perspectives, subsequent theoretical modifications, and an overall methodological sophistication, the overall explanatory power of each perspective has not significantly increased over the past few decades

(Weisburd & Piquero, 2008). The overall stagnation in theoretical progress can only be 111 contributed to one of two sources: (1) our methodologies or (2) the theories themselves. Second, what potential role does the fixation on purely social explanations of antisocial behavior play in the mixture of findings present in the extant literature? Despite the presence of a directly relevant, methodological advanced, and highly developed literature (reviewed in Chapter 2) directly implicating the role of biological factors in the development of antisocial behaviors, criminologists have yet to draw from this line of research. Whether this oversight is directly related to the lack of consensus among findings remains an open question at this time. However, it remains fully possible that this overall preference for purely social explanations of antisocial behavior is directly tied to the mixed empirical findings and the overall limited explanatory power of the leading theoretical perspectives within the field of criminology.

Observing empirical evidence both in favor of and opposing existing conventional criminological theories is not unique to the overview and discussion presented in this project.

While previous researchers have also identified this overall pattern of findings, there is lack of consensus regarding the subsequent actions that should be taken (Bernard & Snipes, 1996; Elliot et al., 1985; Hirschi, 1979). This particular issue remains unresolved currently; however, two competing perspectives have been offered in the extant literature. First, Hirschi (1979) advocated for the falsification and rejection of theories that do not perform well. Put simply, theories that do not adequately explain variation in criminal and delinquent behaviors are of limited utility and should be swept into the dustbin of criminology. Hirschi argued that theoretical integration is largely unnecessary and, perhaps more importantly, not possible since different theoretical perspectives offer competing incompatible assumptions about human nature.

On the other side of the debate, Benard and Snipes (1996; see also Elliott et al., 1985) argued that theoretical integration is a far more effective and feasible approach than falsification. Since

112 all theories explain at least some variance in criminal behavior some of the time, falsification, at least in the most literal sense, becomes largely impossible. In addition, Bernard and Snipes observed that focusing on concepts, and the relationships between concepts, that significantly explain variation in criminal behavior is a much more effective approach than pitting one theory against another. In this way, concepts do not belong to specific theories and can be combined in an effort to explain more variance in criminal behavior.

Theoretical integration has a storied past within criminology (see Bernard & Snipes,

1996), but much of the attention attributed to integration has focused only on purely social concepts specified in theories which were created under the untenable assumption that social influences on behavior work in isolation. While a few notable exceptions exist (Ellis, 2005;

Fishbein, 1990; Robinson & Beaver, 2009; Walsh, 2000, 2002), integration involving concepts and empirical findings stemming from the biosocial perspective is a task that has yet to be attempted. In addition, the few examples that do currently exist either suffer from taking far too broad of an approach (Ellis, 2005; Robinson & Beaver, 2009), or from attempting to integrate concepts that are potentially incompatible with existing theoretical concepts (Fishbein, 1990;

Walsh, 2000). In either case, additional attention regarding the potential integration of existing theories, or at the very least the concepts specified within such theories, into the biosocial perspective is necessary since this type of integration can potentially boost the explanatory power of theories aimed at explaining antisocial behaviors. The next chapter will provide a brief overview of the biosocial integration models that have been offered in previous studies and then offer a new and unique integration strategy.

113

CHAPTER 4

SITUATING EXISTING CRIMINOLOGY THEORIES WITHIN THE BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE

As more biologically informed research appears, the theoretical predictions of the sociological paradigm will be revealed as limited, if not misspecified in important ways. Eventually, it will become commonplace to ask: How can any theory that ignores the human body be complete?

-Francis T. Cullen (2009, p. xvi)

The abundance of criminological theories that have been proposed over the preceding decades is quite staggering and directly contributes to a significant problem within the field of criminology—the proper way to handle to overwhelming number of theories that have and continue to be introduced. The problem is further compounded by the large number of studies, producing conflicting results, which examine each individual theory. Criminologists have observed that the overwhelming number of theories within the field of criminology impedes scientific progress by flooding the literature with research attempting to better specify concepts within a perspective which, even taken as a whole, only explains a minimal amount of variation in criminal behavior (Bernard & Snipes, 1996).

While there seems to be a consensus among criminologists that there are simply too many theories within criminology, and this oversaturation is harmful to empirical progress within the field, there is far less consensus regarding the proper way to address this problem. This particular issue ultimately resulted in a now classic debate within the literature with some criminologists advocating for the falsification of theories (Hirschi, 1979, 1989) and others calling for theoretical integration in an effort to reduce the overall number of theories (Bernard &

114

Snipes, 1996; Elliott et al., 1985; Liska et al., 1989; Thornberry, 1989). Travis Hirshi (1979,

1989) was the most adamant proponent of the falsification process in which one theory should be pitted against another and the one that explains more variance within the outcome of interest should be retained and the other should be discarded. Hirschi argued that this is the only possible way to decrease the overall number of theories within criminology since theories tend to be contradictory with one another, not just in regard to the concepts they emphasize but also in regard to the processes they specify. Proponents of theoretical integration, most notably Elliott and colleagues (1985), argued that theoretical perspectives within criminology are not contradictory in nature and can be integrated in an effort to emphasize those concepts and processes which best explain variation in criminal behavior. In this way, the concepts and variables specified within theories should be independently evaluated and the concepts which explain the greatest proportion of variance in criminal behavior should be retained and integrated into a separate theory.

“Theoretical integration” is a fairly broad term that may encompass any of a number of different processes aimed at combining various aspects of multiple theories into a single, larger theory with greater explanatory power. Importantly, Liska and colleagues (1989) specified various forms of theoretical integration directly relevant to criminological theory. Two primary forms of theoretical integration are possible: propositional and conceptual integration.

Propositional integration involves linking two or more separate theories, but retaining the concepts and processes contained within each individual theory. Theories are ultimately combined into a single larger theory using one of three different strategies based on the individual characteristics of the theories. First, end-to-end integration is developmental in the processes and associations specified in one theory precede those that occur within the other. In

115 this way, this form of propositional integration attempts to impose a causal order among theoretical processes by utilizing the dependent variable specified in one theory as the independent variable in the next. Second, side-by-side integration focuses on integrating theories which explain variation within related outcomes. For example, theories aimed at explaining various forms of delinquency in adolescence, may also explain minor forms of criminal behavior in adulthood. Third, up-and-down integration focuses on conceptually broadening one theory such that it encompasses other perspectives and the concepts contained within them. This form of integration is relatively rare since most theories cannot be broadened conceptually.

Conceptual integration is less concerned with retaining the processes and concepts specified within each theory and more focused on identifying conceptual overlap between separate theories. In this way, conceptual integration focuses on integrating theories which contain similar concepts and attempt to explain similar behavioral outcomes. Liska et al. (1989) caution that this form of theoretical integration may have limited utility and may simply result in a modification of the underlying meanings of previously specified theoretical concepts.

However, conceptual integration is an important first step toward the ultimate goal of propositional integration. An important prerequisite for any form of propositional integration is the identification of overall patterns present within theories and their underlying concepts. Once these patterns have been identified, it is only then possible to integrate individual theories into a more unified and more powerful theory using one of the integration strategies specified above.

Another form of theoretical integration was initially proposed by Thornberry (1989) and appears to be an alternative to more formal forms of theoretical integration. More specifically,

Thornberry advocated for theory elaboration which involves the logical extension of a single theory in an attempt to enhance its overall predictive ability. Theory elaboration essentially

116 specifies the process by which theorists perform various theoretical and conceptual modifications directly in line with advances within the extant literature. The process specified by Thornberry very closely resembles the process which is carried out within the field of criminology currently:

(1) a theoretical perspective is formally presented; (2) limitations of the theory are noted; (3) various empirical tests are performed in an effort to quantify the validity of the concepts and processes included in the theory; (4) specific theoretical and conceptual modifications are offered based on the results of the empirical tests carried out in the previous step; (5) the proposed modifications are either carried out or a rebuttal against each modification is offered; (6) the modified theory is once again subjected to empirical tests and further modifications are offered.

In this way, the overall advantage of theoretical elaboration as specified by Thornberry is not clearly specified. In addition, as observed by Bernard and Snipes (1996), “[r]egardless of one’s orientation, elaboration logically leads to integration” (p. 309).

Despite numerous calls for theoretical integration into the biosocial perspective—or more biologically-informed theories—very few criminologists have attempted to undertake this particular task (Fishbein, 1990). The few attempts to more closely integrate the biosocial perspective and existing criminological theory can be divided into two separate categories. First, some criminologists have attempted to present biosocial theories of antisocial behavior (Ellis,

2005; Robinson & Beaver, 2009; Vila, 1994). Such theories attempt to combine concepts and empirical findings from multiple academic fields including criminology, biology, evolutionary psychology, psychiatry, neuroscience, and molecular genetics. Second, two formal attempts have been made to integrate biological and traditional criminological concepts (Walsh, 2000,

2002). The primary goal of these integrative efforts was to properly situate biological concepts within or alongside existing criminological theories and concepts. All of these previous attempts

117 represent a step in the correct direction, but they also suffer from several important limitations.

This chapter will provide an overview of previous biosocial integration strategies in an effort to specify the contributions made by each previous attempt, but also to highlight the limitations of each previous attempt. Following this discussion, an innovative biosocial integration model focused on integrating existing criminological theory within the biosocial perspective will be presented.

4.1 Precursors to the Biosocial Integration Model

Based largely on the empirical findings presented in Chapter 2, biosocial criminologists have criticized the general practice of ignoring the potential importance of considering the role of biological and genetic contributions to antisocial behavior (for an example see Wright & Cullen,

2012). Meanwhile, conventional criminologists have leveed a series of criticisms against biosocial research including the dangers of determinism, a lack of plausible policy implications, and generalizability issues stemming from analyzing samples of twin and sibling pairs (Akers &

Sellers, 2013; Kubrin et al., 2009; Lilly et al., 2011). While these specific concerns have been directly addressed by a large number of researchers working across a wide range of academic disciplines (Beaver, 2013a; Pinker, 2002; Ridley, 1999; Rowe, 1994; Wright, 1994), conventional criminologists continue to advance new critiques of the biosocial perspective.

Perhaps the most prevalent and consistent of these criticisms is the overall lack of biosocial theories of criminal behavior. For example, Kubrin and colleagues (2009) argue that

“heritability and genetic anomaly studies can only establish whether biology plays a role in determining behavior. They cannot explain how biology influences criminal behavior”

(emphasis in original; p. 50). While this statement is highly debatable, the recognition of an overall lack of biosocial theories of antisocial behavior is a valid one.

118

The overall lack of theoretical development within the biosocial perspective has been noted for quite some time and is widely acknowledged by both conventional and biosocial criminologists (Agnew, 2011; Ellis, 2005; Fishbein, 1990; Robinson & Beaver, 2009; Vila,

1994; Walsh, 2000, 2002). Efforts to address this particular criticism have only been attempted by a relatively small number of biosocial criminologists and can be divided into two separate categories. First, some biosocial criminologists have attempted to develop biosocial theories of criminal and antisocial behavior which take into account all of the known biological and environmental influences and risk factors which have been found to be associated with antisocial and maladaptive behavioral patterns (Ellis, 2005; Robinson & Beaver, 2009). Second, other biosocial criminologists have taken a completely different approach and have advocated for various forms of theoretical integration, typically involving existing conventional criminological theories (Fishbein, 1990; Walsh, 2000, 2002; Vila, 1994). Each of these two approaches will be detailed below.

4.1.1 Biosocial Theories of Antisocial Behavior

Many mainstream criminology textbooks refer to “biosocial theories” of criminal and antisocial behavior (for examples see Akers & Sellers, 2013; Bernard, Snipes, & Gerould, 2010;

Kubrin et al., 2009). Kubrin and colleagues (2009) even go as far as to proclaim that “[a] wide variety of biosocial theories that suggest specific ways that biology influences criminality have been developed” (p. 50). The “biosocial theories” discussed extensively in such texts are not formally-presented and empirically testable explanations of antisocial behavior, rather, the majority of these “theories” are simply empirical findings stemming from various lines of research. More specifically, the findings mistaken for theories are the primary tenets or concepts that are organized by the biosocial perspective.

119

To date, only two biosocial theories of antisocial and criminal behavior have been proposed—evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory (ENA; Ellis, 2005) and Integrated Systems

Theory (IST; Robinson & Beaver, 2009). While both theories are distinct from one another, they closely resemble one another and emphasize many of the same concepts and processes. Broadly, both theories attempt to integrate empirical findings from both conventional criminology and the biosocial perspective in an effort to propose a theory with maximum explanatory power. In addition, both theories attempt to integrate concepts from conventional theoretical perspectives.

For example, IST directly integrates concepts from several theories including labeling, strain, social learning, and social disorganization theories (see pp. 374-380). Both theories also emphasize findings from several academic disciplines which more directly align with the biosocial perspective such as evolutionary psychology, molecular genetics, psychiatry, developmental psychology, and neuroscience. While IST more directly integrates findings from neuroscience research, ENA more directly emphasizes findings from evolutionary psychology.

For example, the first main proposition of ENA which states “that aggressive and acquisitive criminal behaviour evolved as an aspect human reproduction, especially among males” (Ellis,

2005, p. 288).

Both theories attempt to conceptually integrate empirical findings, concepts, and entire theoretical perspectives into a singular biosocial theory of human behavior. Figure 4.1 offers a visual representation of the type of integration that both theories attempt to perform. The circle represents the entire biosocial perspective which encompasses several other perspectives and disciplines including behavior genetics, molecular genetics, and evolutionary psychology (see

Barnes, 2014 for a more detailed discussion). The biosocial perspective also encompasses all of the empirical studies which directly implicate both biological and environmental influences in

120 the etiological processes which ultimately result in antisocial behaviors. The rectangles within the circle represent all of the concepts and empirical findings stemming not only from biosocial research, but also from conventional criminological studies since both theories attempt to integrate theories and findings from both sets of literature. On its face, this form of theoretical integration seems to be quite successful in that all of the theoretical concepts specified in both conventional criminological and biosocial research are neatly compacted within the biosocial perspective. However, the processes which underlie this particular form of theoretical integration are highly complex and difficult, if not wholly impossible, to test empirically. Both theories describe a highly interconnected and complex relationship not just between the theoretical concepts and antisocial behavior, but also between the concepts themselves. Such relationships involve multiple interactions between various concepts in addition to potential two- way causal relationships between two or more concepts. Robinson and Beaver also note the importance of taking into account individual-level variation in exposure to various risk factors based on the frequency, regularity, intensity, and priority of exposure (p. 380).

While both attempts should be lauded for their ambition, novelty, and underlying creativity, it is highly unlikely that either theory can be assessed empirically and breaking each theory into smaller, more digestible pieces would simply result in rehashing the previous research on which each theoretical concept is built. In this way, both theories tend to be examples of Thornberry’s (1989) alternative integration approach of theoretical elaboration

“taken to its logical extreme” (Bernard & Snipes, 1996). Put more simply, the theories are so broad and attempt to take into account so many concepts from such a diverse set of sources that the overall conclusion is that “everything causes everything,” which is not all that useful when attempting to specify the underlying mechanisms which ultimately explain variation in antisocial

121 behavior. Based on these limitations, it is not surprising that neither of these biosocial theories have been subjected to empirical tests.

Directly in line with the comprehensive biosocial theories proposed by Ellis (2005) and

Robinson and Beaver (2009) is Vila’s (1994) general paradigm for understanding criminal behavior which is described as an extension of Cohen and Machalek’s (1988) theory of . Vila does not describe his approach as a theory, but rather as a paradigm in which new, interdisciplinary, and more powerful theories can be developed. The general paradigm is grounded in three levels of measurement: ecological; macro-level; and micro-level.

Vila argues that any useful theory must be ecological, integrate concepts from various perspectives, and developmental. Biological factors are argued to play a significant role at the micro-level, but Vila also stresses the importance of examining interactions between factors stemming from each of the three levels of measurement. While Vila’s general paradigm does not represent a theory in the formal sense, it is directly related to the comprehensive biosocial theories described in this section. Since Vila does not propose a formal theory, there are no concepts or processes which can be subjected to an empirical test. Rather, theories developed within the paradigm can be tested.

4.1.2 Preliminary Models of Biosocial Integration

One of the main driving forces behind the movement toward biosocial theories of criminal and antisocial behavior is the limited explanatory power of conventional theoretical perspectives (Weisburd & Piquero, 2008). This observation is one that has been made by both biosocial and conventional criminologists and has been one of the primary factors motivating the call for theoretical integration (see Bernard & Snipes, 1996 for an overview). To date, Bernard and Snipes (1996; see also Bernard et al., 2010) have proposed the most comprehensive and

122 versatile integration strategy. This particular strategy involves five points of consideration.

First, Bernard & Snipes advise criminologists to shift their attention from whole theories to the concepts which comprise each theory. Rather than focusing on entire theories, and all of the concepts and processes which they organize, Bernard and Snipes suggest that criminologists should focus only on those concepts and processes which best predict criminal behavior, regardless of the perspective they are organized by. Second, theories should be assessed based on their overall contribution to the scientific process rather than their overall validity. Third, rather than attempting to falsify theories, criminologists should work toward identifying risk factors with the most potent effects on criminal behavior. This process is far more welcoming toward the process of integration and will allow for more nuanced explanations of criminal behavior. Fourth, contradictory theories cannot be integrated, but few criminological theories are contradictory with one another allowing for broad integration. Fifth, theories should be interpreted and organized based on the policy implications that should directly stem from them.

Following these guidelines, Bernard and Snipes argued that all existing conventional theories of criminal behavior could be divided into one of two broad categories: individual difference theories or structure/process theories.

While the integration strategy proposed by Bernard and Snipes (1996) was focused almost exclusively on conventional criminological theories, some of the elements of their strategy have been used in an attempt to incorporate elements of the biosocial perspective into conventional criminological theories. For example, several biosocial criminologists have argued that the field of criminology does not need standalone biosocial theories of criminal behavior, rather, biosocial concepts should simply be integrated into existing theories in an effort to increase their explanatory power (Fishbein, 1990; Walsh, 2000, 2002). This particular strategy is

123 directly related to Bernard and Snipes’ suggestion of shifting attention away from existing theories and instead focusing on concepts and processes which best explain criminal behaviors.

In this way, concepts drawn from both the biosocial perspective as well as conventional theories of crime can be combined into a more comprehensive and effective explanatory model. Walsh

(2000, 2002) has perhaps been the most well-known advocate of this form of biosocial integration and has explicitly demonstrated the manner in which biosocial concepts can be directly integrated into several conventional theories of crime.

Figure 4.2 provides a visual representation of the integrative strategy proposed by Walsh

(2000, 2002) and others. As represented by the single-headed arrows, the direction of theoretical integration runs from theoretical concepts and the biosocial perspective to each of the theories examined in the current project. Importantly, and directly in line with Bernard and Snipes’

(1996; Bernard et al., 2010) suggestions, theoretical concepts are allowed to vary freely and may differentially contribute to each of the four theories included. In this way, concepts are not directly tied to a single theory, but may be included in any or all of the theories presented. In addition, concepts stemming directly from the biosocial perspective are also allowed to contribute to any or all of the included theories. This particular integration strategy is appealing since it makes use of existing conventional theories, but fortifies them with biosocial concepts and research findings preventing the need to construct new theories from scratch. In addition, the integration strategy specified by Bernard and Snipes is still highly relevant, allowing for the further integration of any combination of conventional and biosocial concepts in an effort to maximize the explanatory power of any resulting theoretical models.

This form of biosocial integration possesses several attractive attributes and also directly addresses many of the concerns which accompany the comprehensive biosocial theories

124 discussed above. For example, this form of integration is far less complex than the comprehensive theoretical models previously proposed (Ellis, 2005; Robinson and Beaver,

2009). In addition, this form of integration also retains the aspects of criminological theory conventional criminologists are most familiar with, likely minimizing resistance among the more sociologically-minded. However, with these advantages also comes two important limitations.

First, this form of theoretical integration is directly reliant upon assumptions that, based on the literature summarized in Chapter 2, seem to be untenable. For example, in order to integrate biosocial concepts into existing theories one must inherently assume that the primary concepts, processes, and assumptions specified within each theory are directly in line with the biosocial perspective. Failing to satisfy this assumption would result in a nonsensical theory which offers competing explanations for the outcome of interest. For example, social learning theory is built on the underlying assumption that human beings have no predisposition toward crime or conformity and can effectively be described as “blank slates” (Agnew, 2011). This is a fundamental assumption of social learning theories which postulate that all behavior is learned and effectively transcribed onto each individual’s blank slate. This assumption directly contrasts with decades of research resulting in hundreds of individual studies which provide unequivocal evidence indicating that genetic influences differentially predispose individuals to different patterns of behavior (Mason & Frick, 1994; Miles & Carey, 1997; Moffitt, 2005; Rhee &

Waldman, 2002; Ferguson, 2010). This major discrepancy in even the most fundamental aspect of each perspective (the assumptions they make about human nature) suggests that any form of integration would not only be impossible, but would also result in a highly problematic theory which may actually detract from the overall scientific process.

125

The second limitation of this form of biosocial integration is that the underlying problems and issues stemming from the theories themselves are not addressed. Rather than refine or even discard portions of the existing theories that are ineffective or problematic, this form of integration would retain all aspects of the existing theories but also add an additional layer of complexity in the form of biosocial concepts. For example, as reviewed in Chapter 3, the extant literature seems to indicate that strain operationalized as the disconnect between expectations and payoff is an ineffective predictor of delinquent and criminal behavior (Burton & Cullen, 1992).

In his attempt to integrate the biosocial perspective into classic strain theory, Walsh (2000) intricately describes the biological processes which contribute to variation in response to strain as described by Merton (1938) and Agnew (1992). Walsh’s effort should be viewed as a critical turning point in the field of criminology and has significantly influenced the current project, but missing from his discussion is the absolution of the logical and empirical issues that underlie classic strain theory. In this way, the integration of biosocial concepts may increase the explanatory power of existing theories, but this is simply a function of the concepts included in the theory making the overall utility of the theory itself highly questionable. Simply integrating various aspects of the biosocial perspective does not address the other problematic aspects of the theory. In addition, the improved explanatory power stemming from the integration of biosocial concepts may actually detract from the overall scientific process since it may mask significant problems in the underlying logic or assumptions of each theory. While this form of integration may represent a step in the right direction, it still leaves many problems unaddressed.

4.2 The Biosocial Integration Model

The two general biosocial integration strategies utilized in the extant literature do not present a viable solution to the overall problem of limited explanatory power within existing

126 criminological theories. While neither approach presents a definitive solution to this problem, both strategies represent a step in the right direction and open the door for the development of a more effective integration strategy. Any biosocial integration strategy aiming to boost the explanatory power of existing criminological theories, while avoiding the limitations of the existing biosocial integration strategies, would have to meet four specific criteria: (1) allow biosocial and conventional theoretical concepts to exist in a side-by-side fashion; (2) allow for resulting theoretical perspectives and concepts to be subjected to empirical tests; (3) prevent problematic theoretical development stemming from the integration of concepts organized by theoretical perspectives which hold conflicting assumptions regarding human nature; and (4) allow for theoretical modification and refinement to more closely align with empirical findings.

This dissertation attempts to specify an integration strategy which meets each of these four criteria while also providing a general scaffolding for the integration of both existing and future criminological theories. In addition, various forms of integration can be performed at lower levels of theoretical development prior to taking the necessary steps for biosocial integration. This allows for theoretical refinement during the early stages of the integration process in an effort to maximize the explanatory power of each theory. Conversely, the biosocial integration model also allows for theoretical development directly within the biosocial perspective, however, such theories are held to the same standards as theories which are eventually integrated into the perspective. The biosocial integration model presented in the current dissertation also allows for the integration of both complex and simplistic theoretical perspectives into the biosocial perspective. In this way, the integration models specified by

Liska and colleagues (1989) and Bernard and Snipes (1996) are still relevant and can be performed in the lower levels of the biosocial integration model.

127

The biosocial integration model is presented in Figure 4.3. As can be seen from the figure, the direction of integration leads from existing criminological theory—and the concepts which comprise them—to the biosocial perspective. Importantly, the direction of integration suggested by Walsh (2000, 2002) and Fishbein (1990) is inverted in the biosocial integration model. In this way, the various issues which underlie each of the theoretical perspectives can be addressed at the lower levels of integration. For example, the integration process specified by

Bernard and Snipes (1996) can take place at the bottom levels of the integration model. After conceptual and logical issues have been addressed and theories have been refined to an acceptable level, they can then be integrated into the biosocial perspective. The biosocial integration process involves additional theoretical development, but also requires empirical evidence suggesting that the concepts and processes specified in the original theory significantly predict the outcome of interest within the confines of a genetically-informed analysis. In other words, theorists must ensure that the concepts and processes specified in their theories are not subject to genetic confounding and effectively isolate the association between concepts/processes and the outcome of interest. Theoretical processes and concepts that fail to predict antisocial outcomes within the confines of a genetically-informed analysis should either be refined further or discarded.

This form of integration is directly related to Akers’ integrative technique of conceptual absorption (Akers & Sellers, 2013), which refers to one theory engulfing another due to overlap in the concepts specified in each theory. In addition, this form of theoretical integration is also directly in line with up-and-down integration as discussed by Liska et al. (1989). This type of integration is possible in this instance since the biosocial perspective engulfs both biological and environmental influences on behavior, making it possible to situate theories of antisocial

128 behavior which are comprised of both sets of influences neatly within the perspective. In this way, the biosocial perspective is similar to Vila’s (1994) general paradigm but is far more developed due to the exponential increase in the amount of biosocial research performed during recent years. Due to the broad coverage of the biosocial perspective, virtually any theory can be integrated into the biosocial perspective regardless of whether such a theory emphasizes biological influences, environmental influences, or a combination of the two. The only caveat is that all theories must continue to explain a significant amount of variance in antisocial phenotypes after taking genetic influences into account. This particular aspect of the biosocial integration model directly addresses the limitations of earlier integration models. Namely, subjecting conventional theoretical models to genetically-informed tests prevents the possibility of including competing concepts within the same theoretical model.

4.3 Summary and Discussion

One of the most recent critiques of the biosocial perspective is the overall lack of theoretical development within the perspective and the atheoretical nature of the majority of the studies which comprise the perspective (see Kubrin et al., 2009 as an example). While this particular critique is not without merit, the biosocial perspective—at least in its modern form—is relatively young and has only recently developed into a perspective which can support theoretical development. This observation is evidenced by the lack of consensus among biosocial criminologists regarding the most appropriate strategies for developing biosocial theories. One camp of biosocial criminologists have advocated for large, interdisciplinary, and complex theories which attempt to explain all of the empirical findings presented within the biosocial perspective (Ellis, 2005; Robinson & Beaver, 2009; Vila, 1994). Others have argued that the development of novel theoretical perspectives is unnecessary and specific aspects of the

129 biosocial perspective should be directly integrated into existing conventional theories (Fishbein,

1990; Walsh, 2000, 2002). Both perspectives address limitations of the other, but also leave much to be desired by introducing concerning limitations of their own.

The biosocial integration model presented here is an attempt to directly address the limitations of the previously presented integration models. This model of theoretical integration draws directly from various aspects of the previous models, but also makes necessary modifications in an effort to address the limitations of these previous models. The end result is an integration model which allows for theoretical development and integration across multiple levels. In addition, the biosocial integration model allows for the development of new theories, various forms of integration among existing theories, and the integration of existing theories within the biosocial perspective. By emphasizing the importance of utilizing behavior genetic modeling strategies to effectively isolate associations between theoretical concepts and outcomes of interest, the presented model allows for both the theoretical and empirical integration of various theories within the biosocial perspective. In addition, the biosocial perspective is well- suited for this type of integration since it takes into account not only the independent influences of both biological and environmental factors, but also the intricacies of the interplay between both sets of influences (see Vila, 1994).

The previous chapters present a large amount of information directly bearing on the current state of the field of criminology. Two primary patterns of findings can be gleaned from the large amount of literature summarized in the previous chapters. First, antisocial phenotypes—and many of the concepts directly implicated in popular conventional theories of crime—are significantly influenced by genetic factors which, in turn, may result in genetic confounding and sources of spuriousness. In this way, the current empirical standing of the vast

130 majority of the mainstream criminological theories remains unknown, since previous studies did not properly control for genetic confounding and any resulting associations may be spurious.

Second, while the findings garnered from SSSMs has yielded somewhat mixed results for each of the four theoretical perspectives reviewed in the current dissertation (rational choice theory, classic strain theory, social learning theory, and social bonding theory), these perspectives have persisted indicating that the field of criminology views them as useful in at least some capacity.

Based on this observation, the next step in the current project is to attempt to integrate each of these four theories into the biosocial perspective using the biosocial integration model. Since all four theories have already undergone a significant amount of theoretical refinement, the subsequent analysis will focus more exclusively on examining the potential association between the primary concepts of each theory and antisocial behavior within the confines of a genetically- informed model. The next chapter will provide an overview of the data, measures, and analytic strategies used in the current dissertation.

131

Biosocial Concepts

Biosocial Concepts Biosocial Concepts

Biosocial Theory

Conventional Concepts Conventional Concepts

Conventional Concepts

Figure 4.1: Visual Representation of Existing Biosocial Theories

132

Figure 4.2: Visual Representation of Preliminary Models of Biosocial Integration

133

Figure 4.3: Visual Representation of the Biosocial Integration Model

134

CHAPTER 5

METHODS

The previous chapters have provided an overview of the biosocial perspective (Chapter 2) and summarized the key assumptions and current empirical status of four of the most influential theoretical perspectives within the field of criminology (rational choice theory, social learning theory, classic strain theory, and social bonds theory; Chapter 3). In addition, Chapter 4 proposed a biosocial integration model aimed at developing biosocial theories of antisocial behavior which allows for the integration of existing criminological theories into the biosocial perspective. As previously discussed, in order for a theory to be integrated into the biosocial perspective, the primary processes and concepts organized by the theory must continue to explain a significant portion of the variance in antisocial behavior within the confines of a genetically-informed analytic model. Theories that do not meet this minimal standard cannot be integrated into the biosocial perspective and should either be discarded or, if possible, refined with a more careful eye toward biosocial integration. The primary goal of the current project is to determine whether the four theories discussed in Chapter 3 can be integrated into the biosocial perspective. This task will be performed by addressing each of the four research questions proposed in Chapter 1. The current chapter will provide an overview of the sample analyzed in the current project, the operationalization of the examined theoretical concepts, and the analytic methods performed to test the proposed research questions.

5.1 Data

The current dissertation analyzed data from the National Longitudinal Study of

Adolescent Health (Add Health), which is a nationally representative sample of American youth enrolled in middle or high school during the 1994-1995 school year (Harris et al., 2009). The

Add Health employed a four wave panel design and followed respondents for approximately 14 135 years, covering a substantial portion of the life course. Details of the sampling procedures have been specified previously (Harris, 2011; Harris et al., 2009; Harris, Halpren, Smolen, &

Haberstick, 2006), but the study employed a multi-stage cluster design wherein all middle and high schools in the United States had a known probability of selection, resulting in a nationally representative sample of youth. In total, 132 schools (80 of which were high schools) were selected and stratified by region, urbanicity, school type, ethnic mix, and size. Students attending each of these schools were asked to complete a self-report questionnaire during one class period on a day designated by the research team with no make-up day being offered for absent students. This in-school portion of the study was performed between September 1994 and

April 1995. Student’s parents were notified of the survey and the date it would be administered.

Participation was completely voluntary and all respondents provided written informed consent.

In total, the in-school survey was completed by more than 90,000 students, who provided information on a wide range of topics including overall levels of health, school activities, delinquent behaviors, and friendship networks.

The in-school portion of the study also includes information regarding each respondent’s friendship network. More specifically, each respondent was provided with a roster of students enrolled at their current school and were asked to select their five closest male friends and five closest female friends. Based on this information it is possible to identify specific friendship networks comprised of respondents nested within the Add Health sample. In addition, responses from members of each respondent’s nominated peer groups can be used to glean information regarding each respondent’s peer group and each of the individual members of each friendship network. Importantly, this information has been utilized in previous studies which analyze the

Add Health data (Haynie, 2001, 2002).

136

After completion of the in-school portion of the study, a subsample of respondents was selected to participate in the in-home portion of the study. More specifically, students attending each of the 132 schools included in the in-school portion of the study were stratified by grade and sex and approximately 17 students within each stratum were selected for the in-home portion of the study. This process led to the selection of approximately 200 students from each of the

132 schools included in the study, resulting in a sample of 12,105 “core” respondents. In addition to the core sample, a number of additional special populations were oversampled. After taking into account all of the oversampled populations, the final sample included in the wave 1 in-home portion of the study consisted of 20,745 adolescents. All respondents were asked to complete a 90 minute in-home survey which collected detailed information on a wide range of topics including relationships with parents, delinquent behavior, school performance, and personality traits. In addition, information from 17,700 primary caregivers (most commonly the mother) was also collected. The survey was conducted using several computer-based interviewing techniques based on the content of the questions being asked (Czaja & Blair, 2005).

The majority of questions were asked verbally by a trained interviewer and respondents were asked to record their answer on a laptop computer. For more sensitive questions, such as those dealing with sexual or delinquent behaviors, respondents read questions privately and then responded privately on the provided computer. Importantly, this interviewing technique has been found to produce more truthful answers and reduce the overall amount of bias which accompanies collecting information on more sensitive topics (Turner et al., 1998).

One of the subpopulations oversampled during the first wave of data collection were twins and sibling pairs. Adolescents included in the core subsample were asked whether they had a co-twin or lived with a sibling. Cousins, unrelated siblings, half-siblings and co-twins who

137 lived in the same household as a respondent included in the core sample were added to the sample with certainty. In addition, a probability sample of full-siblings was also added to the sample. In total, over 3,000 sibling pairs of adolescents with varying levels of genetic relatedness are nested within the full Add Health sample (Harris et al., 2006).

Twin zygosity was determined using a three-stage process. First, all opposite sex pairs were classified as DZ twins. Second, all same-sex twins were given a self-report survey aimed at assessing confusability of appearance (Rowe & Jacobson, 1998). These items asked respondents to report how often they were confused for their co-twin by family members, strangers, and teachers. While this process allowed for the classification of the vast majority of the same sex twin-pairs, a small number of pairs could not be classified with certainty. In order to assess the zygosity of such pairs, respondents were asked to provide a buccal cell sample which was used for genotyping. Specifically, members of unknown pairs were compared with one another across seven highly polymorphic genotypes. Pairs matched on at least five polymorphisms were classified as MZ twins and pairs matched on fewer than five polymorphisms were classified as

DZ twins. Importantly, this zygosity determination process has been found to be highly valid, with estimates indicating an accuracy rate of over 95 percent (Reitveldt et al., 2000).

Approximately one year after the completion of the first wave of the study, the second wave of the study commenced. Data collection was carried out over approximately five months

(April-August) in 1996. Respondents who were enrolled in grades 7 through 11 were interviewed again at wave 2, while respondents who were in 12th grade during wave 1 interviews were not reinterviewed at wave 2. Participating respondents were interviewed in their home by trained interviewers. Nearly 15,000 respondents (N = 14,738) participated in the second wave of the study, resulting in a retention rate of nearly 90 percent (Harris et al., 2006). The surveys used

138 during the second wave of data collection were highly similar to those used during the first wave of the study due to the limited amount of time that elapsed between each wave.

The third wave of data collection was carried out between August 2001 and April 2002 when respondents were between 18 and 28 years old. Since most respondents had begun to transition into adulthood at the time the third wave of data was collected, the surveys used for the first and second waves of the study were no longer appropriate. For this reason, the questionnaires were modified to tap more age-appropriate topics such as romantic relationships, employment experiences, civic participation, community involvement, and contact with the criminal justice system. During wave 3 interviews, respondents were also asked to provide a saliva sample and a urine sample. These samples were used to obtain biomarker information for a number of different outcomes including the prevalence of a number of sexually-transmitted diseases (including HIV) and the concentration of various genetic polymorphisms. Due to the amount of time that had elapsed between the second and third waves of data collection, additional effort was made to locate and interview respondents. For example, respondents who were incarcerated were located and interviewed while in jail or . In total, 15,197 respondents participated in the third wave of the study and yielded an overall response rate of 77 percent (Harris et al., 2006).

The fourth and final wave of data collection was carried bout between 2007 and 2008 when respondents were between 24 and 34 years old. Once again, the questionnaire administered to respondents was modified to tap more age-appropriate topics and also to link responses to items collected earlier in the study to various life domains in adulthood (Harris,

2011). For example, during wave 1 interviews, respondents were asked to report how likely they thought they would experience various situations including graduating from college, getting

139 married, and getting HIV or AIDS. At wave 4 respondents were asked to report their current marital status, their highest level of education, and were also asked to provide saliva samples which were tested for the presence of HIV antibodies. Respondents were asked about a wide range of topics tapping social, psychological, health, and economic domains. In addition, respondents were asked to provide additional biomarker indicators which allowed for the collection of a large number of measures including: blood pressure, pulse, lipid levels, glucose levels, inflammation levels, height, weight, waist circumference, and genetic polymorphisms.

Due to extended efforts to minimize attrition and additional sources of missingness, a response rate of over 80 percent was achieved (N = 15,701; Harris, 2011).

5.1.1 Description of the Analytic Sample

As was thoroughly discussed in Chapter 2, the twin-based design is the most commonly used behavior genetic methodology, which can be used to isolate the influence of a given environmental influence on the outcome of interest net of the effect of genetic and shared environmental influences (Plomin et al., 2013). In addition, family studies are based on the underlying logic of twin studies but offer modified equations which properly adjust parameter estimates to take into account non-twin sibling pairs. Extending behavior genetic modeling strategies to incorporate twin pairs and singleton sibling pairs provides at least two advantages over traditional models which are limited to samples of twin pairs. First, previous studies have revealed that samples which consist of both twins and non-twin sibling pairs more accurately capture shared environmental influences on a wide range of outcomes, resulting in more conservative tests (Medland & Hatemi, 2009). Second, family studies address some of the limitations of twin studies such as the violation of the equal environments assumption (EEA) and the presence of assortative mating. Based on these advantages of a family-based approach

140 relative to a twin-based approach, the final sample analyzed in the current dissertation was restricted to MZ twin pairs, DZ twin pairs, full sibling pairs, and half-sibling pairs. Respondents were never asked to report the amount of time they resided in their current household making it impossible to determine how long they lived with their co-sibling or their parents. Since research question 4 is directly related to parent-child relationships, it was necessary to drop cousins and unrelated siblings from the final analytic sample.

Table 5.1 provides an overview of the final analytic sample including the number of individuals and pairs which comprise each category of genetic relatedness. As previously mentioned in Chapter 2, behavior genetic methodologies effectively shift the unit of analysis from individual cases to sibling pairs which allows for the comparison between siblings across measures of interest. Importantly, this particular practice effectively divides the overall sample in half which results in a significant reduction in statistical power making it more difficult to detect moderate to small effects and increasing the chances of committing a Type II error

(Plomin et al., 2013). This issue is typically addressed by creating a “double-entered” dataset wherein each sibling pair is included in the sample twice using a two-step process. First, siblings from each pair are randomly assigned the label of sibling 1 and sibling 2 and entered into the dataset in a sequential fashion (e.g., sibling 1’s score on a given measure followed by sibling 2’s score on the same measure). In this way, the resulting dataset is organized such that each row represents a single family and contains information for both siblings contained within that particular family (i.e., wide format), as opposed to each row representing each individual sibling

(i.e., long format). Second, each sibling pair was entered into the dataset a second time, but the order in which each sibling’s scores was entered was reversed (e.g., sibling 2’s score on a given measure was followed by sibling 1’s score on the same measure). This form of organization

141 accounts for the overall drop in statistical power resulting from shifting the unit of analysis from individuals to pairs, and is conventional within behavior genetic analyses (Plomin et al., 2013).

The primary limitation of this approach is that including each sibling pair into the dataset twice results in within-family clustering and may result in significant levels of heteroskedasticity and, subsequently, untrustworthy standard errors. In order address this limitation, robust standard errors were estimated within each statistical model (Kohler & Rodgers, 2001; Smith & Hatemi,

2013). The final analytic sample contained 570 MZ twins (285 pairs), 892 DZ twins (446 pairs),

2,070 full siblings (1,035 pairs), and 732 half-siblings (366 pairs), resulting in a final sample size of N = 4,264 (2,132 pairs). Importantly, the final sample size varies from model to model as a function of missing data on the measures included in each model.

5.2 Measures

5.2.1 Antisocial Behavior

Directly in line with previous studies analyzing the Add Health data (Bernat et al., 2012;

Jang & Franzen, 2013; Maimon et al., 2012; Zheng & Cleveland, 2013), 4 antisocial behavior measures, one at each wave of data collection, were included in the current dissertation. These measures were designed to tap overall levels of antisocial behavior and include both violent and nonviolent behaviors. Due to modifications to the questionnaires administered at each wave of data collection, the items used to create each antisocial behavior index vary slightly from wave to wave. However, these same measures have been used previously (Barnes, Beaver, & Boutwell,

2011; Mears, Cochran, & Beaver, 2013; Schwartz & Beaver, 2013) and have been found to load on a common factor, providing strong evidence that these items are adequately tapping the concept of antisocial behavior. During wave 1 interviews, respondents were asked to report their involvement in 15 delinquent activities over the past 12 months including shoplifting, stealing

142 something worth more or less than $50, getting into a serious physical fight, and shooting or stabbing anyone. Each of the items included in the wave 1 antisocial behavior index (and all other antisocial behavior indexes) are listed in Appendix A. The same 15 items were asked again during wave 2 interviews. Due to the modifications made to the questionnaire used during wave 3 interviews, the wave 3 antisocial behavior index was slightly different from the wave 1 and wave 2 antisocial behavior indexes. More specifically, respondents were asked 12 questions regarding the frequency in which they deliberately wrote a bad check, used someone else’s credit card without their permission, and carrying a handgun to work or school. These same 12 items were asked again during wave 4 interviews. At all four waves of data collection, responses to each item were coded such that 0 = never, 1 = 1 or 2 times, 2 = 3 or 4 times, and 3 = 5 or more times. Responses to each of the items were summed separately at each wave to create the wave

1 (α =.86), wave 2 (α =.83), wave 3 (α = .69), and wave 4 (α = .72) antisocial behavior indexes.

Higher scores on each index are indicative of higher levels of antisocial behavior at each wave.

Due to a limited amount of variation, the wave 3 and wave 4 antisocial behavior measures were dichotomized such that 0 = a score of zero on the respective antisocial behavior measure and 1 = a score of one or higher on the respective antisocial behavior measure. Table 5.2 provides an overview of the waves at which the antisocial behavior measures and all other measures included in the current dissertation are measured.

5.2.2 Prevalence of Illicit Drug Use

A total of three illicit drug use measures, one at each of the first three waves of data,13 were included in the current dissertation. During interviews at each wave, respondents were

13 While drug use measures were collected during wave 4 interviews, the employed surveys did not ask respondents to report on the same substances they were asked to report on during the first three waves of the study. Based on this discrepancy in content, and in an effort to preserve continuity in the concepts examined, the illicit drug use measures were limited to the first three waves of data. 143 asked to report the number of times they used a variety of different drugs in the past 30 days.

More specifically, respondents were asked how often they used marijuana, cocaine, and other drugs which included LSD, PCP, ecstasy, mushrooms, inhalants, ice, heroine, or non-prescribed prescription drugs. Scores on each measure were coded dichotomously where 0 = never and 1 = one or more times. Importantly, these same measures have been used in previous studies analyzing the Add Health data (Harris et al., 2006; Lessem et al., 2006). These scores were then

14 summed separately at each wave to create the wave 1 (KR20 = .46), 2 (KR20 = .39), and 3 (KR20

= .48) illicit drug use measures. These measures indicate the total number of illicit substances each respondent reported using in the past 30 days and range from 0 to 3.

5.2.3 Alcohol Use

A total of four alcohol use measures, one at each wave, were included in the current dissertation. The alcohol use measure was created using three different measures tapping different dimensions of alcohol use measured at each wave. First, respondents were asked how often they consumed any type of alcohol in the past 12 months. This particular measure is designed to tap the overall frequency of alcohol use within the past 12 months. Second, males were asked how frequently they consumed five or more drinks in a single sitting in the past 12 months while females were asked how frequently they consumed four or more drinks in a single sitting over the same period of time. This measure was designed to capture the frequency in which each respondent engaged in binge drinking over the past 12 months. Third, respondents were asked how frequently they were “drunk or very high on alcohol” over the past 12 months.

This particular measure was designed to tap the frequency in which each respondent consumed alcohol to the point of intoxication. Each measure was assessed at all four waves of the Add

14 Since all of the measured included in the illicit drug use measures are dichotomous, Kuder-Richardson (KR20) coefficients (as opposed to traditional Chronbach’s alpha coefficients) were used to assess reliability. 144

Health study and was coded such that 0 = none; 1 = 1 or 2 days; 2 = once a month or less; 3 = 2 to 3 days a month; 4 = 1 to 2 days a week; 5 = 3 to 5 days a week; 6 = every day or almost every day. All three items were summed separately at each wave to create the wave 1 (α = .92), 2 (α

=.92), 3 (α = .89), and 4 (α = .88) alcohol use measures with higher scores indicating more frequent alcohol use. Importantly, all of the alcohol measures used in the current dissertation have been used previously in studies analyzing the Add Health data (Chen and Jacobson, 2012;

Harris et al., 2006; Shin et al., 2009)

5.2.4 Rational Choice Theory

Thoughtfully Reflective Decision-Making (TRDM). Rational choice was measured using an index designed to capture variation in thoughtfully reflective decision-making (TRDM).

TRDM refers to the tendency to collect all available and relevant information regarding a given problem and thinking deliberately, carefully, and thoughtfully about possible solutions prior to acting (Paternoster & Pogarsky, 2009). In addition, TRDM refers to the application of reason and the full consideration of alternative solutions in addition to the consideration of previous decisions and what went right and wrong. Following the lead of previous researchers analyzing the Add Health sample (Paternoster et al., 2011), TRDM was measured using the following four items from the wave 1 in-home portion of the study: (1) when you have a problem to solve, one of the first things you do is get as many facts about the problem as possible; (2) when you are attempting to find a solution to a problem, you usually try to think of as many different approaches to the problem as possible; (3) when making decisions, you generally use a systematic method for judging and comparing alternatives; and (4) after carrying out a solution to a problem, you usually try to analyze what went right and what went wrong. Responses to each item were coded such that 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree nor

145 disagree, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree. Responses to each of the four items were summed to create the wave 1 TRDM measure (α = .74), with higher scores indicating higher levels of

TRDM.

5.2.5 Social Learning Theory

Peer Network Antisocial Behavior. During the in-school portion of the wave 1 interviews, respondents were provided with a current roster of their school and were asked to nominate their five closest male and five closest female friends. Based on this information, the

Add Health research team identified friendship networks among the respondents which allowed for the creation of friendship network variables tapping the overall concentration of various traits and behaviors within each identified network. Two separate networks are available for all respondents who provided information on this particular section of the study. First, the send network includes all individuals identified by the target respondent as friends. Second, the receive network includes all individuals who identified the target respondent as a friend. In the current dissertation, information from the send network was used to create peer network measures. Importantly, a sizable number of studies indicate that direct measures of peer behaviors tend to be more accurate than indirect measures such as asking a respondent to provide information regarding their peers’ behavior (Prinstein & Wang, 2005; Weerman & Smeenk,

2005; Young, Barnes, Meldrum, & Weerman, 2011). Based on the information included in the

Add Health data and the results flowing from this line of research, direct measures of peer antisocial behavior were analyzed in the current dissertation.

The peer network antisocial behavior measure was created using self-reported information from all subjects with available data in each respondent’s peer network. More

146 specifically, the wave 1 antisocial behavior measures (as described above and outlined in

Appendix A) were generated for each of the four subjects which comprise each respondent’s peer network. The individual scores on the wave 1 antisocial behavior indexes were then group mean centered to create the peer network antisocial behavior index.

Peer Network Illicit Drug Use. The same procedure used to create the peer network antisocial behavior score was also used to create the peer network illicit drug use measure.

Directly in line with the illicit drug use measures generated for respondents, a single peer illicit drug use measure was used in the current study. More specifically, the peer network drug use measure was designed to tap overall drug use among members of each respondent’s peer group and was created using a two-step process. First, illicit drug use measures (as described above) were created for each individual within a given respondent’s peer group. Second, the resulting measures were then averaged across all peers within a given group and indicate the average number of substances used by members of each peer group during the past 30 days. All peer network illicit drug use measures were created using responses from the first wave of data collection.15

Peer Network Alcohol Use. Directly in line with the alcohol use measures constructed for each individual respondent, a single peer network alcohol use measure was created using three separate items tapping overall alcohol consumption habits. First, the average number of times that members of each respondent’s peer network consumed alcohol in the past 12 months was created. Second, the average frequency in which members of the respondent’s peer group consumed five or more drinks in a single sitting over the past 12 months was created. Third, the

15 While these measures could be constructed from information collected during wave 2 interviews, the Add Health does not include any additional information regarding contact with the peer group identified during the first wave of the study. In this way, it is possible that respondents have transitioned to other peer groups and no longer have contact with the peers identified during wave 1 interviews. For this reason, the peer group measures were created using wave 1 items. 147 average frequency in which members of each respondent’s peer group consumed alcohol to the point of intoxication over the past 12 months was created. All three items were then summed resulting in the peer network alcohol use measure wherein higher scores indicate higher levels of alcohol use within a given peer group.

5.2.6 Classic Strain Theory

Disconnect between Aspirations and Expectations. Based on the discrepancies regarding the proper operationalization of classic strain (Burton & Cullen, 1992; Burton et al., 1994;

Kubrin et al., 2009), strain was measured in two separate ways. First, as in previous studies

(Hirschi, 1969), strain was operationalized as the disconnect between aspirations and expectations. During wave 1 and wave 2 interviews, respondents were asked two questions regarding college. Recall, during these two interview periods, the vast majority of respondents would have been in middle or high school with the mean ages at each wave ranging between 15 and 17 years old. In this way, higher education was likely an important point of consideration during this time. The first item asked respondents to indicate their desire to attend college and the second item asked respondents to indicate how likely it was that they would actually be able to attend college. Both items were coded as a 5-point scale wherein 1 = low and 5 = high.

Based on Merton’s (1938) conceptualization of strain, it would be expected that respondents who indicated a relatively high score on the first item and a relatively low score on the second would experience greater levels of strain. More specifically, respondents who greatly desired to attend college (aspirations) but felt that they did not have an adequate amount of opportunity to realize such goals (expectations) would suffer from greater feelings of strain. In order to assess levels of strain stemming from a disconnect between aspirations and expectations, the expectation item was subtracted from the aspiration item. The resulting difference score

148 indicates the level of strain each respondent experienced wherein negative scores indicate higher levels of expectations relative to aspirations indicating low levels of strain (i.e., such respondents expect to have adequate opportunity to attending college, but do not necessarily want to attend).

A score of zero on the resulting difference score indicates that aspirations matched expectations.

Finally, a positive score on the difference score indicates a strong desire to attend college, but a lack of the opportunities needed to realize such aspirations which is expected to result in greater overall levels of strain. Importantly, this operationalization not only taps the disconnect between aspirations and expectations, but also blocked opportunities.

Disconnect between Expectations and Realizations. The second operationalization of strain is meant to tap a disconnect between expectations and the realization of socially- acceptable goals. The longitudinal nature of the Add Health dataset allows for the creation of measures which directly compare expectations regarding important social milestones and the realization of such expectations. More specifically, during wave 2 interviews, respondents were asked to indicate the chances that they would experience several important experiences which included: (1) getting married by age 25; (2) graduating from college; and (3) having a middle class income by age 30. Each of these items were coded as follows: 1 = almost no chance; 2 = some chance, but probably not; 3 = a 50-50 chance; 4 = a good chance; 5 = almost certain. Each item was then collapsed into a dummy variable and coded such that 0 = less than a 50-50 chance and 1 = greater than or equal to a 50-50 chance.

During wave 4 interviews (when respondents were between 24 and 34 years old, which allows for the direct comparison between each respondent’s expectations and their realization of each of these particular goals. During wave 4 interviews, respondents were asked to report their current marital status (0 = not married; 1 = married), their current personal income (measured

149 continuously in dollars), and their highest level of educational achievement. The income measure was recoded in order to take into account a “middle-class income” which was defined as the median score on the wave 4 income measure within the full sample. The middle-class income measure was then coded dichotomously such that 0 = less than the median and 1 = greater than or equal to the median. Educational achievement was coded categorically wherein 1

= no school; 2 = 8th grade or less; 3 = greater than 8th grade, but less than high school; 4 = high school or high school equivalent; 5 = some college; 6 = four year college graduate; 7 = graduate or professional degree. This categorical measure was then collapsed into a dichotomous measure coded such that the first five categories were coded as 0 and the sixth and seventh categories were coded as 1.

After the creation of both sets of dummy variables (expectations and realizations), difference scores were calculated wherein each realization measure was subtracted from the corresponding expectation measure. The resulting difference scores range between -1 and 1 and can be interpreted as follows: -1 = expectations were exceeded; 0 = realizations matched expectations; and 1 = expectations exceeded realizations. In this way, respondents with a score of 1 on the resulting difference scores would be expected to have undergone greater feelings of strain. Finally, to capture a more global measure of strain stemming from the disconnect between expectations and realizations, all three of the difference scores were summed. Once again, higher scores indicate greater levels of strain.

150

5.2.7 Social Bonding Theory16

Attachment. While Hirschi (1969) offered an operationalization of each of the bonds identified in his initial proposal of social bonding theory, the proper measurement strategy for each bond has been a point of contention in the extant literature (Kubrin et al., 2009). Based on this observation, attachment was operationalized using four distinct measures, each aimed at tapping separate socializing agents: parental attachment; school attachment; neighborhood attachment; and peer attachment. During wave 1 and wave 2 interviews, respondents were asked how close they felt to their mother/father and how much they felt that their mother/father cares about them with all four items coded as follows: 1 = not at all; 2 = very little; 3 = somewhat; 4 = quite a bit; 5 = very much. All four items were summed separately at each wave to create the wave 1 (α = .73) and wave 2 (α = .68) parental attachment measures. Higher scores on each resulting measure indicate higher levels of parental attachment. Importantly, these same measures have been previously used in studies analyzing the Add Health data (Beaver, 2008;

Haynie, 2001; Schreck, Fisher, & Miller, 2004). Following the lead of previous studies (Schreck et al., 2004), school attachment was measured using a three item index comprised of responses to the following three questions assessed during wave 1 and wave 2 interviews: (1) feelings of closeness to people at school; (2) feeling like part of school; (3) happiness at current school.

Each item was coded such that 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree. All three items were summed separately at each wave to

16 The social bond of belief was not assessed in the current dissertation. This is primarily due to the fact that the Add Health does not contain measures tapping the overall construct of belief as described by Hirschi (1969). Previous studies tend to operationalize belief as one’s feelings regarding the police or breaking a given set of laws or rules (see Kubrin et al., 2009 for a thorough overview), but these items were not collected during the Add Health study. For this reason, the bond of belief cannot be properly measured and assessed in the current study. However, and as previously discussed in Chapter 3, the bonds of belief and commitment are rarely studied (for example see Schreck et al., 2004) and tend to have small overall effects on delinquency. 151 create the wave 1 (α = .77) and 2 (α = .78) school attachment measures with higher scores indicating higher overall levels of school attachment.

Neighborhood attachment was measured using the same two items collected during wave

1 and 2 interviews. More specifically, respondents were asked how safe they felt in their current neighborhood and how happy or unhappy they would be if they had to leave their current neighborhood. Responses to both items were measured using a 5-point scale and ranged from 1 to 5 with higher scores indicating greater feelings of safety and greater feelings of unhappiness

(indicating contentment with one’s current neighborhood) respectively. Both items were then summed to create the wave 1 (α = .55) and wave 2 (α = .54) neighborhood attachment measures, with higher scores on the resulting measures indicating greater feelings of neighborhood attachment. The measurement strategy for neighborhood attachment is similar to previously used measures (Manning et al., 2005). Finally, peer attachment was measured using a single item included in wave 1 and 2 interviews. More specifically, respondents were asked how much their friends cared about them with responses coded as follows: 1 = not at all; 2 = very little; 3 = somewhat; 4 = quite a bit; 5 = very much. In this way, higher scores on the wave 1 and 2 peer attachment measures represent higher overall levels of peer attachment. Importantly, this same item has been used previously as a measure of peer attachment (Schreck et al., 2004).

Involvement. As with the social bond of attachment, multiple measures of involvement, each tapping a separate facet of involvement, were included in the current dissertation. First, and following the lead of previous studies analyzing the Add Health data (Beaver, 2008; Maldonado-

Molina, Reingle, & Jennings, 2011; Prado, et al., 2009), a measure of parental involvement was included in the current dissertation. During wave 1 and 2 interviews, respondents were asked whether they had engaged in 10 different activities with their mother and/or father in the past

152 four weeks: (1) went shopping; (2) played a sport; (3) attended a religious or church-related event; (4) talked about someone they were dating or a party they attended; (5) attended a movie, play, concert, or sporting event; (6) talked about a personal problem; (7) had a serious argument about their behavior; (8) talked about work or grades; (9) worked on a project for school; (10) talked about other things they are doing in school. Each item was coded dichotomously for mothers and fathers separately such that 0 = no and 1 = yes. All 20 items were then summed separately at each wave to create the wave 1 (KR20 = .71) and 2 (KR20 = .71) parental involvement measures wherein higher scores represent greater levels of parental involvement.

The second measure of involvement included in the current dissertation was community involvement which was measured using a single item. More specifically, during wave 3 interviews, respondents were asked to indicate whether they regularly participated in volunteer or community service work when they were between 12 and 18 years old. As in previous studies, responses were coded dichotomously such that 0 = no and 1 = yes (Paternoster &

Pogarsky, 2009).

Commitment. A significant number of studies examining the potential association between commitment and subsequent delinquency have focused on educational commitment which is typically operationalized as a measure of grade point average (GPA; Agnew, 1991;

Krohn & Massey, 1980). However, a complementary line of research provides preliminary evidence suggesting that individuals with overall lower GPAs are significantly more likely to engage in criminal and delinquent behaviors relative to individuals with higher GPAs even after accounting for other important social bonds such as school attachment (for a current example see

Hoffmann, Erickson, & Spence, 2013). In addition, previous studies have found that GPA does not sufficiently capture the concept of school commitment (Cerkovich & Giordano, 1992). For

153 these reasons, GPA was not used as a measure of commitment in the current dissertation. While

Hirschi (1969) did not recognize religion as a possible source of commitment, subsequent researchers have explored the impact of religion on criminal and delinquent behavior (Johnson et al., 2000; Johnson & Jang, 2010; Johnson, 2011). In addition, religious commitment has been recently recognized as a potential mechanism linking religiosity and antisocial outcomes (Akers

& Sellers, 2013; Kubrin et al., 2009).

Based on these observations and the dearth of literature exploring the potential association between religiosity—operationalized as a social bond—and antisocial behavior, the current dissertation included a measure of religious commitment. More specifically, four identical items at wave 1 and 2 tapping overall religiosity were used to create the religious commitment measure. First, respondents were asked how often they attended religious services in the past 12 months. Second, respondents were asked how often they attended youth group,

Bible classes, or choir affiliated with their church in the past 12 months. Responses to both of these questions were coded such that 1 = never; 2 = less than once a month; 3 = once a month or more, but less than once a week; 4 = once a week or more. Third, respondents were asked how often they prayed in the past 12 months. Responses were coded on a five item scale were 1 = never; 2 = less than once a month; 3 = at least once a month; 4 = at least once a week; and 5 = at least once a day. Fourth, respondents were asked how important religion is to them with responses coded such that 1 = not important at all; 2 = fairly unimportant; 3 = fairly important; and 4 = very important. Principle components analysis revealed that all four items loaded on a common factor. Based on these results, the four items were summed separately at each wave to create the wave 1 (α = .75) and 2 (α = .75) religious commitment measures. Higher scores on the resulting measures are indicative of greater levels of religious commitment.

154

5.2.8 Demographic Covariates

In an effort to prevent confounding stemming from uncontrolled differences across demographic differences within the analytic sample, three demographic controls were included in some steps of the analysis. Age was recorded during wave 1 in-home interviews and was coded continuously. Sex was self-reported during wave 1 interviews and coded such that 0 = female and 1 = male. Finally, race was also self-reported during the in-home portion of the first wave of the study and was coded such that 0 = Caucasian and 1 = all other races.

5.3 Analytic Plan

This section outlines the analytic strategy used to address the research questions presented in Chapter 1. While the analytic plan for each research question is somewhat unique, a general four-step analytic plan was carried out for each question. First, in an effort to determine whether the examined outcome measures comport with the extant literature, it was first necessary to examine the genetic and environmental influences on each of the examined outcomes and theoretical measures. Second, prior to attempting to integrate the examined theoretical concepts into the biosocial perspective, it was necessary to examine whether such concepts significantly predict each of the antisocial outcomes net of the effects of demographic covariates. Third, a genetically-informed analytic strategy was used to effectively control for genetic effects on the examined outcome measure and isolate the association between each conceptual measure and outcome measure. As previously discussed in Chapter 2, genetic and environmental influences often work both independently and interactively to shape variation in antisocial outcomes. In this way, theoretical concepts which do not significantly predict antisocial outcomes within the confines of a genetically-informed model may still play an important role through gene- environment interplay. Based on this possibility, the fourth general step in the analytic plan was

155 to examine the potential moderating effect of examined theoretical concepts on genetic influences on each examined outcome. Below, a more technical discussion of analytic techniques employed at each step of the analysis will be provided. Importantly, this discussion will be organized by the analytic strategy used. Subsequently, the application of each analytic methodology will be applied to the four research questions proposed in Chapter 1.

5.3.1 Univariate ACE Decomposition Model

The first step in the analytic plan is to estimate the genetic and environmental contributions to the examined outcomes and theoretical measures. The overall purpose of this particular step in the analytic strategy is to establish whether genetic factors significantly influence each of the examined outcomes. While there is strong indirect and theoretical evidence suggesting that each of the examined outcomes are significantly influenced by genetic factors

(see Turkheimer, 2003), directly estimating the genetic and environmental influences on each outcome is a critical aspect of the current dissertation. Since the main methodological concern regarding theoretical integration within the biosocial perspective revolves around difficulties surrounding genetic confounding, establishing significant genetic influences on each of the examined outcomes is critically important. More specifically, genetic confounding is not a concern unless genes significantly influence both the examined independent variable and the examined outcome. In this way, genetic confounding cannot occur if the examined outcomes are not significantly influenced by genetic factors.

There are a large number of analytic techniques which allow for the direct estimation of genetic influences on a given outcome measure (see Beaver, 2013a and Plomin et al., 2013 for overviews). However, the univariate ACE decomposition model is arguably the most commonly used. This particular modeling strategy is built upon a structural equation modeling (SEM)

156 framework and used to analyze pairs of twins and siblings. The ACE model is a behavior genetic modeling strategy which allows for the decomposition of variance within a given phenotype into three separate latent constructs: additive genetic influences (symbolized as A); shared environmental influences (symbolized as C); and nonshared environmental influences

(symbolized as E). Importantly, the resulting estimates are latent in the sense that the specific genes and environments contributing to each component remain unknown and apply only to the examined sample. In this way, it is not possible to extend the findings to each of the individuals included in the sample. The ACE model is univariate in that it only examines the phenotypic variance within a single measure.

Since the univariate ACE model is built on an SEM framework, it can be presented using a path diagram which is illustrated in Figure 5.1. The rectangles presented in the figure represent the observed scores for each sibling on the measure of interest (e.g., antisocial behavior). These observed measures (and the covariance between them) are used to define A, C, and E which are latent factors. This procedure is symbolized by the single headed arrow running from each latent factor to the observed score for each sibling. Each of these single headed arrows represents a path estimate (symbolized as a1, a2, c1, c2, e1, and e2) which are collectively used to provide the final estimates of A, C, and E. The double-headed arrow running from the A estimate for one sibling to the A estimate for the second sibling symbolizes the covariance estimates for the two latent constructs. Importantly, these covariance estimates are constrained to reflect the level of genetic relatedness shared by each pair with MZ twin pairs constrained to 1.00, DZ twin and full sibling pairs constrained to .50, and the covariance between half sibling pairs constrained to .25, which is represented by the figures presented above the double-headed arrow connecting each A estimate. The second double-headed arrow, which connects both C components, symbolizes the

157 covariance for both of the latent constructs. The covariance between both C components is constrained to correlate at 1.00 for all sibling pairs since shared environmental influences equally impact both siblings. Finally, the E components of the model are allowed to vary freely and effectively capture any variance that is not explained by the A and C components, providing an estimate of nonshared environmental influences on the examined outcome plus measurement error.

As with most SEM applications, the ACE model allows for the estimation of nested models in an effort to determine whether a more parsimonious model provides a better fit to the data (Medland & Hatemi, 2009). This procedure involves constraining the A and/or C components of the full ACE model to zero in an effort to maximize model fit. Importantly, the E component cannot be constrained to zero since it also capture measurement error. This model- fitting procedure is typically carried out in two steps. First, model fit diagnostics are used to determine the overall goodness of fit. In an effort to assess overall model fit, several diagnostics were utilized in the current study. More specifically, the comparative fit index (CFI), the

Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were all used to determine the adequacy of model fit. More common fit statistics, such as chi-square and the Akaike information criteria (AIC), have been found to be overly influenced by large sample sizes and model complexity and make comparing nested models difficult (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

In addition, the employed model fit diagnostics have well established cut-off points indicative of good or acceptable model fit. Values greater than or equal to .95 on the CFI and TLI indicate good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999), while values below .05 on RMSEA indicate close fit and values up to .08 represent reasonable errors of approximation (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).

158

The second step in assessing the best-fitting and most parsimonious model involves the examination of the standard errors and corresponding p-values for each of the estimated components to determine statistical significance. Nonsignificant components are examined more closely and possibly constrained to zero in a nested model. Wald’s tests of parameter constraints are used to determine whether the constraint of a given estimate to zero results in a significant change in overall model fit. More specifically, Wald’s tests indicate whether constraining a given parameter results in a significant change in the overall chi-square estimate. A significant change in chi-square would indicate a significantly different model, whereas a nonsignificant change would indicate that the nested model is more parsimonious than the full model, and would therefore be favored (assuming all model fit statistics fall within accepted ranges).

The traditional univariate ACE model was developed for use with continuous outcome measures; however, such models have been modified to decompose the variance within dichotomous and categorical measures as well (see Neale, 2009 for a more in-depth overview).

These models, which are referred to as threshold liability models, rely on covariance estimates which are estimated using tetrachoric correlation coefficients rather than the more traditional

Pearson correlation coefficients. In addition, threshold models identify an underlying

“threshold” for each dichotomous or categorical measure included in the model which essentially represents the point in a normal distribution in which a respondent would move from one category to the next. Aside from these differences, the basic logic and interpretation of the threshold model is essentially synonymous with a traditional univariate ACE model. In addition, the model-fitting and evaluation procedures are also highly similar and carried out in the same overall pattern. All univariate decomposition models (both univariate ACE and threshold

159 liability models) were estimated using the statistical software package Mplus 7.11 (Muthén &

Muthén, 2012).

5.3.2 Multivariate Regression

The second step in the analytic plan is to examine the potential association between each of the theoretical concepts and each outcome measure. These associations were estimated using multivariate regression modeling strategies which were adjusted for age, race, and gender in an effort to eliminate common sources of confounding. In this way, each examined outcome measure was regressed on the conceptual measures and the demographic controls. Importantly, the multivariate models were conducted using linear regression (or OLS) for continuous outcome measures (e.g., wave 1 antisocial behavior measures), and logistic regression for binary outcomes (e.g., wave 4 antisocial behavior). In addition, negative binomial regression models were used to examine the illicit drug use measures. Negative binomial models were employed, as opposed to Poisson models, for two reasons. First, the standard deviation of each measure exceeded its mean, indicating the presence of over-dispersion. Second, likelihood ratio tests comparing each model to a Poisson model were estimated and revealed that negative binomial models were more appropriate than Poisson models. The overall purpose of this particular step in the analytic plan is to establish whether each examined theoretical measure is significantly associated with the examined outcomes prior to controlling for potentially confounding genetic influences.

5.3.3 DeFries-Fulker Analysis

The next step in the analytic strategy is to remove any potential sources of genetic confounding and effectively isolate the potential association between each theoretical measure and each examined outcome. While there are a number of different analytic techniques which

160 would allow for this type of procedure (many of which were reviewed in Chapter 2), DeFries-

Fulker (DF) analysis seems to be the most appropriate and straightforward. DF analysis is a regression-based statistic that is appropriate when analyzing samples of twin dyads and sibling pairs. This modeling technique is commonly used in behavior genetic research and provides accurate estimates of the proportion of overall variance in the phenotype of interest that is explained by additive genetic and shared environmental influences, with the residual variance attributed to nonshared environmental influences and error. In addition, recent studies have examined the accuracy of the estimates garnered from DF analysis relative to other modeling strategies and have concluded that DF analysis produces estimates that are directly in line with alternative modeling strategies (Medland & Hatemi, 2009; Smith & Hatemi, 2012).

The original DF equation proposed by DeFries and Fulker (1985) was developed to be used with clinical samples of twins where one twin had an extreme score on the outcome measure of interest. Rodgers and colleagues (1994) modified the original equation to allow for the use of samples drawn from the general population. The modified equation is as follows:

(5.1)

where K1 is the score� on= the � outcome+ � � measure+ � � + �for one� sibling, ∗ � + K �2 is the score on the same outcome measure for their co-sibling. R measures the level of genetic similarity between each sibling and their co-sibling (R = 1.0 for MZ twins, R = .5 for DZ twins and full siblings, and R =

.25 for half siblings), and R * K2 is an interaction term created by multiplying R by K2. In equation 1, = the constant, = the proportion of variance in the outcome measure that is due

2 to the shared� environment (c ),� is not typically interpreted in equation 1, and = the proportion of the variance in the� outcome explained by genetic factors (h2). The� remaining variance in the outcome measure is captured by the error term (e) which is equal to the

161 proportion of the variance in the outcome measure explained by the nonshared environment (e2) plus error.

In a more recent modification, Rodgers and Kohler (2005) provided a modified equation that provides an improvement over equation 5.1. The new DF equation is as follows:

(5.2)

where is still the outcome� = � measure+ � � for− one � sibling,+ � [� K ∗2 remains� − � the] same + � outcome measure for their co-sibling,� and R is still the measure of genetic similarity between the siblings. The new term included in equation 5.2 that was not included in equation 1 is , which is equal to the mean value of . Also, the main effect of R has been dropped from� equation 2 but is still included in the� interaction term, . In equation 2., = the proportion of variance in the outcome measure that can [be� ∗explained� − � by] c2, the proportion� of variance in the outcome measure that can be explained by h2, and e =� the= proportion of variance in the outcome measure that can be explained by e2 plus error.

Importantly, the coefficients in the DF equation are latent factors that provide an estimate of the proportion of variance in the outcome measure that can be explained by genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences. In this way, the coefficients in the DF equation do not implicate specific genetic or environmental influences on the examined phenotype. In an effort to provide a better understanding of the specific genes or environments that contribute to nonshared influences on the outcome measure, equation 5.2 has been modified to include sources of nonshared variance (Rodgers et al., 1994):

(5.3)

Equation 5.3� is= virtually � + � identical� − � to equation+ �[� 5.2 ∗ �with − � the exception] + ���� of one new + � term, ENVDIF.

This term is a difference score that is created by subtracting the first siblings’s score on a given

162 measure from their co-sibling’s score on the same measure. The resulting term (symbolized as

ENVDIF in equation 5.3) measures the difference between siblings on that measure. The corresponding coefficient ( ) is different from the other coefficients in the DF equation and

does not provide an estimation� of explained variance. Rather, does not express the magnitude of the effect but indicates whether ENVDIF significantly contributes� to nonshared environmental influences and the direction of the association. The remaining coefficients included in equation

5.3 are interpreted in the same manner as in equation 5.2.

The previously presented equations were created to take continuous measures into account and are therefore estimated using OLS regression. However, additional modifications to the original DF equations have been made in an effort to allow for the examination of binary outcome measures (Barnes & Beaver, 2012; DeFries & Fulker, 1985; Rodgers & Kohler, 2005;

Rodgers, Rowe, & Li, 1994). The logistic DF equation takes the following form:

(5.4) [] where all of ���the terms −and � [resulting�] = �coefficients + ��2 are+ �2 interpreted� ∗ [� the]+� same��� as with equation+ � 5.3.

Importantly, other variables can also be included in the model as difference measures including race, age, and gender. In addition, the resulting coefficients can be transformed into odds ratios by exponentiating each coefficient which aids in interpretation. The logistic DF equation will be used for all binary outcomes, while the traditional DF equation (equation 5.3) will be used for all continuous outcomes.

5.3.4 Gene-Environment Interaction

As previously discussed in Chapter 2, genetic and environmental influences commonly work both independently and interactively to generate phenotypic variation. This particular phenomenon, commonly referred to as gene-environment interplay, typically takes two forms—

163 gene-environment interaction (G × E) and gene-environment correlation (rGE). Despite this observation, univariate decomposition models such as ACE or threshold models ignore any possible interplay between genetic and environmental influences due to the simplicity of the modeling strategy (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000). The failure to consider gene-environment interplay in the development of a particular phenotype may potentially result in biased A, C, and E parameters. Purcell (2002) demonstrated that failing to account for an interaction between genetic and shared environmental influences results in artificially inflated estimates of C, while failing to account for an interaction between genetic and nonshared environmental influences may result in inflated estimates of A. In addition, theoretical measures that do not perform well in genetically informed models may not directly influence the examined phenotype, but may significantly moderate genetic influences on the same phenotype. In this way, failing to consider gene-environment interplay in regard to the examined theoretical measures may result in an important oversight which results in the incorrect dismissal of a given theory or concept. For these reasons, models which adequately test for the presence of G × Es are estimated in the current dissertation.

While there are several strategies available for modeling gene-environment interplay (see for example Brendgen et al., 2012; Johnson, 2007), many of these strategies focus on the potential moderating effect of continuous variables. Broadly speaking, there are two general methodological approaches used to test for the presence of G × Es. First, samples containing measures of genetic influences, measures of specific environments, and direct measures of behavioral outcomes can be analyzed using one of the previously mentioned analytic modeling strategies. Most often, the potential moderator is multiplied with a latent measure of genetic influences to create a product term. While this may be appropriate in many instances, some

164 studies have revealed that genetic moderating effects are strongest in the tails of the potential moderating influence (Asbury, Wachs, & Plomin, 2005; Beaver, 2011). In line with these findings, the second approach for detecting G × Es simply involves stratifying the analytic sample on the measure of interest and then separately estimating heritability estimates for each group. A significant difference in the resulting heritability estimates would provide evidence indicating that the stratifying variable significantly moderates genetic influences on the examined outcome. This dissertation will employ the latter approach.

Following the lead of previous researchers (Asbury et al., 2005; Beaver, 2011), potential

G × Es were examined using a four-step process. First, dummy indicator variables were created to identify respondents who scored in the top and bottom 25th percentiles of each theoretical variable. For example, the high TRDM measure was coded such that respondents who scored in the top 25th percentile of the continuous TRDM measure were coded 1 and all other respondents were coded 0. Alternatively, all respondents who scored in the bottom 25th percentile of the

TRDM were coded as 1 and all other respondents were coded as 0 on the low TRDM measure.

For dichotomous measures (wave 1 and 2 peer attachment measures and the wave 3 community involvement measure), the sample was stratified using the existing coding. Second, using only the subsample of respondents who were received as score of 1 on the respective high moderator variable, heritability coefficients were estimated for each examined outcome using equation 5.2

(or 5.4 for dichotomous outcome measures). Third, the process was repeated, but the analytic sample was restricted to sibling pairs wherein both siblings received a score of 1 on the respective low moderator variable. Thus far, the specified process has yielded two separate heritability estimates for each outcome—one for respondents who scored in the top 25th percentile of a given theoretical measure and a second for respondents who scored in the bottom

165

25th percentile on the same theoretical measure. The fourth step of the process involved directly comparing these two heritability coefficients in an effort to determine whether they are significantly different from one another. More specifically, Wald’s tests of parameter constraints were used to compare the heritability coefficients from the two separate equations. A Wald’s test which results in a significant chi-square estimate indicates that the two estimates are significantly different from one another, while a nonsignificant chi-square estimate indicates that the two estimates are not significantly different from one another. In this way, a significant difference would indicate the presence of a moderating effect, in which genetic influences on the examined outcome are significantly moderated by the examined theoretical variable. Now that the general analytic strategy has been described, it is possible to apply the general strategy to each individual research question.

5.3.5 Research Question 1

The first research question asked: Does adequately controlling for genetic influences alter the overall association between thoughtfully reflective decision making (TRDM) and antisocial behavior? The analytic plan aimed at addressing this research question was carried out in four steps. First, a univariate ACE model was estimated to decompose the variance in the TRDM measure into the common A, C, and E components. Second, a series of regression equations were estimated wherein each of the outcome measures was separately regressed on the TRDM measure and the demographic covariates. Importantly, these multivariate models were conducted using linear regression (or OLS) for continuous outcomes measures (e.g., wave 1 antisocial behavior), logistic regression for binary outcomes (e.g., wave 4 antisocial behavior), and negative binomial regression for count outcomes (e.g., wave 1 illicit drug use). Third, equations 5.3 and 5.4 (depending on the examined outcome) will be used to examine whether

166 differences in TRDM significantly predict differences in each of the examined outcomes after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. Finally, the stratification G × E modeling strategy will be used to determine whether TRDM significantly moderates genetic influences on each outcome.

5.3.6 Research Question 2

The second research question asked: Does exposure to antisocial peers significantly predict subsequent antisocial behavior even after taking genetic influences into account? The same general four-step analytic plan was used to examine this research question. First, ACE models were estimated to decompose the variance in the peer delinquency, peer drug use, and the peer alcohol use measures into the common A, C, and E factors. Second, multivariate regression was used to examine the potential association between each of the peer network measures and each of the examined outcomes net of the effects of age, sex, and race. Third, DF analysis was used to examine whether differential exposure to peers (as measured using each of the three peer network measures) results in significant differences in each of the examined outcomes after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. Fourth, the G × E modeling strategy was used to examine whether any of the peer network measures significantly moderate genetic influences on each of the examined outcomes.

5.3.7 Research Question 3

The third research question asked: After controlling for genetic influences, does strain, as defined by Merton (1934), significantly predict antisocial behavior? The first step in addressing this research question involved decomposing the variance in both of the strain measures (the disconnect between aspirations and expectations and the disconnect between expectations and realizations) using univariate ACE models. Second, multivariate regression models were used to

167 examine the potential association between both strain measures and each outcome while controlling for the included demographic covariates. Third, DF analysis was used to examine whether differential feelings of strain significantly predict differences in each of the examined outcomes while controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. Fourth, G × E stratification models were used to determine whether any of the strain measures significantly moderated genetic influences on each of the examined outcome measures. Importantly, since the second strain measure (the disconnect between expectations and realizations) takes into account occurrences at both wave 2 and wave 4, only outcomes measured at wave 4 (i.e., the wave 4 delinquency index and alcohol use measure) will be examined in an effort to preserve causal order.

5.3.8 Research Question 4

The fourth research question asked: Does controlling for genetic influences reduce or eliminate the association between attachment, involvement, and commitment and antisocial behavior? First, the variance of each of the examined attachment (parental attachment, school attachment, neighborhood attachment, and peer attachment), involvement (parental involvement and community involvement), and religious commitment measures were decomposed to examine the extent to which genetic and environmental factors influenced each of the outcomes.

Importantly, since peer attachment and community involvement were measured using single, ordinal-level measures, threshold liability models were employed; univariate ACE models were used for all other measures. Second, each of the examined bond measures were included in baseline multivariate regression models, along with all of the demographic covariates. Third, DF analysis was used to determine whether differences in each of the examined bonds results in significant differences in antisocial behavior after controlling for genetic and shared

168 environmental influences. Fourth, the G × E stratification modeling strategy was used to examine whether each of the social bond measures significantly moderate genetic influences on each of the examined outcomes.

169

Table 5.1: Description of the Final Analytical Sample by Level of Genetic Relatedness Pair Type (Genetic Relatedness) Individuals (Pairs) Percentage of Overall Sample MZ Twins (r = 1.00) 570 (285) 13% DZ Twins (r = .50) 892 (446) 21% Full Siblings (r = .50) 2,070 (1,035) 49% Half Siblings (r = .25) 732 (366) 17% Total 4,264 (2,132) 100%

170

Table 5.2: Overview of the Waves at which Each Included Measure is Assessed Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4 Outcome Measures Antisocial Behavior X X X X Illicit Drug Use X X X Alcohol Use X X X X

Rational Choice Theory Thoughtfully Reflective Decision Making (TRDM) X

Social Learning Theory Peer Network Antisocial Behavior X Peer Network Illicit Drug Use X Peer Network Alcohol Use X

Classic Strain Theory Aspirations/Expectations X X Expectations/Realizationsa X

Social Bonding Theory Parental Attachment X X School Attachment X X Neighborhood Attachment X X Peer Attachment X X Parental Involvement X X Community Involvementb X Religious Commitment X X aThe expectations/realizations measure includes items from waves 2 and 4 bThe community involvement measure was a retrospective measure assessed at wave 3, but asked about community involvement between the ages of 12 and 18

171

Figure 5.1: Path Diagram of a Univariate ACE Model

172

CHAPTER 6

RESULTS

This chapter presents the results of the analytic plan presented in the previous chapter.

The results are organized by research question and by examined outcome. For example, the first section of the current chapter will review the results for Research Question 1 in relation to each of the antisocial behavior measures, the illicit drug use measures, and the alcohol use measures.

As a reminder, the analytic plan included four interlocking steps. First, univariate decomposition models were estimated to provide estimates of h2, c2, and e2. Second, multivariate regression models were estimated to examine the potential association between each theoretical measure and outcome net of the effects of demographic influences (age, sex, and race). Third, DF models were estimated to effectively partial out any genetic and shared environmental influences on each examined outcome and effectively isolate any potential association between each theoretical variable and outcome. Fourth, in an effort to detect any potential G × E effects, stratification models were estimated to compare h2 estimates for each outcomes across levels

(high vs. low) of each theoretical variable.

6.1 Research Question 1

The first research question asked: Does adequately controlling for genetic influences alter the overall association between thoughtfully reflective decision making (TRDM) and antisocial behavior? As reviewed in Chapter 5, the previously outlined analytic steps were applied to this question.

173

6.1.1 Antisocial Behavior

Prior to estimating the analytic models examining each of the four measures of antisocial behavior, descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations, and sample sizes for all four antisocial behavior measures and all other measures, were calculated and are presented in

Table 6.1. Importantly, since the wave 3 and wave 4 antisocial behavior measures were coded dichotomously, the means reported in the Table indicate the proportion of the overall sample that engaged in at least one antisocial behavior within each wave. In addition to descriptive statistics, the Table also includes the results from the univariate decomposition models for all four antisocial behavior measures (and all other measures). Importantly, only the results from the best-fitting model are presented in Table 6.1, but the results of all nested models are presented in

Appendix B. The results of the univariate decomposition models indicated that all four measures of antisocial behavior were heritable with genes explaining between 41 (at wave 4) and 46 (at waves 1 and 3) percent of the overall variance in antisocial behavior. Importantly, shared environmental influences failed to significantly explain any variance in any of the antisocial behavior measures, and nonshared environmental influences accounted for any of the variance that was not accounted for by genetic factors. In addition to the antisocial behavior measures, the mean, standard deviation, sample size, and univariate model results for the TRDM measure are also presented in Table 6.1. The result of the univariate ACE model revealed that genetic influences did not explain any of the variance in TRDM, while shared environmental influences explained 13 percent of the variance, and nonshared environmental influences accounted for the remaining 87 percent of the variance.

The next step in the analysis involved estimating multivariate regression models to examine the association between TRDM and each of the antisocial behavior outcome measures while controlling for age, sex, and race. Importantly, the results of the multivariate regression 174 models, and all other analyses, are organized by outcome such that the results for the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.2, the results for the wave 2 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.3, the results for the wave 3 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.4, and the results for the wave 4 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.5. The results of the multivariate regression models indicated that

TRDM was negatively and significantly associated with antisocial behavior at waves 1, 2, and 4, while the association between TRDM and the wave 3 antisocial behavior measure was only marginally significant (p < .10). Interestingly, the magnitude of the association between TRDM and antisocial behavior appeared to diminish over time with the largest effect on the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure and the smallest effect on the wave 3 and 4 antisocial behavior measures.

The next step in the analysis involved the estimation of DF models in an effort to isolate the association between TRDM and each outcome while controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences on each examined outcome. In addition, between-sibling differences in age, sex, and race were also controlled. The results of the DF models are presented in the fourth and fifth columns of the previously mentioned Tables. The results of the DF models were similar to the traditional multivariate regression analyses, and revealed a negative and significant association between TRDM and antisocial behavior at waves 1, 2, and 4. The association between TRDM and the wave 3 antisocial behavior measure was nonsignificant.

The fourth and final step of the analysis involved the comparison of heritability estimates from subsamples stratified by scores in the top 25th percentile of the TRDM and the bottom 25th percentile of the TRDM measure. The results of the Wald’s tests used to compare both sets of coefficients within each outcome revealed that the TRDM measure significantly moderated

175 genetic influences on the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure. More specifically, for respondents who scored in the top 25th percentile of the TRDM measure, genetic influences explained approximately 51 percent of the overall variance in the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure.

Conversely, for respondents who scored in the bottom 25th percentile of the TRDM measure, genetic influences accounted for 23 percent of the overall variance in the same wave 1 antisocial behavior measure. The significant difference between these two coefficients indicates that different levels of TRDM significantly moderates the effect of genetic influences on the wave 1 antisocial behavior index, wherein higher scores on the TRDM measure result in stronger genetic influences on antisocial behavior. Conversely, lower levels of TRDM seem to result in overall lower levels of genetic influence on the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure. The TRDM measure did not significantly moderate genetic influences on any other antisocial behavior measure.

6.1.2 Alcohol Use

Genetic influences explained a significant portion of the variance in alcohol use at each of the four waves. More specifically, genetic influences explained 59 percent of the variance in alcohol use at waves 1 and 2 and 39 percent of the variance in alcohol use at waves 3 and 4.

Importantly, shared environmental influences had no significant influence on alcohol use at waves 1 and 2, but explained 14 percent of the variance at wave 3 and 9 percent of the variance at wave 4 (p < .10). Finally, nonshared environmental influences explained 41 percent of the variance at waves 1 and 2, 54 percent of the variance at wave 3, and 59 percent of the variance at wave 4. The results of the remaining analyses are organized by outcome with the results for the wave 1 alcohol use measure reported in Table 6.6, the wave 2 alcohol use results reported in

Table 6.7, the wave 3 alcohol use results reported in Table 6.8, and the wave 4 alcohol use

176 results reported in Table 6.9. The results of the multivariate regression models indicated that higher levels of TRDM were significantly associated with lower levels of alcohol use at all four waves, but the overall magnitude of the effect was modest, with Beta coefficients ranging between -.04 (wave 2) and -.09 (wave 1). The results of the DF models indicated that after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences, siblings with higher levels of

TRDM were significantly less likely to consume alcohol at wave 1 and wave 4, but TRDM was not significantly associated with alcohol use at waves 2 and 3.

The final step in the analysis, G × E stratification models, indicated that TRDM did not significantly moderate genetic influences on any of the examined alcohol use measures.

However, the difference in heritability coefficients between respondents who had a high score on the TRDM measure (h2 = .63, p < .01) and respondents who had a low score on the TRDM measure (h2 = .41, p < .01) was marginally significant (p < .10) on the wave 3 alcohol use measure.

6.1.3 Illicit Drug Use

As reported in Table 6.1, each of the examined illicit drug use measures was under significant genetic influence, with genetic influences explaining between 39 (wave 2) and 55

(wave 1) percent of the variance in drug use variety. Shared environmental influences did not explain any of the variance in the illicit drug use measures, and nonshared environmental influences explained between 46 (wave 1) and 61 (wave 2) percent of the variance in illicit drug use. The results of the remaining analyses are organized by outcome with the results for the wave 1 illicit drug use measure reported in Table 6.10, wave 2 illicit drug use results reported in

Table 6.11, and wave 3 illicit drug use results reported in Table 6.12. Negative binomial regression models were used to examine the association between TRDM and illicit drug use

177 during waves 1, 2, and 3 net of the effects of age, sex, and race. Importantly, due to the use of negative binomial regression models, the tables reporting the results of models examining the illicit drug use measures report unstandardized regression coefficients, and in order to ease interpretation, incidence rate ratios (IRRs) which can be interpreted similarly to odds ratios

(ORs). The results indicated that higher levels of TRDM resulted in significantly lower levels of drug use at all three waves, but the overall effect sizes were modest with IRRs ranging between

.96 (wave 4) and .92 (wave 1). The results of the DF models revealed that after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences, TRDM was only significantly associated with illicit drug use at wave 1. Finally, the G × E models revealed that differences in h2 estimates across levels of TRDM were marginally significant for the wave 1 illicit drug use measure.

More specifically, for siblings that scored in the top 25th percentile on the TRDM measure, genetic influences explained approximately 49 percent of the variance in the wave 1 illicit drug use measure, relative to 23 percent for the sibling subsample that scored in the bottom 25th percentile of the TRDM measure.

6.2 Research Question 2

The second research question asked: Does exposure to antisocial peers significantly predict subsequent antisocial behavior even after taking genetic influences into account? The same four step analytic plan was applied to this research question. As a reminder, the peer group measures included in the analyses aimed at addressing this research question were created using responses from each respondent’s peers reporting on their own behavior as opposed to each respondent reporting on their peers’ behaviors. The means, standard deviations, and sample sizes for each of the examined peer group measures are reported in Table 6.1.

178

6.2.1 Antisocial Behavior

The first step in the analysis was to decompose the variance in each of the peer group measures using univariate ACE models. The results, which are reported in Table 6.1, revealed that genetic influences did not explain any of the variance in any of the three peer group measures, leaving all of the variance to be explained by shared and nonshared environmental influences. Shared environmental influences explained 21 percent of the variance of peer antisocial behavior while nonshared environmental influences explained the remaining 80 percent of the variance. In regard to the peer drug use measure, shared environmental influences explained 24 percent of the variance and nonshared environmental influences explained the remaining 76 percent of the variance. Finally, shared environmental influences explained 32 percent of the variance in peer alcohol use, while nonshared environmental influences explained the remaining 68 percent of the variance.

The second step of the analysis involved the estimation of multivariate regression models to examine the association between each peer group measure and the four antisocial behavior outcome measures while holding the effects of age, sex, and race constant. Once again, the results of all models examining the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table

6.2, all models examining the wave 2 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.3, all models examining the wave 3 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.4, and all models examining the wave 4 antisocial behavior measure are presented in Table 6.5. The results of the multivariate regression models indicated that peer antisocial behavior, drug use, and alcohol use were significantly associated with antisocial behavior at waves 1 and 2. As expected, the results indicated that siblings who had more antisocial peers, peers who use more drugs, and peers who consumed more alcohol were significantly more likely to engage in antisocial behavior at waves 1, 2, and 4. Interestingly, peer drug use (as opposed to peer 179 delinquency) had the strongest effect on delinquency at each wave. None of the peer measures were significantly associated with antisocial behavior at wave 3. The third step of the analysis involved the estimation of DF models which effectively partial out the effect of genetic and shared environmental influences on the examined outcome in an effort to isolate the potential association between the examined theoretical measure and outcome. The results of the DF models indicated that peer antisocial behavior, drug use, and alcohol use were not significantly associated with antisocial behavior at waves 1, 2, and 3. At wave 4, peer alcohol use was significantly associated with antisocial behavior, and peer antisocial behavior was marginally significant.

The fourth step of the analysis examined whether peer influences significantly moderated genetic influences on each of the antisocial behavior measures. The results revealed that genetic influences on the wave 1 antisocial behavior measure were stronger when paired with less antisocial peer groups (marginally significant) and with peer groups with higher overall rates of illicit drug use. None of the examined peer group measures significantly moderated genetic influences on the wave 2 antisocial behavior measure. Importantly, since the wave 3 and 4 antisocial behavior measures are dichotomous, it was not possible to estimate the proportion of variance explained by genetic influences using DF models. Rather, the coefficients presented in

Tables 6.4 and 6.5 are unstandardized regression coefficients which indicate the overall influence of genes on each examined outcome. Peer drug and alcohol use significantly moderated genetic influences on the wave 3 antisocial behavior measure wherein less exposure to substance using peers resulted in greater levels of genetic risk. Finally, greater exposure to antisocial peers significantly moderated genetic influences on the wave 4 antisocial behavior measure where greater exposure to antisocial peers significantly increased genetic influences.

180

6.2.2 Alcohol Use

As mentioned previously, all analyses are organized by outcome with the results for the wave 1 alcohol use measure reported in Table 6.6, wave 2 alcohol use results reported in Table

6.7, wave 3 alcohol use results reported in Table 6.8, and the wave 4 alcohol use results reported in Table 6.9. In the multivariate regression models, peer alcohol use was significantly associated with alcohol use at all four waves. Moreover, peer drug use significantly predicted alcohol use at waves 1-3, and peer antisocial behavior was significantly associated with alcohol use at waves 1 and 2, and the association was marginally significant at wave 3. However, peer drug and alcohol use was not significantly associated with alcohol use at wave 4. The results of the DF models revealed a significant association between peer antisocial behavior and alcohol consumption at wave 2, as well as a marginally significant association between peer alcohol use and alcohol consumption at wave 2, with all other models producing nonsignificant findings. Finally, the G

× E models revealed only one significant moderating effect wherein peer antisocial behavior significantly moderated genetic influences on alcohol use at wave 1. More specifically, siblings exposed to lower levels of antisocial peers had significantly greater levels of genetic influences on the wave 1 alcohol use measure. The G × E models failed to reveal any other significant moderating influences.

6.2.3 Illicit Drug Use

Once again, all analyses are organized by outcome with the results for the wave 1 illicit drug use measure reported in Table 6.10, wave 2 illicit drug use results reported in Table 6.11, and wave 3 illicit drug use results reported in Table 6.12. The results of the multivariate regression models revealed that all three of the peer measures were significantly associated with illicit drug use at waves 1, 2, and 3 with one exception. More specifically, the association

181 between peer antisocial behavior and illicit drug use at wave 3 was nonsignificant. Across all of the models, peer illicit drug use was the most robust and consistent predictor of illicit drug use.

The results of the DF models produced far fewer significant associations, wherein peer drug and alcohol use were significant with illicit drug use at wave 1 and all other examined associations were nonsignificant. The results of the G × E models produced two significant findings. First, peer alcohol use significantly moderated genetic influences on illicit drug use at wave 2 where siblings who were members of peer groups with overall lower levels of alcohol use experienced significantly greater genetic influences. Second, siblings who were members of the least antisocial peer groups experienced significantly greater genetic influences on the wave 3 illicit drug use measure.

6.3 Research Question 3

The third research question asked: After controlling for genetic influences, does strain, as defined by Merton (1934), significantly predict antisocial behavior? As discussed in Chapter 5, strain was assessed using three separate measures which tap blocked opportunities, a disconnect between aspirations and expectations, as well as the disconnect between expectations and realizations. Since the latter measure includes items from waves 2 and 4, only outcomes measured at wave 4 were examined in an effort to preserve causal order. The descriptive statistics as well as the results for the univariate ACE models for all strain measures are reported in Table 6.1. The results of the ACE models revealed that the vast majority of the variance in the wave 1 blocked opportunity measure was explained by nonshared environmental influences.

While the best fitting model was a full ACE model, genetic and shared environmental influences

182 did not explain a significant portion of the variance in the measure.17 Approximately 26 percent of the variance in the wave 2 blocked opportunity measure was explained by genetic influences, while the remaining 74 percent of the variance was explained by nonshared environmental influences. Along the same lines, approximately 29 percent of the variance in the expectations- realization disjunction measure was explained by genetic influences, while the remaining 71 percent of the variance was explained by nonshared environmental influences.

6.3.1 Antisocial Behavior

The multivariate regression models did not reveal any significant associations between either of the blocked opportunity measures. The only significant association to emerge was between the expectation-realization disjunction measure and the antisocial behavior at wave 4.

While the association was significant, the magnitude of the effect was modest (OR = 1.31). The results of the DF models produced a similar set of findings with only two significant associations. First, the association between the expectation-realization disjunction measure and antisocial behavior at wave 4 remained statistically significant even after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. Second, while the wave 2 blocked opportunity measure was not significantly associated with antisocial behavior at wave 4 in the multivariate regression models, this association was significant in the DF models. However, the association was not in the expected direction and indicated that lower overall levels of blocked opportunities resulted in higher levels of antisocial behavior at wave 4. The G × E models identified a single significant moderating effect where greater feelings of blocked opportunities at wave 1 resulted in significantly greater genetic influences on antisocial behavior at wave 1.

17 However, as is evidenced in Appendix B (Table B.8), constraining both parameters to zero resulted in a significant change in chi-square indicating suboptimal fit. For this reason, the full model was retained and the nonsignificant parameters are reported in Table 6.1. 183

6.3.2 Alcohol Use

The results of the multivariate regression models revealed significant associations between both blocked opportunity measures and alcohol use at wave 4. In addition, a marginally significant association between blocked opportunities and alcohol use at wave 3 was also detected. Importantly, all of the significant associations (and the marginally significant association) were modest in size (Beta = -.04) and negative, indicating that greater levels of blocked opportunities resulted in lower levels of alcohol use. The DF models failed to reveal any significant associations. However, the association between the expectation-realization disjunction measure and alcohol use at wave 4 was marginally significant. Directly in line with the significant associations stemming from the multivariate regression models, the association between the expectation-realization disjunction measure and alcohol use at wave 4 was negative, indicating that lower overall levels of strain resulted in significantly greater levels of alcohol use.

The G × E models revealed two significant moderating effects. First, greater levels of blocked opportunities at wave 1 resulted in significantly greater genetic influences on alcohol use at wave

2. Second, greater levels of blocked opportunities at wave 2 resulted in significantly greater genetic influences on alcohol use at wave 4. Finally, the moderating effect of blocked opportunities at wave 2 on genetic influences on alcohol use at wave 3 was marginally significant where greater levels of blocked opportunities resulted in greater levels of genetic influence.

6.3.3 Illicit Drug Use

The multivariate regression models did not result in any significant associations between any of the strain measures and illicit drug use. Along the same lines, the DF models did not

184 reveal any significant associations. Finally, none of the examined strain measures significantly moderated genetic influences on illicit drug use.

6.4 Research Question 4

The fourth research question asked: Does controlling for genetic influences reduce or eliminate the association between attachment, involvement, and commitment and antisocial behavior? In an effort to address this question, a large number of measures tapping the social bonds of attachment, involvement, and commitment were examined. The descriptive statistics as well as the results for the univariate decomposition models for all social bonding measures are reported in Table 6.1. The results of the univariate decomposition models indicated that all of the social bonding measures were under significant genetic influence with heritability estimates ranging between .27 (wave 1 peer attachment) to .59 (wave 2 parental attachment). In addition, shared environmental influences failed to significantly explain any variance within the vast majority of the examined measures, leaving the remaining variance to be explained by nonshared environmental influences and measurement error. The only social bonding measures which were under significant shared environmental influence were neighborhood attachment at waves 1 (c2 =

.16) and 2 (c2 = .18), parental involvement at wave 1 (c2 = .12), and religious commitment at waves 1 (c2 = .39) and 2 (c2 = .39).

6.4.1 Antisocial Behavior

The multivariate regression models revealed that the vast majority of the examined social bonding measures were significantly associated with antisocial behavior at wave 1 after controlling for age, sex, and race. The only two exceptions were peer attachment at wave 1 and parental involvement at wave 1. A similar pattern of findings emerged when examining antisocial behavior at wave 2. The only measures that were not significantly associated with

185 antisocial behavior at wave 2 were peer attachment at wave 1 and parental involvement at wave

2. Importantly, the association between peer attachment at wave 2 and antisocial behavior at wave 2 was only marginally significant. The multivariate regression models examining antisocial behavior at wave 3 revealed a more substantial number of nonsignficant associations.

More specifically, school attachment at wave 2, neighborhood attachment at wave 1, peer attachment at waves 1 and 2, parental attachment at waves 1 and 2, and community involvement were all nonsignificant. The regression models examining antisocial behavior at wave 4 revealed that, once again, the vast majority of the examined social bonding measures were significantly associated with antisocial behavior. The only two exceptions were peer attachment at waves 1 and 2. In addition, the association between neighborhood attachment and antisocial behavior at wave 4 was only marginally significant.

The results of the DF models revealed a pattern of results that was highly similar to the multivariate regression models. While including adequate controls for genetic and shared environmental influences on each examined outcome resulted in attenuated effect sizes, many of the previously detected associations remained statistically significant. Two patterns stemming directly from the results of the DF models warrant additional attention. First, in some instances, relatively robust associations detected in the multivariate regression models fell from significance when controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. For example, the association between neighborhood attachment at wave 1 was significantly associated with antisocial behavior at wave 2 in the multivariate regression models, but was nonsignificant in the

DF models. Second, in a few instances, the direction of the association detected in the multivariate regression models reversed once genetic and shared environmental influences were controlled. For example, the associations between religious commitment at waves 1 and 2 and

186 antisocial behavior at wave 3 were negative and statistically significant. The results flowing from the DF models indicated that these associations remained statistically significant, but that higher levels of religious commitment resulted in higher levels of antisocial behavior at wave 3.

The G × E models revealed a substantial number of significant moderating influences.

For antisocial behavior at wave 1, the moderating influence of religious commitment at wave 1 was marginally significant. For antisocial behavior at wave 2, genetic influences were significantly moderated by neighborhood attachment at wave 1, wherein lower levels of neighborhood attachment resulted in higher levels of genetic influence. A similar pattern of findings emerged from the models examining antisocial behavior at wave 3, wherein neighborhood attachment and peer attachment significantly moderated genetic influences. In addition, the moderating effects of parental attachment at wave 1, peer attachment at wave 2, and community involvement were all marginally significant. The results of the G × E models revealed that genetic influences on antisocial behavior at wave 4 were significantly moderated by neighborhood attachment at wave 2 and religious commitment at wave 1. In addition, the moderating effects of community involvement were marginally significant.

6.4.2 Alcohol Use

The multivariate regression models revealed that all of the examined social bonding measures were significantly associated with alcohol use at wave 1, with the only exception being peer attachment at wave 1. A similar pattern emerged when examining alcohol use at wave 2 where neighborhood attachment at wave 1 and peer attachment at wave 2 were not significantly associated with alcohol use. At wave 3, all but three social bonding measures (parental attachment at wave 2, neighborhood attachment at wave 1, and parental involvement at wave 1) were significantly associated with alcohol use. However, several associations were positive,

187 indicating that higher levels of bonding were associated with greater levels of alcohol use. For example, the associations between peer attachment at waves 1 and 2 and alcohol use at wave 3 were positive. The multivariate regression models examining alcohol use at wave 4 revealed far fewer significant associations. In addition, two of the significant associations detected were in the unexpected direction and indicated that the presence of social bonds resulted in greater levels of alcohol use.

The results of the DF models examining alcohol use at wave 1 were highly similar to the results of the multivariate regression models. While many of the effect sizes were attenuated, the majority of the significant associations detected in the multivariate models remained statistically significant. The DF models examining alcohol use at subsequent waves produced a different pattern of findings. More specifically, the vast majority of the significant associations detected in the multivariate regression models fell from statistical significance after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences on alcohol use. In addition, there was no discernable pattern revealing a consistent association between any of the examined social bonding measures and alcohol use at waves 2, 3, and 4.

The G × E analyses revealed a number of significant moderating influences. For alcohol use at wave 1, higher levels of religious commitment at wave 1 resulted in significantly greater levels of genetic influences. Neighborhood attachment at wave 2 was also found to significantly moderate genetic influences on alcohol at wave 2. For alcohol use at wave 3, lower levels of community involvement and religious commitment at waves 1 and 2 resulted in significantly greater levels of genetic influence. In addition, the moderating effects of neighborhood attachment at wave 2 were marginally significant. Neighborhood attachment at waves 1 and 2 also significantly moderated genetic influences on alcohol use at wave 4. In addition, higher

188 levels of parental attachment at wave 2 resulted in significantly greater levels of genetic influence on alcohol use at wave 4. Finally, the moderating effects of religious commitment at wave 1 were marginally significant.

6.4.3 Illicit Drug Use

The multivariate regression models examining the associations between the social bonding measures and illicit drug use revealed primarily significant associations across drug use at waves 1 and 2. The association between peer attachment and illicit drug use was nonsignificant at both waves, and peer attachment at wave 2 and parental involvement at wave 2 also failed to significantly predict illicit drug use at wave 2. In addition, the associations between neighborhood attachment at wave 1 and parental involvement at wave 1 and illicit drug use at wave 2 were only marginally significant. The models examining illicit drug use at wave 3 revealed far fewer significant associations. More specifically, parental attachment at waves 1 and 2, school attachment at waves 1 and 2, and religious commitment at waves 1 and 2 were the only significant predictors to emerge.

The results of the DF models revealed far fewer significant associations. For illicit drug use at wave 1, the only significant predictors to emerge were parental attachment at wave 1, school attachment at wave 1, and neighborhood attachment at wave 1. For illicit drug use at wave 2, the only significant predictors wee parental attachment (waves 1 and 2), school attachment (waves 1 and 2), neighborhood attachment (wave 1), parental involvement (wave 1) and religious commitment (waves 1 and 2). Finally, only school attachment at wave 1 was significantly associated with illicit drug use at wave 3. The association between parental attachment at wave 1 and illicit drug use at wave 4 was marginally significant.

189

The G × E models also revealed a number of significant moderating influences. At wave

1, genetic influences on illicit drug use were significantly greater for siblings who experienced higher levels of neighborhood attachment. In addition, the moderating influence of parental involvement at wave 1 was marginally significant. At wave 2, greater levels of both parental attachment at wave 2 and peer attachment at wave 2 resulted in significantly greater genetic influences on illicit drug use. The moderating effects of religious commitment at wave 2 were marginally significant. At wave 3, greater levels of neighborhood attachment at wave 2, peer attachment at wave 2, and religious commitment at wave 1 resulted in significantly greater levels of genetic influence on illicit drug use. The moderating influences of both school attachment at wave 2 and parental involvement at wave 1 were marginally significant.

190

Table 6.1: Descriptive Statistics for all Included Measures Mean SD h2 c2 e2 N (Pairs) Outcomes W1 Antisocial Behavior 4.77 6.20 .46** .00 .54** 2089 W2 Antisocial Behavior 3.21 4.77 .42** .00 .58** 1923 W3 Antisocial Behaviora .25 .44 .46** .00 .54** 1696 W4 Antisocial Behaviora .16 .37 .41** .00 .59** 1761 W1 Drug Use .18 .48 .55** .00 .46** 2070 W2 Drug Use .19 .48 .39** .00 .61** 1908 W3 Drug Use .28 .58 .44** .00 .56** 1719 W1 Alcohol Use .76 1.24 .59** .00 .41** 2101 W2 Alcohol Use .86 1.36 .59** .00 .41** 1931 W3 Alcohol Use 1.41 1.44 .32** .14** .54** 1701 W4 Alcohol Use 1.41 1.40 .32** .09† .59** 1759 Rational Choice W1 TRDM 15.17 2.52 .00 .13** .88** 2086 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior 4.76 5.18 .00 .21** .80** 735 W1 Peer Drug Use .21 .46 .00 .24** .76** 736 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .88 1.08 .00 .32** .68** 738 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .30 .88 .10 .06 .84** 2101 W2 Aspirations/Expectations .28 .86 .26** .00 .74** 1788 Expectations/Realizations 1.13 1.10 .29** .00 .71** 1564 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment 18.38 2.17 .53** .00 .47** 1423 W2 Parental Attachment 17.85 2.18 .59** .00 .41** 1303 W1 School Attachment 11.28 2.59 .44** .00 .56** 2067 W2 School Attachment 11.21 2.53 .42** .00 .58** 1692 W1 Neighborhood Attachment .01 .75 .34** .16** .50** 2104 W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.02 .74 .34** .18** .49** 1942 W1 Peer Attachmentb 4.21 .81 .27** .00 .74** 2098 W2 Peer Attachmentb 4.28 .83 .33** .00 .67** 1930 W1 Parental Involvement 6.80 3.44 .39** .12* .50** 1424 W2 Parental Involvement 6.24 3.44 .51** .00 .49** 1424 W3 Community Involvementa .43 .50 .53** .00 .47** 1716 W1 Religious Commitment .02 .78 .42** .39** .19** 1807 W2 Religious Commitment .01 .77 .32** .39** .29** 1640 Results from best-fitting ACE models reported (full models are presented in Appendix B) aIndicates a dichotomous variable (logistic regression and threshold liability models were used) bIndicates a categorical variable (threshold liability models were used) †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01 191

Table 6.2: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 1 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.35** .05 -.14 -.20** .03 .51** .05 .23* .10 6.71** Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .23** .04 .20 -.05 .08 .41* .17 .83** .16 3.49 † W1 Peer Drug Use 2.90** .44 .22 .14 .61 .60** .12 .22 .15 3.87* W1 Peer Alcohol Use .93** .15 .17 -.05 .28 .73** .19 .86** .20 .21 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .08 .14 .01 -.09 .10 .57** .08 .33** .09 3.81* Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.57** .07 -.21 -.38** .06 .54** .08 .33** .10 2.50 W1 School Attachment -.49** .04 -.21 -.23** .03 .59** .09 .63** .12 .02 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.72** .14 -.09 -.33** .12 .46** .13 .61** .12 .67 W1 Peer Attachment -.19 .13 -.02 -.15 .09 .52** .07 .55** .10 .07 W1 Parental Involvement -.05 .03 -.03 -.01 .03 .45** .10 .74** .20 1.72 W3 Community Involvement -1.00** .20 -.08 -.44* .18 .47** .08 .53** .09 .36 W1 Religious Commitment -1.11** .14 -.14 -.42* .17 .62** .12 .36 .10 2.85† †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

192

Table 6.3: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 2 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.22** .04 -.11 -.11** .03 .41** .05 .62** .14 1.87 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .15** .04 .18 .01 .02 .72** .20 .73** .20 .00 W1 Peer Drug Use 1.77** .33 .18 .17 .28 .40** .13 .35** .14 .08 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .62** .14 .15 .06 .15 .71** .23 .73** .16 .00 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .06 .11 .01 -.02 .08 .53** .06 .40** .12 1.00 W2 Aspirations/Expectations .09 .11 .02 .01 .08 .46** .05 .26** .09 1.74 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.30** .05 -.14 -.13** .04 .52** .09 .38** .13 .64 W2 Parental Attachment -.32** .04 -.15 -.18** .04 .47** .09 .34** .13 .74 W1 School Attachment -.24** .03 -.13 -.08** .03 .38** .09 .23* .12 .93 W2 School Attachment -.28** .03 -.15 -.16** .03 .55** .12 .73** .18 .68 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.35** .11 -.05 -.05 .10 .19* .09 .56** .10 7.57** W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.62** .11 -.10 -.39** .10 .38** .11 .45** .13 .17 W1 Peer Attachment -.07 .10 -.01 -.10 .07 .52** .06 .38** .11 1.34 W2 Peer Attachment -.19† .10 -.03 -.12† .07 .41** .08 .31** .08 .79 W1 Parental Involvement -.06* .03 -.05 -.03 .02 .56** .12 .59** .27 .01 W2 Parental Involvement .01 .02 .01 .00 .02 .58** .13 .73** .21 .38 W3 Community Involvement -.69** .16 -.07 -.35** .13 .40** .09 .50** .11 .48 W1 Religious Commitment -.79** .11 -.14 -.45** .13 .30** .10 .33 .10 .04 W2 Religious Commitment -.79** .10 -.13 -.43** .14 .67** .13 .38† .20 1.51 †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

193

Table 6.4: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 3 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE OR Coef. SE b SE b SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.03† .02 .97 .97 .01 1.63** .22 1.19* .59 .50 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .02 .01 1.01 1.01 .01 1.58 1.63 2.75** .99 .38 W1 Peer Drug Use .21 .15 1.23 .98 .20 1.35** .52 3.86** .90 5.81* W1 Peer Alcohol Use .07 .07 1.07 1.06 .09 1.27 .87 5.13** 1.39 5.54* Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations -.02 .05 .98 .97 .04 1.24** .23 1.37** .43 .07 W2 Aspirations/Expectations -.05 .05 .95 .98 .04 1.47** .25 .73 .55 1.50 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.08** .02 .92 .97 .02 1.04** .32 1.84** .36 2.74 † W2 Parental Attachment -.09** .02 .92 .95* .02 1.53** .33 1.60** .37 .02 W1 School Attachment -.05** .02 .96 .98 .01 1.24** .42 .86 .74 .20 W2 School Attachment -.03 .02 .97 1.00 .02 .86† .48 1.65* .73 .81 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.01 .05 .99 .97 .05 .12 .58 1.95** .55 5.24* W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.21** .06 .81 .82** .05 .73† .41 2.28* .91 2.42 W1 Peer Attachment -.02 .05 .98 .97 .04 1.04** .25 2.24** .36 7.51** W2 Peer Attachment .02 .05 1.02 .97 .04 .83** .31 1.65** .32 3.42† W1 Parental Involvement .01 .01 1.01 1.02 .01 1.31* .55 2.39** .69 1.50 W2 Parental Involvement .00 .01 1.00 .99 .01 1.18* .52 2.01** .76 .80 W3 Community Involvement -.01 .08 .99 .98 .07 1.22** .27 1.93** .31 3.07† W1 Religious Commitment -.15** .06 .86 1.05 .08 1.74** .50 2.03** .58 .14 W2 Religious Commitment -.14* .06 .87 1.06 .08 .88 .60 1.40* .68 .33 Note: G × E coefficients are unstandardized logistic regression coefficients and do not represent the proportion of variance explained. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

194

Table 6.5: Results from Models Examining Antisocial Behavior at Wave 4 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE OR Coef. SE b SE b SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.06** .02 .94 .94** .02 1.07** .28 1.96** .75 1.23 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .04** .01 1.04 1.03 † .02 11.68** 3.64 1.48 1.15 7.16** W1 Peer Drug Use .60** .16 1.83 1.20 .24 1.21 .83 1.93* .97 .32 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .19** .07 1.20 1.22* .11 1.58 1.04 2.38† 1.37 .22 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .04 .05 1.05 .94 .04 1.41** .32 1.58** .52 .08 W2 Aspirations/Expectations -.05 .07 .95 .88* .05 1.40** .35 2.36** .62 1.84 Expectations/Realizations .27** .05 1.31 1.10* .04 .18 1.34 1.09 1.27 .24 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.11** .03 .90 .93** .02 1.65** .50 1.63** .61 .00 W2 Parental Attachment -.12** .03 .89 .95† .03 1.64** .46 1.10 .83 .32 W1 School Attachment -.08** .02 .92 .96* .02 .02 .57 1.70 1.16 1.68 W2 School Attachment -.07** .02 .93 .96* .02 -.69 .62 .98 1.26 1.41 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.12† .06 .89 .96 .06 1.94** .61 1.98** .75 .00 W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.17** .06 .85 .80** .05 1.58** .54 4.09** 1.11 4.13* W1 Peer Attachment -.04 .06 .96 .94 .04 1.40** .32 .86 .55 .71 W2 Peer Attachment -.03 .06 .97 1.00 .05 1.05** .37 2.11 .53 2.67 W1 Parental Involvement -.04* .02 .96 .96* .02 2.20** .75 2.23 1.51 .00 W2 Parental Involvement -.04* .02 .96 1.00 .02 1.84** .61 3.79** 1.46 1.52 W3 Community Involvement -.43** .10 .65 .84* .07 1.20** .37 -.04 .63 2.87† W1 Religious Commitment -.31** .06 .73 .80* .07 2.34** .67 -1.28 1.38 5.59* W2 Religious Commitment -.32** .07 .73 .71** .07 1.84** .62 2.45** .69 .43 Note: G × E coefficients are unstandardized logistic regression coefficients and do not represent the proportion of variance explained. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

195

Table 6.6: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 1 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.04** .01 -.09 -.02** .01 .62** .04 .61** .11 .01 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .04** .01 .15 -.01 .01 .22 .20 .79** .15 5.23* W1 Peer Drug Use .65** .09 .23 .03 .10 .25** .08 .55* .22 1.62 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .34** .04 .29 -.04 .05 .64** .22 .84** .15 .59 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .00 .02 .00 .00 .02 .63** .05 .61** .11 .05 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.08** .01 -.15 -.04** .01 .65** .06 .52** .10 1.25 W1 School Attachment -.06** .01 -.13 -.03** .01 .67** .08 .70** .12 .03 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.06* .03 -.03 .00 .02 .65** .08 .47** .10 1.76 W1 Peer Attachment .01 .03 .01 -.02 .02 .71** .05 .72** .06 .04 W1 Parental Involvement -.02** .01 -.06 -.01† .01 .75** .11 .60** .12 .88 W3 Community Involvement -.23** .04 -.09 -.02 .03 .56** .06 .67** .08 1.28 W1 Religious Commitment -.24** .03 -.15 -.14** .03 .79** .08 .25* .13 12.38** †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

196

Table 6.7: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 2 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.02** .01 -.04 .00 .01 .58** .05 .76** .12 1.96 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .04** .01 .13 .02* .01 .88** .23 .35 .26 2.24 W1 Peer Drug Use .73** .10 .24 -.03 .11 .47** .12 .59** .19 .30 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .37** .04 .29 .08† .05 .61* .29 .90** .16 .71 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations -.02 .02 -.01 .00 .02 .71** .05 .34** .08 14.10** W2 Aspirations/Expectations .05 .03 .03 .03 .02 .59** .06 .51** .11 .37 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.05** .01 -.08 -.02* .01 .51** .07 .65** .09 1.47 W2 Parental Attachment -.04** .01 -.07 -.03** .01 .58** .08 .66** .09 .46 W1 School Attachment -.05** .01 -.09 -.01 .01 .54** .09 .53** .12 .00 W2 School Attachment -.03** .01 -.06 .00 .01 .72** .13 .76** .14 .04 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.04 .03 -.02 -.02 .03 .74** .09 .76** .09 .02 W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.06* .03 -.03 -.04 .03 .45** .09 .79** .11 5.39* W1 Peer Attachment .06* .03 .03 .00 .02 .65** .06 .67** .07 .04 W2 Peer Attachment .03 .03 .02 .01 .02 .71** .06 .73** .06 .05 W1 Parental Involvement -.01† .01 -.03 -.01† .01 .64** .12 .84** .14 1.26 W2 Parental Involvement -.01* .01 -.04 -.01* .01 .55** .14 .72** .20 .44 W3 Community Involvement -.15** .05 -.06 -.05 .04 .57** .06 .71** .07 2.31 W1 Religious Commitment -.27** .30 -.16 -.18** .03 .61** .10 .60** .16 .00 W2 Religious Commitment -.29** .03 -.17 -.18** .04 .69** .10 .43** .14 2.45 †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

197

Table 6.8: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 3 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.04** .01 -.07 -.01 .01 .63** .04 .41** .11 3.66 † Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .01† .01 .05 .00 .01 .10 .25 .15 .27 .02 W1 Peer Drug Use .20* .09 .06 .10 .11 .53** .09 .25 .19 1.72 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .13** .04 .09 .00 .05 .73** .13 .77** .13 .04 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations -.05† .03 -.03 .00 .02 .61** .05 .68** .08 .61 W2 Aspirations/Expectations -.04 .03 -.02 .03 .02 .69** .05 .47** .11 3.62† Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.03** .01 -.05 -.01 .01 .22 .19 .36 .18 .28 W2 Parental Attachment -.02 .01 -.02 .00 .01 .67** .07 .61** .08 .30 W1 School Attachment -.03** .01 -.05 -.01† .01 .62** .07 .61** .12 .00 W2 School Attachment -.02* .01 -.04 .00 .01 .44* .20 .58 .42 .09 W1 Neighborhood Attachment .05 .03 .02 -.02 .03 .78** .09 .72** .11 .17 W2 Neighborhood Attachment .07* .03 .04 .03 .03 .64** .08 .85** .08 3.35† W1 Peer Attachment .11** .03 .06 .01 .02 .58** .06 .49** .07 .91 W2 Peer Attachment .11** .03 .06 .00 .02 .49** .06 .51** .06 .10 W1 Parental Involvement .00 .01 .01 .00 .01 .62** .11 .56** .14 .12 W2 Parental Involvement .02* .01 .04 .00 .01 .73** .09 .53** .19 .93 W3 Community Involvement .09† .05 .03 .00 .04 .50** .06 .68** .06 4.76* W1 Religious Commitment -.21** .03 -.11 -.09* .04 .33** .10 .74** .10 8.54** W2 Religious Commitment -.23** .04 -.12 -.06 .04 .40** .11 .72** .10 4.79* †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

198

Table 6.9: Results from Models Examining Alcohol Use at Wave 4 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE Beta Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.04** .01 -.07 -.02** .01 .35** .13 .41 .39 .04 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .00 .01 .00 .00 .01 .32 † .18 .37 .24 .03 W1 Peer Drug Use .11 .09 .03 -.07 .10 .51** .10 .30 .19 1.00 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .10* .04 .08 -.01 .06 .54** .16 .81** .13 1.62 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations -.06* .03 -.04 .00 .02 .53** .05 .38** .10 1.65 W2 Aspirations/Expectations -.07* .03 -.04 -.03 .02 .64** .05 .24* .12 9.64** Expectations/Realizations -.02 .02 -.01 .03† .02 .41** .12 .59* .25 .43 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.05** .01 -.07 -.03* .01 .58** .07 .49** .08 .65 W2 Parental Attachment -.02 .01 -.03 .00 .01 .53** .07 .27** .09 5.45* W1 School Attachment -.02** .01 -.04 .00 .01 .35** .07 .39** .12 .05 W2 School Attachment -.01 .01 -.03 -.01 .01 .48** .09 .41* .17 .13 W1 Neighborhood Attachment .04 .03 .02 -.01 .03 .28** .11 .80** .09 13.16** W2 Neighborhood Attachment .07* .03 .04 -.03 .03 .27** .10 .75** .10 10.77** W1 Peer Attachment .03 .03 .02 -.03 .02 .44** .06 .43** .07 .01 W2 Peer Attachment .07* .03 .04 .02 .02 .48** .07 .55** .06 .56 W1 Parental Involvement .00 .01 .01 -.02* .01 .40** .15 .69** .17 1.71 W2 Parental Involvement .01† .01 .04 .00 .01 .22* .11 .50** .19 1.61 W3 Community Involvement .04 .05 .01 -.01 .04 .41** .07 .49** .07 .76 W1 Religious Commitment -.21** .03 -.12 -.06 .04 .24* .12 .52** .10 3.29† W2 Religious Commitment -.22** .03 -.12 -.08† .04 .54** .10 .41** .10 .91 †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

199

Table 6.10: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 1 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE IRR Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.08** .02 .92 -.01** .00 .49** .06 .23 † .13 3.16 † Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .06** .01 1.06 .00 .00 -.07 .08 .26 .20 2.20 W1 Peer Drug Use 1.08** .08 2.94 .10* .04 .39* .15 .60** .15 .93 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .42** .05 1.53 .03* .02 .57 .38 .58** .18 .00 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .06 .05 1.06 .00 .01 .54** .06 .62** .13 .25 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.16** .02 .85 -.02** .00 .48** .08 .48** .17 .00 W1 School Attachment -.12** .01 .89 -.01** .00 .44** .10 .43* .19 .00 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.18** .06 .83 -.02* .01 .46** .10 .02 .06 15.13** W1 Peer Attachment .05 .06 1.05 .01 .01 .54** .07 .49** .09 .27 W1 Parental Involvement -.06** .02 .95 .00 .00 .59** .15 1.00** .27 3.22† W3 Community Involvement -.39** .10 .68 .01 .01 .53** .08 .34** .10 2.24 W1 Religious Commitment -.48** .05 .62 -.01 .01 .52** .09 .38* .18 .51 †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

200

Table 6.11: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 2 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE IRR Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.07** .02 .93 .00 .00 .33** .05 .23 † .13 .56 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .05** .01 1.05 .00 .00 .51 .36 .64** .22 .09 W1 Peer Drug Use .84** .10 2.23 .01 .04 .51* .25 .32* .14 .41 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .33** .05 1.39 .02 .01 .11 .13 .55** .16 4.46* Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .07 .05 1.06 .01 .01 .34** .06 .44** .15 .37 W2 Aspirations/Expectations .08 .05 1.09 .00 .01 .38** .07 .47** .15 .31 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.13** .02 .88 -.02** .00 .39** .09 .30** .10 .45 W2 Parental Attachment -.10** .02 .90 -.01** .00 .36** .10 .12† .07 4.41* W1 School Attachment -.10** .01 .90 -.01* .00 .63** .11 .45* .18 .67 W2 School Attachment -.12** .01 .89 -.01** .00 .71** .12 .57** .20 .39 W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.10† .05 .90 .00 .01 .30* .14 .06 .09 2.36 W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.21** .05 .81 -.03** .01 .40** .12 .30† .16 .27 W1 Peer Attachment .00 .06 1.00 .00 .01 .36** .07 .52** .11 1.50 W2 Peer Attachment -.05 .05 .95 .00 .01 .55** .07 .25** .08 7.00** W1 Parental Involvement -.03† .02 .97 -.01* .00 .62* .25 .73** .26 .08 W2 Parental Involvement -.01 .01 .99 .00 .00 .43* .20 .42* .20 .00 W3 Community Involvement -.43** .09 .65 -.01 .01 .40** .09 .29** .09 .79 W1 Religious Commitment -.41** .05 .66 -.04** .01 .42** .17 .63** .17 .86 W2 Religious Commitment -.52** .05 .60 -.06** .01 .55** .16 .18 .13 3.37† †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

201

Table 6.12: Results from Models Examining Illicit Drug Use at Wave 3 Multivariate Regression DF Model G × E Model

Top 25% Bottom 25% b SE IRR Coef. SE h2 SE h2 SE χ 2 Rational Choice W1 TRDM -.04** .01 .96 .00 .00 .48** .06 .50** .14 .01 Social Learning W1 Peer Antisocial Behavior .01 .01 1.01 .00 .00 .12 .19 .99** .32 5.46* W1 Peer Drug Use .45** .12 1.56 .03 .04 .42** .14 .41** .15 .00 W1 Peer Alcohol Use .15** .06 1.17 -.01 .02 .58* .24 .64* .27 .02 Classic Strain W1 Aspirations/Expectations .01 .04 1.01 .00 .01 .46** .07 .40** .10 .26 W2 Aspirations/Expectations -.02 .05 .98 .01 .01 .46** .07 .48** .12 .01 Social Bonding W1 Parental Attachment -.10** .02 .90 -.01 † .01 .36** .10 .57** .09 2.57 W2 Parental Attachment -.07** .02 .93 .00 .01 .53** .10 .48** .10 .11 W1 School Attachment -.08** .01 .92 -.01* .00 .46** .09 .73** .16 2.21 W2 School Attachment -.07** .01 .93 .00 .00 .25** .09 .05 .06 3.07† W1 Neighborhood Attachment -.02 .05 .98 .00 .01 .15 .10 .75** .15 11.07** W2 Neighborhood Attachment -.07 .05 .93 -.02 .01 .60** .11 .75** .20 .46 W1 Peer Attachment .05 .05 1.05 .00 .01 .45** .08 .53** .09 .44 W2 Peer Attachment .03 .05 1.03 -.01 .01 .39** .07 .70** .08 8.59** W1 Parental Involvement -.02 .01 .98 .00 .00 .67** .12 .34* .14 3.17† W2 Parental Involvement -.03 .01 .97 .00 .00 .54** .14 .42* .21 .25 W3 Community Involvement -.03 .07 .97 .00 .02 .50** .08 .53** .08 .06 W1 Religious Commitment -.27** .05 .76 .02 .02 .31** .10 .70** .16 4.26* W2 Religious Commitment -.36** .05 .70 -.02 .02 .42** .13 .21† .12 1.67 †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

202

CHAPTER 7

DISCUSSION

This final chapter aims to achieve three main goals. First, a summary of the overall results stemming from the analyses will be provided. The findings will be contextualized within the larger goals of the dissertation and the extant literature. Second, some of the limitations of the current dissertation will be presented and discussed. While every possible precaution was taken to minimize the potential biasing effects of any limitations, some issues remain and warrant attention. Third, the final section of this chapter will provide a discussion of the future directions of theoretical development and refinement within the biosocial perspective. This discussion will focus on the future refinement of existing theoretical perspectives to fit more cleanly within the biosocial perspective, and also on the development of new theoretical perspectives which have yet to be formally proposed.

7.1 Summary of Results

Chapter 6 provides a detailed description of all of the analyses performed in the current dissertation; however, the abundance of findings flowing from the analysis increases the difficulty of identifying overarching patterns. Based on this observation, this section will provide a summary of the overall pattern of findings flowing from the analyses and effectively situate such findings within the context of the previously proposed research questions.

The first research question was focused on rational choice theory and asked: does adequately controlling for genetic influences alter the overall association between thoughtfully reflective decision making (TRDM) and antisocial behavior? The results flowing from the multivariate regression models (SSSMs which do not include any controls for genetic influences 203 on the outcome) fell directly in line with previous studies (Paternoster & Pogarsky, 2009;

Paternoster et al., 2011) and revealed that higher levels of TRDM resulted in significantly lower levels of antisocial behavior, alcohol use, and illicit drug use. In fact, the TRDM measure was significantly associated with each of the examined outcomes in all 11 estimated models. While these findings provide preliminary support for rational choice (or deterrence) theory, Research

Question 1 is more focused on what happens to this association after genetic influences are controlled. The results of the DF models, which effectively partial out the effects of genetic and shared environmental influences, revealed that several of the previously detected associations were driven by uncontrolled genetic and shared environmental influences, with only 55 percent

(6 out of 11) of the examined equations identifying significant associations. In addition, within the statistically significant associations, the magnitude of the effect of TRDM on each examined outcome was attenuated after genetic influences on the outcome were controlled. Despite these findings, it is worth reiterating that the majority, 55 percent, of the estimated equations yielded significant associations, indicating that TRDM may play a significant role in the etiological development of antisocial behavior and substance use.

The second research question was directly aimed at examining the utility of social learning theory within the biosocial perspective and asked: does exposure to delinquent peers predict subsequent antisocial behavior even after taking genetic influences into account? Once again, the results of the multivariate regression models provided substantial support for social learning theory and revealed that 27 out of the 33 (82 percent) models estimated revealed statistically significant associations between peer behaviors and each of the examined outcomes.

Importantly, these findings fall directly in line with the extant literature (Huizinga et al., 1991;

Loeber & Dishion, 1987; Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986; Warr, 2002), leading to the

204 conclusion that peer behavior is one of the most robust correlates of subsequent criminal and antisocial behavior (Pratt et al., 2010). The results of the genetically-informed models did not produce such a promising outlook. Only 18 percent of the estimated models (6 out of 33) revealed statistically significant associations. However, several models did indicate that peer antisocial behavior and substance use significantly moderated genetic influences on the examined outcomes. This overall pattern of findings indicates that while peers may not have much of a direct effect on antisocial outcomes once genetic influences are adequately controlled, peer behavior may still play a significant role in the moderation of both genetic and environmental influences that ultimately culminate into important behavioral patterns.

The third research question was aimed at examining the potential role of classic strain theory within the biosocial perspective and asked: after controlling for genetic influences, does strain, as defined by Merton (1934), significantly predict antisocial behavior? The results of the multivariate models did not provide much support for classic strain theory. In fact, of the 21 models estimated, only 4 models (19 percent) revealed statistically significant associations. As a reminder, a fairly wide range of operationalizations of strain were included in various models, including two measures of individual perceptions blocked opportunities, which prior research has indicated is directly in line with Merton’s (1934) description of strain (Burton & Cullen, 1992;

Kubrin et al., 2009). The genetically informed models produced a highly similar pattern of findings with 14 percent (3 out of 21) of the estimated models producing statistically significant associations. Once again, however, strain did appear to moderate genetic influences on the examined outcomes, wherein higher levels of strain resulted in significantly greater levels of genetic influence on both antisocial behavior and alcohol use. Feelings of strain may not

205 produce a significant direct effect on antisocial behavior, but they may significantly moderate genetic influences on such behavior.

The fourth and final research question was aimed at examining social bonding theory within a biosocial context and asked: does controlling for genetic influences reduce or eliminate the association between attachment, involvement, and commitment and antisocial behavior?

Taken as a whole, the multivariate regression models provided support for social bonding theory with 71 percent of the estimated equations resulting in significant associations. The most consistent and robust associations involved parental attachment, school attachment, and religious commitment. Importantly, these findings fall directly in line with previous studies examining social bonding theory (Costello & Vowell, 1999; Kempf, 1993; Rankin & Kern, 1994; Wright,

Caspi, Moffitt, & Silva, 1999). Despite these promising findings, the univariate decomposition models revealed that all of the social bonding measures were under some degree of genetic influence, leaving open the possibility of genetic confounding. The results of the DF models provided additional evidence of this possibility, with a large portion of the statistically significant associations identified in the multivariate models falling from significance after controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences. More specifically, only 53 of the total 125 (42 percent) estimated models identified significant associations between social bonding measures and the examined outcomes. Once again, parental attachment, school attachment, and religious commitment had the most consistent effects on the examined outcomes. In addition, a number of models revealed that social bonding measures significantly moderated genetic influences on the examined outcomes. These moderating effects however, were somewhat inconsistent and did not produce a discernable pattern.

206

In an effort to better contextualize the overall findings of the current dissertation, the number of significant models (p ≤ .10) were tabulated by theory and by the type of model estimated with the results presented in Table 7.1. Each cell in the Table indicates the number of estimated models that identified a significant (p ≤ .10) association between measures from each theoretical perspective and the specified outcome. The total number of significant associations

(as well as the total number of models estimated) are presented in the final two rows of the

Table. In an effort to provide a more detailed overview of the primary findings of the current dissertation, Figure 7.1 offers a visual representation of the percentage of all significant associations by outcome and the model estimated.

As can be seen in Figure 7.1, across all three outcome categories, the multivariate regression models (SSSMs) identified more significant associations than the DF models which controlled for genetic and shared environmental influences on each outcome. For all antisocial behavior measures, the SSSMs identified significant associations in 67 percent of all estimated models while the DF models only identified significant associations in 46 percent of the estimated models. The largest discrepancy in results comes from models examining alcohol use.

Models which did not control for genetic influences found significant associations in 73 percent of the 50 models examining alcohol use, while models which included genetic controls only found significant associations in 29 percent of all estimated models. The models examining illicit drug use also produced a relatively large discrepancy in results. More specifically, the

SSSMs identified significant associations in 66 percent of the estimated models, but the DF models only identified significant associations in 32 percent of the estimated models. Taken together, the SSSMs identified 131 significant associations within the 190 estimated models (69

207 percent), while the DF models only identified 68 significant associations within the same number of estimated models (36 percent).

Collectively, the results flowing from the conducted analyses can be organized into three conclusions which have direct implications for each of the theoretical perspectives examined.

First, at the most basic and elementary level, the results reify the claim that virtually all of the variables criminologists examine or are interested in are under some sort of genetic influence.

Aside from the TRDM measure and the social learning measures, all of the remaining measures

(25 out of 31 or approximately 81 percent) examined in the current dissertation were significantly influenced by genes. Even measures that are typically viewed as “purely social,” such as parental involvement, were under substantial genetic influence (between 39 and 51 percent of the variance). This finding demonstrates the need for more biologically-conscious theorizing within the field of criminology. Even variables that have been thought of as the bedrock of the social construction of behavior, such as Mertonian strain, are significantly influenced by genes. In addition, this finding provides direct evidence against a “this-or-that” approach, in which conventional criminologists may claim to focus on purely social causes of behavior while biosocial researchers focus on biological or biosocial causes. Such an approach is simply not possible due to the pervasive influence of genes and other biological factors which permeates virtually all variables related to behavior.

The second conclusion stemming from the findings has more direct implications for criminological theory and the field of criminology. While the genetically informed models identified fewer significant associations than the SSSMs, some of the examined theoretical concepts exerted a significant influence on the examined outcomes even after controlling for genetic influences. This finding falls directly in line with previous theoretical and empirical

208 evidence indicating that nonshared environments likely exert more influence on behavioral outcomes than shared environments (Johnson et al., 2009; Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). In addition, even environmental factors which seem to be clear examples of shared environmental influences, such as neighborhood attachment, are far more likely to result in differences between siblings from the same household, indicating that such influences still have a significant effect on behavior, but not likely in the ways our current theories seem to indicate. In short, this finding does not indicate that environments fail to exert an influence on behavioral outcomes, rather, these findings indicate that shared environmental influences exert little to no impact on such outcomes.

The minimal impact of shared environmental influences on behavioral outcomes is so pervasive that prominent behavioral geneticists have repeatedly acknowledged this finding

(Turkheimer, 2000), and have also attempted to elucidate this overall pattern of findings

(Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). Explanations stemming from this line of research have implicated the importance of distinguishing between “objective” and “effective” environmental influences (Goldsmith, 1993). Shared environmental influences are typically operationalized as objective environmental influences in that two siblings from the same household will possess the same score on a given measure. For example, both siblings from a single household would likely receive the same score on a household SES measure. Despite such influences being measured in an objective way, the behavioral outcomes produced by such measures may distinctly vary between siblings. In this way, an objective environmental influence may produce an effective contribution to behavioral outcomes. For example, while a poverty-stricken household may manifest as an objective environmental influence in the data, living within such a household may be differentially processed and interpreted by siblings within the household and may ultimately

209 result in behavioral variation. In this way, what appears to be a shared environmental influence, would actually load into the E component of a biometric model and be interpreted as a source of the nonshared environment. Not only has this particular interpretation been supported in the extant literature (for a systematic review of the literature see Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000), it also seems to possess strong face validity. To assume that two individuals, even those who are genetically identical, would interpret an environmental influence as complex as poverty in the exact same way seems quite unlikely.

The third conclusion that can be drawn from the results centers on gene-environment interplay. As was discussed in detail in Chapter 2, a substantial portion of the literature has effectively demonstrated that genetic and environmental influences cannot be cleanly separated from one another (Beaver, 2013; Walsh, 2011). Rather, genetic influences appear to be moderated by environmental influences (Purcell, 2002) and environmental influences seem to be influenced by genes to some degree (Jaffee & Price, 2007; Kendler & Baker, 2007; Scarr &

McCartney, 1983). The results of the current dissertation also directly reflect this reality. While there were no pervasive or highly robust G × Es identified in the analyses, several significant moderating effects were detected. Modeling G × Es, at least in a latent framework, is still a relatively novel approach and statistical methods are still being developed and refined to allow for this type of modeling strategy (Brendgen et al., 2012; Johnson, 2007; Plomin et al., 2013).

With these considerations in mind, it appears that a significant number of the concepts identified in conventional criminological theories may only provide weak or inconsistent direct effects on criminological outcomes once genetic influences are controlled, but such factors may significantly moderate genetic influences. Precisely which environmental influences significantly moderate genetic influences and in what ways remains an open empirical question

210 and illustrates the need for additional research. In short, the findings of the current dissertation indicate that the concepts specified in some of the examined theoretical perspectives may contribute to the etiological development of antisocial behavior, but likely not in the ways such theories tend to indicate.

7.2 Limitations

As with any empirical investigation, the current dissertation is not free from methodological limitations and should be viewed with caution. Five limitations in particular warrant additional attention and explanation. First, the current dissertation only examined a limited number of theoretical perspectives and the examination of additional perspectives may provide a better understanding of the role of conventional criminological theory within the biosocial perspective. The theories examined in the current dissertation were selected using three distinct criteria: (1) all four constitute some of the most favored theories among criminologists based on the most recent data (Cooper et al., 2010); (2) comprehensive meta- analyses or literature reviews have been performed on all examined theories within the past 10 years; and (3) appropriate measures were present in the available data. This does not necessarily mean that the examination of additional theories which meet other criteria will yield similar results. For example, the examined theories focus on individual-level processes18 which result in antisocial behavior. Theories which focus on macro-level processes, such as neighborhood-level influences (e.g., Sampson et al., 1997), may result in a different pattern of findings. For this reason, future research would benefit from taking a more expanded approach and examining additional theoretical perspectives.

18 An exception would be Merton’s (1934) strain theory which has been conceptualized as a multilevel theory with both individual- and macro-level components (Baumer, 2007). Directly in line with the majority of the extant literature (Pratt & Cullen, 2005), classic strain theory was examined as an individual-level theory in the current dissertation. 211

The second limitation of the current dissertation is related to some of the measures employed. More specifically, the Add Health did not contain variables tapping Hirschi’s (1969) conceptualization of the social bond of belief. Other social bonds were measured in several different ways tapping various dimensions of each bond in an effort to fully explore any potential association between each bond and antisocial behavior and substance use. For example, several sources of attachment were examined including parents, schools, neighborhoods, and peers in an effort to fully examine the influence of the overall bond of attachment on antisocial behavior.

Despite this use of a diverse set of social bond measures, it was not possible to examine the potential effects of belief on the examined outcomes due to data limitations within the Add

Health. Directly related to this issue was the strategy used to measure antisocial behavior.

While the Add Health includes an index tapping multiple antisocial behaviors at all four waves, measures based on official records (such as arrest) are not available. While self-report measures have been found to be valid and reliable (Kirk, 2006; Piquero, Farrington, & Blumstein, 2003;

Thornberry & Krohn, 2000), future research may benefit from examining other indicators of antisocial behavior including official records.

Third, and directly in line with the previous limitation, the Add Health contains an impressive amount of information on a large sample of youth, however, the data are limited in ways that are meaningful in regard to the current dissertation. For example, the Add Health does not seem to contain many chronic, violent offenders. Based on the nationally representative nature of the data, and the scarcity of such offenders in the general population, this particular limitation is not all that surprising. Despite such individuals being relatively rare in the general population, previous studies have indicated that they can account for the majority of crime in a specific geographic area (for a well-known example see Wolfgang, Figlio, & Sellin, 1972). In

212 this way, examining additional samples which contain an adequate number of chronic offenders may reveal patterns that could not be detected in the current dissertation. Similarly, the current dissertation relied upon a sample of twins and sibling pairs as opposed to singletons. While a recent study has revealed that findings flowing from the twin subsample of the Add Health are not significantly different from the nationally representative sample of non-twin respondents

(Barnes & Boutwell, 2013), the generalizability of the sibling subsample has not yet been explored making it is unclear whether the results of the current study will generalize to any larger population.

The fourth limitation of the current dissertation is directly related to the behavior genetic modeling strategies used. As discussed in Chapter 2, behavior genetic modeling strategies employ statistical models which estimate genetic and environmental influences on a given behavioral outcome as latent factors. In this way, it is possible to determine the overall effect of each set of influences on a given phenotype, but it is not possible to determine the precise genes and environments which constitute such influences. The DF equations employed in the current dissertation were aimed at identifying some of the nonshared environmental influences involved in the underlying etiology of the examined outcomes, but much more work needs to be done to identify a more precise and comprehensive list of nonshared environments involved in the etiology of antisocial behavior. In addition, the Add Health contains information on a limited number of genetic polymorphisms, but this information is extremely limited and must be expanded in an effort to better understand the molecular genetic processes which underlie the latent heritability estimates provided in the current study.

Finally, the G × E modeling strategy employed was limited in the sense that it relied upon dichotomous moderating measures which required to use of somewhat arbitrary cut points and

213 the creation of dummy indicator variables. While this modeling strategy has been used previously (Asbury et al., 2005; Beaver, 2011), it does not allow for the direct examination of the potential moderating effects of continuous measures. Previous studies have provided in-depth descriptions of additional G × E modeling strategies (Brendgen et al., 2012; Johnson, 2007) but such strategies are relatively new and possess additional limitations. Gene-environment interplay seems to play an important role in the development of antisocial behavior, but the ways in which genetic and environmental factors interact with one another are highly complex and developing statistical models which can adequately capture such intricacies should remain a priority.

7.3 Future Directions

Taken together, these findings seem to indicate that the examined theoretical perspectives are, in the words of Nagin (2007), “woefully incomplete.” While some of the concepts stemming from these theories are significantly associated with criminogenic outcomes, even after controlling for genetic influences, such associations are rather weak and the theoretical explanations of the underlying the mechanisms of such associations are likely misspecified. In this way, conventional theories, at least in their current form, are simply nothing more than a philosophical exercise aimed at solving the puzzle of human behavior with only a limited number of pieces. While the findings flowing from the current project do not necessarily indicate that the examined theories should be completely discarded, these findings do indicate that the manner in which such theories are presented should be discarded. More specifically, some of the concepts specified in extant theories seem to play some sort of role in the development of antisocial behavior (e.g., TRDM), but the processes specified in the accompanying theories are quite likely misspecified. This conclusion is firmly supported by the 214 results of the univariate decomposition models which indicated that over 80 percent of the measures examined in the current dissertation were under some sort of genetic influence. This finding clearly indicates that criminological theories which fail to acknowledge genetic influences on behavioral outcomes are inadequate. With these findings in mind, four distinct suggestions for future theoretical development flow directly from the results of the current dissertation.

First, criminologists should aim to use genetically informed research designs in an effort to avoid model misspecification and the identification of spurious associations. As illustrated in

Chapter 2, the paucity of studies which employ genetically sensitive research designs within

Criminology, the flagship journal of the American Society of Criminology, is alarming and unintuitive. Most other fields which aim to explain behavioral phenomena are moving in the opposite direction, with genetically sensitive research designs becoming the rule as opposed to the exception (for an overview within the field of psychology see Duncan, Popllastri, & Smoller,

2014). In addition, criminologists, just like other social scientists, frequently rely upon quasi- experimental designs to test hypotheses and are constantly striving to employ more robust and appropriate research methods in an effort to detect causal effects. As outlined in Chapter 2, behavior genetic research designs are widely considered some of the most robust and conservative quasi-experimental tests of environmental influences (Johnson et al., 2009; McGue et al., 2010). In addition, previous studies have revealed that results stemming from SSSMs can be incorrect and highly misleading (for an example see Harden et al., 2008).

Second, the results of the current dissertation do not indicate that genes are the only factor that influences behavioral phenotypes, but they do indicate that genes are intricately involved in the underlying developmental process. In this way, future criminological theory

215 should strive to embrace the role of biological and genetic influences. In addition, the role of biology should extend beyond genetically informed quantitative modeling strategies. Rather, future theorizing and theoretical development should include neurological, genetic, biochemical, and other biological factors in an attempt to more fully explain antisocial behavior and its correlates. Third, and along the same lines, future theorizing should focus on nonshared, as opposed to shared, environmental influences. As more and more evidence begins to accumulate

(Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000), it appears that focusing on shared environments as meaningful and consistent influences on antisocial phenotypes is becoming untenable. As is clearly evident in the extant literature, and from the results of the current study, shared environmental influences have little to no influence on most behavioral outcomes making a continued focus on such factors within criminological theory superfluous. A more concentrated focus on nonshared environmental influences, in tandem with an intensified focus on biological influences, will likely result in better theories and more effective treatment. Fourth, and finally, future research should continue to explore the many ways in which genes and environments work collectively to influence behavioral phenotypes. Gene-environment interplay is highly complex, but likely holds the key for more informed and powerful theory.

To sum up the findings of the current dissertation as succinctly as possible: criminology needs biology. Without biology, our models are misspecified, our theories incomplete, and our research largely unnecessary. This does not have to be the case, criminology can progress and formulate powerful theoretical perspectives, conduct impactful research, and develop effective and humane treatment strategies (Cullen, 2009). The biosocial perspective offers the tools required to reach these goals. However, it should be noted that simply acknowledging the role of biology in the etiology of behavior is only a very small first step. Even engaging and ultimately

216 achieving the suggestions offered above would only constitute a few steps in the many miles that must be traveled to reach these ultimate goals. The journey facing the field of criminology is difficult and will require criminologists to challenge the very foundations of conventional criminology. In this way, the field of criminology currently finds itself in a Kuhnian crisis

(1996) in the sense that a consistent set of findings which do not align with the current sociological paradigm are emerging. Furthermore, this pattern of findings has begun to result in a “blurring of a paradigm and the consequent loosening of the rules for normal research” (Kuhn,

1996, p. 84). This blurring of the current paradigm can result in a tumultuous time within a given discipline, but it also opens the preverbal door for revolutionary science. This process, in turn, naturally leads to the development of new theories. According to Kuhn (1996), “[s]ince new paradigms are born from old ones, they ordinarily incorporate much of the vocabulary and apparatus…that the traditional paradigm had previously employed. But they seldom employ these borrowed elements in quite the traditional way” (p. 149). This is where the field of criminology currently finds itself. New pieces of the puzzle have been discovered, but it currently remains unclear how such pieces fit within the existing picture. While much work remains to be done, it is quite clear that the payoff will be worthwhile.

217

Table 7.1: Tabulation of Significant Associations between Theoretical Measures and Antisocial Outcomes Organized by Theory and Model Estimated Antisocial Behavior Alcohol Use Illicit Drug use Totals Percentages SSSM DF Model SSSM DF Model SSSM DF Model SSSM DF Model SSSM DF Model Rational Choice 4 3 4 2 3 1 11 6 100% 55% Number of Models 4 4 3 11 Social Learning 9 2 10 2 8 2 27 6 82% 18% Number of Models 12 12 9 33 Classic Strain 1 2 3 1 0 0 4 3 19% 14% Number of Models 8 8 5 21 Social Bonding 33 25 34 15 22 13 89 53 71% 42% Number of Models 46 46 33 125 Total: 47 32 51 20 33 16 131 68 69% 36% 70 70 50 190 Note: Tabulations reflect number of models which identified associations significant at the p ≤ .10 level.

218

80% 73% 69% 70% 67% 66%

60%

50% 46%

40% 36% 32% 29% 30%

20%

10% Percentage of Significant Percentage of Significant Associations

0% Antisocial Behavior Alcohol Use Illicit Drug use Overall Examined Outcome Measure

SSSM DF Model

Figure 7.1: Visual Representation of the Percentage of All Significant Associations by Model Estimated

219

APPENDIX A

ITEMS FOR ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR MEASURES

Wave 1 and 2 Antisocial Behavior Indexes

1. In the past 12 months, how often did you paint graffiti or signs on someone else’s

property or in a public place?

2. In the past 12 months, how often did you deliberately damage property that didn’t belong

to you?

3. In the past 12 months, how often did you lie to your parents or guardians about where

you had been or whom you were with?

4. In the past 12 months, how often did you take something from a store without paying for

it?

5. In the past 12 months, how often did you get into a serious physical fight?

6. In the past 12 months, how often did you hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or

care from a doctor or nurse?

7. In the past 12 months, how often did you run away from home?

8. In the past 12 months, how often did you drive a car without the owner’s permission?

9. In the past 12 months, how often did you steal something worth more than $50?

10. In the past 12 months, how often did you go into a building or house to steal something?

11. In the past 12 months, how often did you use or threaten to use a weapon to get

something from someone?

12. In the past 12 months, how often did you sell marijuana or other drugs?

13. In the past 12 months, how often did you steal something worth less than $50?

220

14. In the past 12 months, how often did you take part in a fight where a group of your

friends was against another group?

15. In the past 12 months, how often did you act loud, rowdy, or unruly in a public place?

16. Have you ever carried a weapon at school?

17. Have you ever used a weapon in a fight?

Wave 3 and 4 Antisocial Behavior Indexes

1. In the past 12 months, how often did you damage property that didn’t belong to you?

2. In the past 12 months, how often did you steal something worth more than $50?

3. In the past 12 months, how often did you go into a house or building to steal something?

4. In the past 12 months, how often did you use or threaten to use a weapon to get

something you wanted?

5. In the past 12 months, how often did you sell marijuana or other drugs?

6. In the past 12 months, how often did you steal something worth less than $50?

7. In the past 12 months, how often did you get into a serious physical fight?

8. In the past 12 months, how often did you buy, sell, or hold stolen property?

9. In the past 12 months, how often did you use someone else’s credit card without their

permission?

10. In the past 12 months, how often did you deliberately write a bad check?

11. In the past 12 months, how often did you use a weapon in a fight?

12. In the past 12 months, how often did you carry a handgun to school or work?

221

APPENDIX B

UNIVARIATE ACE AND THRESHOLD LIABILITY MODEL RESULTS

Table B.1: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1 and Wave 2 Antisocial Behavior Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Antisocial Behavior ACE .44** .01 .54** 33.19 .76 .93 .04 (.24-.65) (-.10-12) (.43-.66) CE .00 .24** .76** 48.48 17.67** .60 .90 .05 (.00-.00) (.19-.28) (.72-.81) AE .46** .00 .54** 33.72 .04 .76 .94 .04 (.38-.55) (.00-.00) (.45-.62) E .00 .00 1.00** 119.81 127.41** .00 .73 .08 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Antisocial Behavior ACE .42** .00 .58** 60.26 .25 .80 .06 (.18-.66) (-.11-.11) (.44-.72) CE .00 .20** .80** 68.97 12.08** .14 .78 .06 (.00-.00) (.16-.24) (.76-.84) AE .42** .00 .58** 61.61 .00 .25 .81 .06 (.32-.52) (.00-.00) (.48-.68) E .00 .00 1.00** 115.55 86.73** .00 .64 .08 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

222

Table B.2: Threshold Liability Model Results for the Wave 3 and Wave 4 Antisocial Behavior Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W3 Antisocial Behavior ACE .36* .07 .58** 13.04 .90 .95 .03 (.08-.64) (-.10-.23) (.43-.72) CE .00 .26** .74** 19.38 6.31** .83 .93 .04 (.00-.00) (.20-.32) (.68-.80) AE .46** .00 .54** 13.63 .61 .91 .96 .03 (.36-.57) (.00-.00) (.43-.64) E .00 .00 1.00** 87.19 73.65** .00 .58 .09 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W4 Antisocial Behavior ACE .28 .08 .64** 59.07 .00 .22 .08 (-.09-.65) (-.12-.28) (.44-.84) CE .00 .22** .78** 61.13 2.17 .00 .31 .08 (.00-.00) (.14-.29) (.71-.86) AE .41** .00 .59** 59.52 .56 .00 .33 .08 (.28-.55) (.00-.00) (.45-.72) E .00 .00 1.00** 95.03 35.90** .00 .03 .09 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. No clear best fitting model for the Wave 4 Antisocial behavior measure. All models estimated using a weighted least squares estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

223

Table B.3: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1 and Wave 2 Alcohol Use Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Alcohol Use ACE .59** .00 .41** 11.66 1.00 1.00 .01 (.52-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.48) CE .00 .29** .71** 80.48 247.13** .68 .92 .04 (.00-.00) (.26-.33) (.67-.74) AE .59** .00 .41** 12.72 .00 1.00 1.00 .01 (.52-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.48) E .00 .00 1.00** 296.01 -- .00 .69 .12 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Alcohol Use ACE .59** .00 .41** 10.80 1.00 1.00 .00 (.51-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.49) CE .00 .30** .70** 69.91 248.81** .70 .92 .06 (.00-.00) (.26-.34) (.67-.74) AE .59** .00 .41** 11.79 .02 1.00 1.00 .00 (.51-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.49) E .00 .00 1.00** 281.16 308.20** .00 .68 .12 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

224

Table B.4: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 3 and Wave 4 Alcohol Use Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W3 Alcohol Use ACE .32** .14** .54** 43.49 .85 .96 .05 (.13-.52) (.04-.25) (.44-.63) CE .00 .31** .69** 55.09 10.46** .79 .95 .05 (.00-.00) (.28-.35) (.65-.72) AE .53** .00 .47** 52.39 6.75** .81 .95 .05 (.47-.59) (.00-.00) (.41-.53) E .00 .00 1.00** 327.62 314.04** .00 .65 .14 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W4 Alcohol Use ACE .32** .09 † .59** 14.05 .98 1.00 .02 (.15-.50) (-.01-.19) (.50-.68) CE .00 .26** .74** 32.10 12.90** .88 .97 .04 (.00-.00) (.22-.29) (.71-.78) AE .45** .00 .55** 18.07 2.99 † .96 .99 .02 (.39-.52) (.00-.00) (.48-.61) E .00 .00 1.00** 235.46 203.55** .00 .69 .11 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

225

Table B.5: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Wave 1, Wave 2, and Wave 3 Illicit Drug Use Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Drug Use ACE .55** .00 .46** 23.25 .93 .98 .03 (.46-.63) (.00-.00) (.37-.54) CE .00 .26** .74** 61.67 165.74** .72 .93 .05 (.00-.00) (.21-.31) (.69-.79) AE .55** .00 .46** 25.36 .00 .92 .98 .03 (.46-.63) (.00-.00) (.37-.54) E .00 .00 1.00** 153.21 -- .20 .82 .09 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Drug Use ACE .29** .07 .65** 17.77 .93 .98 .02 (.09-.48) (-.04-.18) (.54-.76) CE .00 .21** .79** 23.72 7.85** .88 .97 .03 (.00-.00) (.17-.26) (.74-.84) AE .39** .00 .61** 19.29 1.41 .93 .98 .02 (.31-.47) (.00-.00) (.53-.69) E .00 .00 1.00** 87.24 85.63** .26 .83 .07 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W3 Drug Use ACE .37** .04 .58** 9.24 1.00 1.01 .00 (.17-.57) (-.07-.16) (.48-.69) CE .00 .24** .76** 21.55 13.50** .90 .98 .03 (.00-.00) (.19-.29) (.71-.81) AE .44** .00 .56** 9.81 .55 1.00 1.01 .00 (.36-.52) (.00-.00) (.48-.64) E .00 .00 1.00** 103.09 117.77** .06 .78 .07 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

226

Table B.6: Univariate ACE Model Results for the Thoughtfully-Reflective Decision Making (TRDM) Measure A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 TRDM ACE .07 .09* .84** 1.18 1.00 1.05 .00 (-.08-.17) (.01-.17) (.76-.92) CE .00 .13** .88** 1.95 .80 1.00 1.04 .00 (.00-.00) (.10-.16) (.84-.91) AE .21** .00 .79** 5.62 4.70** 1.00 1.03 .00 (.16-.27) (.00-.00) (.73-.85) E .00 .00 1.00** 59.12 62.99** .22 .82 .05 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

227

Table B.7: Univariate ACE Model Results for Peer Antisocial Behavior, Peer Illicit Drug Use, and Peer Alcohol Use Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Peer Delinquency ACE .00 .21** .80** 24.65 .00 .52 .04 (.00-.00) (.08-.33) (.67-.92) CE .00 .21** .80** 26.89 .11 .00 .52 .04 (.00-.00) (.08-.33) (.67-.92) AE .24* .00 .76** 30.37 11.10** .00 .41 .05 (.01-.48) (.00-.00) (.53-.99) E .00 .00 1.00** 33.82 12.59** .00 .38 .05 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W1 Peer Drug Use ACE .00 .24** .76** 7.12 1.00 1.05 .00 (.00-.00) (.15-.33) (.68-.85) CE .00 .24** .76** 7.76 .00 1.00 1.05 .00 (.00-.00) (.15-.33) (.68-.85) AE .31** .00 .69** 11.90 28.92** 1.00 1.00 .00 (.17-.44) (.00-.00) (.56-.83) E .00 .00 1.00** 27.51 -- .28 .84 .04 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W1 Peer Alcohol Use ACE .00 .32** .68** 25.08 .64 .90 .04 (.00-.00) (.23-.41) (.59-.77) CE .00 .32** .68** 27.36 .02 .61 .90 .04 (.00-.00) (.23-.41) (.59-.77) AE .42** .00 .58** 43.49 51.85 .20 .80 .06 (.27-.57) (.00-.00) (.43-.73) E .00 .00 1.00** 85.40 52.90 .00 .57 .09 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

228

Table B.8: Univariate ACE Model Results for Classic Strain Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Aspirations/Expectations ACE .10 .06 .84** 22.57 .68 .91 .03 (-.07-.26) (-.03-.15) (.75-.93) CE .00 .11** .89** 23.98 1.35 .67 .92 .03 (.00-.00) (.08-.14) (.86-.92) AE .19** .00 .81** 24.65 1.93 .66 .91 .03 (.13-.26) (.00-.00) (.74-.87) E .00 .00 1.00** 53.32 46.38** .00 .75 .00 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Aspirations/Expectations ACE .26** .00 .74** 29.87 .42 .84 .04 (.18-.33) (.00-.00) (.67-.82) CE .00 .12** .88** 42.32 43.61** .06 .77 .04 (.00-.00) (.07-.16) (.84-.93) AE .26** .00 .74** 32.59 .00 .36 .84 .04 (.18-.33) (.00-.00) (.67-.82) E .00 .00 1.00** 66.93 -- .00 .62 .06 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Expectations/Realizations ACE .29** .00 .71** 52.71 .33 .82 .06 (.22-.36) (.00-.00) (.65-.78) CE .00 .15** .85** 69.86 70.21** .06 .77 .06 (.00-.00) (.11-.19) (.81-.89) AE .29** .00 .71** 57.50 .08 .26 .82 .06 (.22-.36) (.00-.00) (.65-.78) E .00 .00 1.00** 126.82 70.74** .00 .58 .09 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

229

Table B.9: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Parental Attachment Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Parental Attachment ACE .53** .00 .47** 53.15 .58 .88 .06 (.44-.62) (.00-.00) (.38-.56) CE .00 .26** .74** 77.11 133.18** .35 .84 .07 (.00-.00) (.22-.31) (.69-.79) AE .53** .00 .47** 57.98 .00 .54 .88 .06 (.44-.62) (.00-.00) (.38-.56) E .00 .00 1.00** 171.16 146.47** .00 .63 .11 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Parental Attachment ACE .59** .00 .41** 31.97 .86 .96 .05 (.51-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.49) CE .00 .29** .71** 71.28 232.48** .60 .90 .07 (.00-.00) (.25-.34) (.66-.75) AE .59** .00 .41** 34.88 .00 .85 .96 .05 (.51-.66) (.00-.00) (.34-.49) E .00 .00 1.00** 195.66 -- .00 .71 .12 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

230

Table B.10: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 School Attachment Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 School Attachment ACE .44** .00 .56** 29.54 .92 .98 .04 (.39-.49) (.00-.00) (.51-.61) CE .00 .22** .78** 92.13 259.15** .64 .91 .07 (.00-.00) (.19-.26) (.74-.81) AE .44** .00 .56** 32.23 .00 .91 .98 .04 (.39-.49) (.00-.00) (.51-.61) E .00 .00 1.00** 283.31** -- .00 .72 .12 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 School Attachment ACE .42** .00 .58** 22.35 .92 .98 .03 (.33-.49) (.00-.00) (.51-.65) CE .00 .21** .79** 73.20 135.83** .57 .89 .06 (.00-.00) (.17-.25) (.75-.83) AE .42** .00 .58** 24.38 .00 .91 .98 .03 (.33-.49) (.00-.00) (.51-.65) E .00 .00 1.00** 185.60 -- .00 .72 .10 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

231

Table B.11: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Neighborhood Attachment Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Neighborhood Attachment ACE .34** .16** .50** 52.78 .88 .97 .05 (.20-.48) (.08-.23) (.43-.58) CE .00 .33** .67** 77.64 23.84** .81 .95 .06 (.00-.00) (.30-.36) (.64-.71) AE .56** .00 .44** 72.07 16.31** .83 .96 .06 (.51-.61) (.00-.00) (.39-.49) E .00 .00 1.00** 447.73 443.44** .00 .71 .15 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Neighborhood Attachment ACE .34** .18** .49** 92.09 .75 .93 .07 (.19-.48) (.09-.26) (.41-.57) CE .00 .34** .66** 112.11 21.11** .69 .92 .08 (.00-.00) (.31-.38) (.62-.69) AE .58** .00 .42** 113.59 17.75** .69 .92 .08 (.53-.64) (.00-.00) (.36-.47) E .00 .00 1.00** 485.30 405.37** .00 .66 .17 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

232

Table B.12: Threshold Liability Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Peer Attachment Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Peer Attachment ACE .27** .00 .74** .48 1.00 1.04 .00 (.21-.32) (.00-.00) (.68-.79) CE .00 .14** .86** 12.84 79.03** .92 .97 .02 (.00-.00) (.11-.18) (.83-.89) AE .27** .00 .74** .48 .00 1.00 1.04 .00 (.27-.32) (.00-.00) (.68-.79) E .00 .00 1.00** 80.12 79.03** .06 .65 .08 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Peer Attachment ACE .33** .00 .67** 13.82 .93 .97 .03 (.27-.39) (.00-.00) (.61-.73) CE .00 .17** .83** 44.15 110.58** .66 .86 .06 (.00-.00) (.14-.21) (.79-.87) AE .33** .00 .67** 15.45 .00 .92 .97 .03 (.27-.39) (.00-.00) (.61-.73) E .00 .00 1.00** 126.50 110.58** .00 .60 .11 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a weighted least squares estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

233

Table B.13: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Parental Involvement Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Parental Involvement ACE .39** .12* .50** 26.96 .94 .98 .04 (.23-.55) (.02-.21) (.42-.58) CE .00 .33** .67** 53.44 23.04** .84 .96 .06 (.00-.00) (.29-.36) (.64-.71) AE .55** .00 .45** 33.86 5.75* .92 .98 .04 (.49-.61) (.00-.00) (.40-.51) E .00 .00 1.00** 343.06 358.34** .00 .71 .16 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Parental Involvement ACE .42** .06 .52** 15.38 .98 .99 .02 (.27-.58) (-.04-.16) (.45-.59) CE .00 .29** .71** 40.69 29.06** .86 .97 .05 (.00-.00) (.25-.33) (.68-.75) AE .51** .00 .49** 16.77 1.37 .98 .99 .02 (.45-.56) (.00-.00) (.44-.55) E .00 .00 1.00** 257.12 299.24** .00 .73 .14 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

234

Table B.14: Threshold Liability Model Results for Wave 3 Community Involvement Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W3 Community Involvement ACE .38** .10 .53** 30.34 .83 .91 .06 (.15-.61) (-.04-.23) (.41-.64) CE .00 .31** .69** 40.93 10.51** .76 .90 .06 (.00-.00) (.26-.36) (.64-.74) AE .53** .00 .47** 32.16 1.85 .82 .92 .05 (.44-.61) (.00-.00) (.39-.56) E .00 .00 1.00 175.09 143.94** .00 .56 .13 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting models in bold. All models estimated using a weighted least squares estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

235

Table B.15: Univariate ACE Model Results for Wave 1 and Wave 2 Religious Commitment Measures A C E χ2 Δχ2 CFI TLI RMSEA W1 Religious Commitment ACE .43** .39** .19** 14.97 1.00 1.00 .02 (.35-.51) (.33-.45) (.15-.22) CE .00 .61** .39** 127.27 104.85** .87 .97 .09 (.00-.00) (.59-.64) (.36-.41) AE .84** .00 .16** 217.83 165.49** .76 .94 .12 (.82-.87) (.00-.00) (.13-.18) E .00 .00 1.00 1534.68 3478.64** .00 .60 .30 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) W2 Religious Commitment ACE .32** .39** .29** 15.15 .99 1.00 .02 (.20-.44) (.32-.47) (.23-.35) CE .00 .56** .44** 57.00 25.87** .92 .98 .06 (.00-.00) (.53-.59) (.41-.47) AE .77** .00 .23** 180.53 106.80** .69 .92 .11 (.73-.81) (.00-.00) (.19-.28) E .00 .00 1.00 1018.38 1502.18** .00 .58 .25 (.00-.00) (.00-.00) (1.00-1.00) Best fitting model in bold. All models estimated using a maximum-likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. †p ≤ .10; *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01

236

APPENDIX C

IRB APPROVAL FORMS

237

238

The University of Michigan, ICPSR DSDR National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health Restricted Use Data Contract

Attachment D: Security Pledge

Pledge of Confidentiality

,through my involvement with and work on my project TYPE OR PRINT YOUR NAME will have access to Sensitive Data collected by the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health). By virtue of my affiliation with this project, I have access to Sensitive Data about respondents generally perceived as personal and private. I understand that access to this Sensitive Data carries with it a responsibility to guard against unauthorized use and to abide by the Sensitive Data Security Plan. To treat information as confidential means to not divulge it to anyone who is not a project member, or cause it to be accessible to anyone who is not a project member. Anything not specifically named as "public information" is considered confidential.

Disclosing confidential information from Add Health directly or allowing non-authorized access to such information may subject you to criminal prosecution and/or civil recovery and may violate the code of research ethics of your institution.

I agree to fulfill my responsibilities on this project in accordance with the following guidelines:

1. I agree not to permit non-project personnel access to these Sensitive Data, in either electronic or paper copy.

2. I agree to not attempt to identify individuals, families, households, schools, or institutions.

3. I agree that in the event the identity of an individual, family, household, school, or institution is discovered inadvertently, I will (a) make no use of this knowledge, (b) advise the Investigator of the incident who will report it to Russel S. Hathaway within one (1) business day of discovery, (c) safeguard or destroy the information as directed by the Investigator after consultation with Russel S. Hathaway, and (d) not inform any other person of the discovered identity.

Updates and to the Add Health data and codebooks will only be distributed through the Add Health list server.

EMAIL: PROVIDE YOUR EMAIL ADDRESS TO SUBSCRIBE TO THIS LIST SERVER

18

239

REFERENCES

Agnew, R. (1985). Social control theory and delinquency: A longitudinal test. Criminology, 23, 47-61.

Agnew, R. (1991). A longitudinal test of social control theory and delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 28, 126-156.

Agnew, R. (1992). Foundation for a general strain theory of crime and delinquency. Criminology, 30, 47-88.

Agnew, R. (2011). Toward a unified criminology: Integrating assumptions about crime, people, and society. New York, NY: New York University Press.

Agnew, R., Brezina, T., Wright, J. P., & Cullen, F. T. (2002). Strain, personality traits, and delinquency: Extending general strain theory. Criminology, 40, 43-72.

Agnew, R., Cullen, F. T., Burton Jr, V. S., Evans, T. D., & Dunaway, R. G. (1996). A new test of classic strain theory. Justice Quarterly, 13, 681-704.

Akers, R. L. (1973). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Akers, R. L. (1977). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Akers, R. L. (1985). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach, (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Akers, R. L. (1998). Social learning social structure: A general theory of crime and deviance. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press.

Akers, R. L., & Cochran, J. K. (1985). Adolescent marijuana use: A test of three theories of deviant behavior. Deviant Behavior, 6, 323-346.

Akers, R. L., & Jensen, G. F. (2006). The empirical status of social learning theory of crime and deviance: The past, present, and future. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins (Eds.), Taking stock: The status of criminological theory (pp. 37-76). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Akers, R. L. & Lee, G. (1996). A longitudinal test of social learning theory: Adolescent smoking. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 317-343.

Akers, R. L. & Sellers, C. S. (2013). Criminological theories: Introduction, evaluation, and application, (6th ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press

Anderson, E. (1999). The code of the street: Violence, decency and the moral life of the inner city. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. 240

Anderson, L. S., Chiricos, T. G., & Waldo, G. P. (1977). Formal and informal sanctions: A comparison of deterrent effects. Social Problems, 25, 103-114.

Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2010). The psychology of criminal conduct (5th ed.). New Providence, NJ: Elsevier.

Andrews, K. H., & Kandel, D. B. (1979). Attitude and behavior: A specification of the contingent consistency hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 44, 298-310.

Anwar, S., & Loughran, T. A. (2011). Testing a Bayesian learning theory of deterrence among serious juvenile offenders. Criminology, 49(3), 667-698.

Asbury, K., Dunn, J. F., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2003). Nonshared environmental influences on individual differences in early behavioral development: A monozygotic twin difference study. Child Development, 74, 933-943.

Asbury, K., Wachs, T. D., & Plomin, R. (2005). Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intelligence, 33, 643-661.

Baier, C. J., & Wright, B. R. (2001). “If you love me, keep my commandments”: A meta- analysis of the effect of religion on crime. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 38, 3-21.

Baker, J. H., Maes, H. H., & Kendler, K. S. (2012). Shared environmental contributions to substance use. Behavior Genetics, 42, 345-353.

Barkow, J. H. (2006). Introduction: Sometimes the bus does wait. In J. H. Barkow (Ed.), Missing the revolution: Darwinism for social scientists (pp. 3-62). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Barnes, J. C. (2014). The impact of biosocial criminology on public policy: Where should we go from here? In M. DeLisi & K. M. Beaver (Eds.), Criminological theory: A life-course approach (2nd ed.). Burlington, MA: Jones and Bartlett

Barnes, J. C., & Beaver, K. M. (2012). Marriage and desistance from crime: A consideration of gene–environment correlation. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74, 19-33.

Barnes, J. C., Beaver, K. M., & Boutwell, B. B. (2011). Examining the genetic underpinnings to Moffitt’s developmental : A behavioral genetic analysis. Criminology, 49, 923- 954.

Barnes, J. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2013). A demonstration of the generalizability of twin-based research on antisocial behavior. Behavior Genetics, 43, 120-131.

241

Battin, S. R., Hill, K. G., Abbott, R. D., Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (1998). The contribution of gang membership to delinquency beyond delinquent friends. Criminology, 36, 93-116.

Baumer, E. P. (2007). Untangling research puzzles in Merton's multilevel anomie theory. Theoretical Criminology, 11, 63-93.

Beauchamp, J. P., Cesarini, D., Johannesson, M., Lindqvist, E., & Apicella, C. (2011). On the sources of the height–intelligence correlation: New insights from a bivariate ACE model with assortative mating. Behavior Genetics, 41, 242-252.

Beaver, K. M. (2008). Nonshared environmental influences on adolescent delinquent involvement and adult criminal behavior. Criminology, 46, 341-369.

Beaver, K. M. (2009). Molecular genetics and crime. In A. Walsh & K. M. Beaver (Eds.), Biosocial criminology: New directions in theory and research. New York, NY: Routledge.

Beaver, K. M. (2011). Genetic influences on being processed through the criminal justice system: Results from a sample of adoptees. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 282-287.

Beaver, K. M. (2011). Environmental moderators of genetic influences on adolescent delinquent involvement and victimization. Journal of Adolescent Research, 26, 84-114.

Beaver, K. M. (2013a). Biosocial criminology: A primer (2nd ed.). Kendall Hunt Publishing Company.

Beaver, K. M. (2013b). The familial concentration and transmission of crime. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 40, 139-155.

Beaver, K. M., Barnes, J. C., May, J. S., & Schwartz, J. A. (2011). Psychopathic personality traits, genetic risk, and gene-environment correlations. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38, 896-912.

Beaver, K. M., DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., & Barnes, J. C. (2010). Monoamine oxidase A genotype is associated with gang membership and weapon use. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 51, 130-134.

Beccaria, C. (1963 [1764]). On crimes and punishments. Translated with an introduction by H. Paolucci. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Becker, H. S. (1963). Outsiders: Studies in the sociology of deviance. New York, NY: Free Press.

Bernard, T. J., & Snipes, J. B. (1996). Theoretical integration in criminology. Crime and Justice, 20, 301-348.

242

Bernard, T. J., Snipes, J. B., & Gerould, A. L. (2010). Vold’s theoretical criminology, 6th Ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Bernat, D. H., Oakes, J. M., Pettingell, S. L., & Resnick, M. (2012). Risk and direct protective factors for youth violence: Results from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 43, S57-S66.

Bentham, J. (1948). An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. Edited with an introduction by L. J. Lafleur. New York, NY: Hafner.

Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., & Nagin, D. (1978). Deterrence and incapacitation: Estimating the effects of sanctions on the crime rate. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, 250, 223-228.

Bouchard, T. J., & McGue, M. (1990). Genetic and rearing environmental influences on adult personality: An analysis of adopted twins reared apart. Journal of Personality, 58, 263- 292.

Boutwell, B. B., Beaver, K. M., & Barnes, J. C. (2012). More alike than different: Assortative mating and antisocial propensity in adulthood. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 39, 1240- 1254.

Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., & Girard, A. (2012). Evaluating gene-environment interplay. In B. Laursen, T. D. Little, & N. A. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental research methods (pp. 687-705). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Brown, S. E., Esbensen, F. A., & Geis, G. (2013). Criminology: Explaining Crime and its Context (8th ed.). Waltham, MA: Anderson.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-159). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakefield, X. O., Ropers, H. H., & Van Oost, B. A. (1993). Abnormal behavior associated with a point in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science, 262, 578-580.

Burgess, R. L., & Akers, R. L. (1966). A differential association-reinforcement theory of criminal behavior. Social Problems, 14, 128-147.

Burkett, S. R., & Warren, B. O. (1987). Religiosity, peer associations, and adolescent marijuana use: A panel study of underlying causal structures. Criminology, 25, 109-132.

243

Burt, S. A., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Krueger, R. F. (2006). Differential parent-child relationships and adolescent externalizing symptoms: cross-lagged analyses within a monozygotic twin differences design. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1289-1298.

Burton Jr, V. S., & Cullen, F. T. (1992). The empirical status of strain theory. Journal of Crime and Justice, 15, 1-30.

Burton Jr, V. S., Cullen, F. T., Evans, T. D., & Dunaway, R. G. (1994). Reconsidering strain theory: Operationalization, rival theories, and adult criminality. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 10, 213-239.

Burton Jr, V. S., & Dunaway, R. G. (1994). Strain, relative deprivation, and middle class delinquency. In G. Barak, Varieties of criminology: Readings from a dynamic discipline (pp. 79-95). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Cadoret, R. J., Cain, C. A., & Crowe, R. R. (1983). Evidence for gene-environment interaction in the development of adolescent antisocial behavior. Behavior Genetics, 13, 301-310.

Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewart, M. A. (1996). An adoption study of drug abuse/dependency in females. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 88- 94.

Capaldi, D. M., Pears, K. C., Kerr, D. C., & Owen, L. D. (2008). Intergenerational and partner influences on fathers’ negative discipline. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 347-358.

Carlson, M. D., Mendle, J., & Harden, K. P. (2014). Early adverse environments and genetic influences on age at first sex: Evidence for gene × environment interaction. Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0035479.

Carrier, N., & Walby, K. (2014). Ptolmizing Lombroso: The pseudo-revolution of biosocial criminology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Criminology, 6, 1-45.

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., ... & Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851-854.

Catalano, R. F., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Newcomb, M. D., & Abbott, R. D. (1996). Modeling the etiology of adolescent substance use: A test of the social development model. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 429-455.

Cernkovich, S. A., & Giordano, P. C. (1992). School bonding, race, and delinquency. Criminology, 30(2), 261-291.

Chen, P., & Jacobson, K. C. (2012). Developmental trajectories of substance use from early adolescence to young adulthood: Gender and racial/ethnic differences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 154-163.

244

Cleveland, H. H., Wiebe, R. P., & Rowe, D. C. (2005). Sources of exposure to smoking and drinking friends among adolescents: a behavioral-genetic evaluation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 153-170.

Cloward, R. A., & Ohlin, L. E. (1960). Delinquency and opportunity: A theory of delinquent gangs. New York, NY: Free Press.

Cochran, J. K., & Akers, R. L. (1989). Beyond hellfire: An exploration of the variable effects of religiosity on adolescent marijuana and alcohol use. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 26, 198-225.

Cohen, A. K. (1955). Delinquent boys: The culture of the gang. New York, NY: Free Press.

Cohen, L. E., & Machalek, R. (1988). A general theory of expropriative crime: An evolutionary ecological approach. American Journal of Sociology, 94, 465-501.

Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: the case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218.

Conley, D., Rauscher, E., Dawes, C., Magnusson, P. K., & Siegal, M. L. (2013). Heritability and the equal environments assumption: evidence from multiple samples of misclassified twins. Behavior Genetics, 43, 415-426.

Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York, NY: Scribner.

Cooper, H. (2010). Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-by-step approach (4th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.

Cooper, J. A., Walsh, A., & Ellis, L. (2010). Is criminology moving toward a paradigm shift? Evidence from a survey of the American Society of Criminology. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 21, 332-347.

Costello, B. J., & Vowell, P. R. (1999). Testing control theory and differential association: A reanalysis of the Richmond Youth Project data. Criminology, 37, 815-842.

Cromwell, P. F., Olson, J. N., & D'Aunn W. A. (1991). Breaking and entering: An ethnographic analysis of burglary. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Cronk, N. J., Slutske, W. S., Madden, P. A., Bucholz, K. K., Reich, W., & Heath, A. C. (2002). Emotional and behavioral problems among female twins: An evaluation of the equal environments assumption. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 829-837.

Cullen, F. T. (2009) Preface. In A. Walsh & K. M. Beaver (Eds.), Biosocial criminology: New directions in theory and research (pp. xv-xvii). New York, NY: Routledge.

245

Cullen, F. T. (2011). Beyond adolescence-limited criminology: Choosing our future—The American Society of Criminology 2010 Sutherland Address. Criminology, 49, 287-330.

Curry, G. D., Decker, S. H., & Egley Jr, A. (2002). Gang involvement and delinquency in a middle school population. Justice Quarterly, 19, 275-292.

Czaja, R., & Blair, J. (2005). Designing surveys: A guide to decisions and procedures. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.

Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

De Haan, W., & Vos, J. (2003). A crying shame: The over-rationalized conception of man in the rational choice perspective. Theoretical Criminology, 7, 29-54.

Deater Deckard, K., & Plomin, R. (1999). An adoption study of the etiology of teacher and parent reports of externalizing behavior problems in middle childhood. Child ‐Development, 70, 144-154.

DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1985). Multiple regression analysis of twin data. Behavior Genetics, 15, 467-473.

DeLisi, M., Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & Vaughn, M. G. (2008). The etiology of criminal onset: The enduring salience of nature and nurture. Journal of Criminal Justice, 36, 217- 223.

Douglas, K., Chan, G., Gelernter, J., Arias, A. J., Anton, R. F., Poling, J., ... & Kranzler, H. R. (2011). 5-HTTLPR as a potential moderator of the effects of adverse childhood experiences on risk of antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatric Genetics, 21, 240-248.

Dugan, L., LaFree, G., & Piquero, A. R. (2005). Testing a rational choice model of airplane hijackings. Criminology, 43, 1031-1066.

Duncan, L. E., Pollastri, A. R., & Smoller, J. W. (2014). Mind the gap: Why many geneticists and psychological scientists have discrepant views about gene–environment interaction (G× E) research. American Psychologist, 69, 249-268.

Durkheim, E. (1951 [1857]). Suicide. Translated by J. A. Spaulding & G. Simpson. New York, NY: Free Press.

Eaves, L., Foley, D., & Silberg, J. (2003). Has the “equal environments” assumption been tested in twin studies? Twin Research, 6, 486-489.

Elliott, D. S., Ageton, S. S., & Canter, R. J. (1979). An integrated theoretical perspective on delinquent behavior. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 16, 3-27.

246

Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. S. (1985). Explaining delinquency and drug use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Elliot, D. S., & Menard, S. (1996). Delinquent friends and delinquent behavior: Temporal and developmental patterns. In D. Hawkins (Ed.) Current theories of crime and deviance. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

Ellis, L. (2005). A theory explaining biological correlates of criminality. European Journal of Criminology, 2, 287-315.

Ellis, L., & Walsh, A. (1999). Criminologists' opinions about causes and theories of crime and delinquency. The Criminologist, 24, 4-6.

Empey, L. T., & Stafford, M. (1991). American delinquency: Its meaning and construction. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Esbensen, F. A., & Deschenes, E. P. (1998). A multisite examination of youth gang membership: Does gender matter? Criminology, 36, 799-828.

Esbensen, F. A., & Huizinga, D. (1993). Gangs, drugs, and delinquency in a survey of urban youth. Criminology, 31, 565-589.

Evans, T. D., Cullen, F. T., Dunaway, R. G., & Burton, V. S. (1995). Religion and crime reexamined: The impact of religion, secular controls, and social ecology on adult criminality. Criminology, 33, 195-224.

Exum, M. (2002). The application and robustness of the rational choice perspective in the study of intoxicated and angry intentions to aggress. Criminology, 40, 933-966.

Fagan, J., & Wexler, S. (1987). Family origins of violent delinquents. Criminology, 25, 643-670.

Fagan, A. A., & Wright, E. M. (2011). Gender differences in the effects of exposure to intimate partner violence on adolescent violence and drug use. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35, 543- 550.

Farnworth, M., & Leiber, M. J. (1989). Strain theory revisited: Economic goals, educational means, and delinquency. American Sociological Review, 54, 263-274.

Ferguson, C. J. (2010). Genetic contributions to antisocial personality and behavior: A meta- analytic review from an evolutionary perspective. The Journal of , 150, 160-180.

Fishbein, D. H. (1990). Biological perspectives in criminology. Criminology, 28, 27-72.

Fishbein, D. H. (2001). Building bridges. ACJS Today, 17, 1-5.

247

Foley, D. L., Eaves, L. J., Wormley, B., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H., Kuhn, J., & Riley, B. (2004). Childhood adversity, Monoamine Oxidase A genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 738-744.

Fowler, J. H., Settle, J. E., & Christakis, N. A. (2011). Correlated genotypes in friendship networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1993-1997.

Fowler, T., Shelton, K., Lifford, K., Rice, F., McBride, A., Nikolov, I., ... & Van Den Bree, M. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on the relationship between peer alcohol use and own alcohol use in adolescents. Addiction, 102, 894-903.

Frazer, K. A., Murray, S. S., Schork, N. J., & Topol, E. J. (2009). Human genetic variation and its contribution to complex traits. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, 241-251.

Gallupe, O., & Baron, S. W. (2014). Morality, self-control, deterrence, and drug use: street youths and situational action theory. Crime & Delinquency, 60, 284-305.

Goldsmith, H. H. (1993). Nature-nurture issues in the behavioral genetic context: Overcoming barriers to communication. In R. Plomin & G. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 325-339). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Grasmick, H. G., & Bursik Jr, R. J. (1990). Conscience, significant others, and rational choice: Extending the deterrence model. Law & Society Review, 24, 837-861.

Grasmick, H. G., Bursik, R. J., & Arneklev, B. J. (1993). Reduction in drunk driving as a response to increased threats of shame, embarrassment, and legal sanctions. Criminology, 31, 41-67.

Gray, L. N., Ward, D. A., Stafford, M. C., & Menke, B. A. (1985). Observational and experiential effects in probability learning: the case of a deviant behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 78-85.

Green, D. E. (1989). Measures of illegal behavior in individual-level deterrence research. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 26, 253-275.

Gunderson, E. P., Tsai, A. L., Selby, J. V., Caan, B., Mayer-Davis, E. J., & Risch, N. (2006). Twins of mistaken zygosity (TOMZ): evidence for genetic contributions to dietary patterns and physiologic traits. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 540-549.

Haberstick, B. C., Lessem, J. M., Hopfer, C. J., Smolen, A., Ehringer, M. A., Timberlake, D., & Hewitt, J. K. (2005). Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) and antisocial behaviors in the presence of childhood and adolescent maltreatment. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 135, 59-64.

248

Harden, K. P., Hill, J. E., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2008). Gene-environment correlation and interaction in peer effects on adolescent alcohol and tobacco use. Behavior Genetics, 38, 339-347.

Harden, K. P., & Mendle, J. (2012). Gene-environment interplay in the association between pubertal timing and delinquency in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 73-87.

Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102: 458-489.

Harris, J. R. (2009). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Free Press.

Harris, K. M. (2011). Design features of Add Health. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina.

Harris, K. M., Halpern, C. T., Smolen, A., & Haberstick, B. C. (2006). The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) twin data. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 988-997.

Harris, K. M., Florey, F., Tabor, J., Bearman, P. S., Jones, J., & Udry, J. R. (2009). The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research Design. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina, 2003. [WWW Document] http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design.

Haynie, D. L. (2001). Delinquent peers revisited: Does network structure matter? American Journal of Sociology, 106, 1013-1057.

Haynie, D. L. (2002). Friendship networks and delinquency: The relative nature of peer delinquency. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 18, 99-134.

Haynie, D. L., & McHugh, S. (2003). Sibling deviance: In the shadows of mutual and unique friendship effects? Criminology, 41, 355-392.

Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Hirschi, T. (1979). Separate and unequal is better. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 16, 34-38.

Hirschi, T. (1989). Exploring alternatives to integrated theory. In S. F. Messner, M. D. Krohn, & A. E. Liska (Eds.), Theoretical integration in the study of deviance and crime (pp. 37-49). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

249

Hoeve, M., Stams, G. J. J., Van Der Put, C. E., Dubas, J. S., van der Laan, P. H., & Gerris, J. R. (2012). A meta-analysis of attachment to parents and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 771-785.

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1-55.

Huizinga, D., Esbensen, F. A., & Weiher, A. W. (1991). Are there multiple paths to delinquency? Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 82, 83-118.

Iervolino, A. C., Pike, A., Manke, B., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences in adolescent peer socialization: Evidence from two genetically sensitive designs. Child Development, 73, 162-174.

Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Dodge, K. A., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., & Tully, L. A. (2005). Nature × nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities interact with physical maltreatment to promote conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 67-84.

Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 44-55.

Jaffee, S. R., & Price, T. S. (2007). Gene–environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 432-442.

Jaffee, S. R., Price, T. S., & Reyes, T. M. (2013). Behavior genetics: Past, present, future. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 1225-1242.

Jang, S. J. (2002). The effects of family, school, peers, and attitudes on adolescents' drug use: Do they vary with age? Justice Quarterly, 19, 97-126.

Jang, S. J., & Franzen, A. B. (2013). Is being “spiritual” enough without being religious? A study of violent and property crimes among emerging adults. Criminology, 51, 595-627.

Johnson, B. R. (2011). More God, less crime: Why faith matters and how it could matter more. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Press.

Johnson, B. R., De Li, S., Larson, D. B., & McCullough, M. (2000). A systematic review of the religiosity and delinquency literature: A research note. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 16, 32-52.

Johnson, B. R., & Jang, S. J. (2010). Religion, race, and drug use among American youth. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion, 6, 1-22.

250

Johnson, W. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on behavior: capturing all the interplay. Psychological Review, 114, 423-440.

Johnson, W., Turkheimer, E., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (2009). Beyond heritability twin studies in behavioral research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 217-220.

Junger-Tas, J. (1992). An empirical test of social control theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 8, 9-28.

Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 633-645.

Kaplan, H. B. (1996). Empirical validity of the applicability of an integrative theory of deviant behavior to the study of drug use. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 345-377.

Keller, M. C., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., Corley, R. P., Stallings, M. C., ... & Zietsch, B. P. (2013). The genetic correlation between height and IQ: Shared genes or assortative mating? PLoS Genetics, 9, e1003451.

Kempf, K. L. (1993). The empirical status of Hirschi’s control theory. In F. Adler & W. S. Laufer (Eds.), New directions in criminological theory (Advances in Criminological Theory, Vol. 4, pp. 143-185). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Kendler, K. S. (1983). Overview: A current perspective on twin studies of schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 11, 1413-1425.

Kendler, K. S., & Baker, J. H. (2007). Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 37, 615-626.

Kendler, K. S., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Gillespie, N., Aggen, S. A., & Prescott, C. A. (2007). Creating a social world: A developmental twin study of peer-group deviance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 958-965.

Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1993). A test of the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Behavior Genetics, 23, 21-27.

Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene–environment interaction predicting children's mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903-913.

Kirk, D. S. (2006). Examining the divergence across self-report and official data sources on inferences about the adolescent life-course of crime. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 22, 107-129.

251

Klepper, S., & Nagin, D. (1989). The deterrent effect of perceived certainty and severity of punishment revisited. Criminology, 27, 721-746.

Kohler, H. P., & Rodgers, J. L. (2001). DF-analyses of heritability with double-entry twin data: Asymptotic standard errors and efficient estimation. Behavior Genetics, 31: 179-191.

Krohn, M. D., & Massey, J. L. (1980). Social control and delinquent behavior: An examination of the elements of the social bond. The Sociological Quarterly, 21, 529-543.

Krohn, M. D., Thornberry, T. P., Rivera, C., & Le Blanc, M. (2001). Later delinquency careers. In R. Loeber & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Child delinquents: Development, intervention, and service needs (pp. 67-93). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Krueger, R. F., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bleske, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Assortative mating for antisocial behavior: Developmental and methodological implications. Behavior Genetics, 28, 173-186.

Kubrin, C. E., Stucky, T. D., & Krohn, M. D. (2009). Researching theories of crime and deviance. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

Lamb, D. J., Middeldorp, C. M., Van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2012). Gene– environment interaction in teacher rated internalizing and externalizing problem behavior in 7 to 12 year old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 818-825. ‐ Lasley, J. R.‐ (1988).‐ Toward‐ a control theory of white-collar offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 4, 347-362.

Lauritsen, J. L. (1993). Sibling resemblance in juvenile delinquency: Findings from the National Youth Survey. Criminology, 31, 387-409.

Lefgren, L., & McIntyre, F. (2006). The relationship between women's education and marriage outcomes. Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 787-830.

Lemert, E. M. (1951). Social pathology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Lessem, J. M., Hopfer, C. J., Haberstick, B. C., Timberlake, D., Ehringer, M. A., Smolen, A., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Relationship between adolescent marijuana use and young adult illicit drug use. Behavior Genetics, 36, 498-506.

Lilly, J. R., Cullen, F. T., & Ball, R. A. (2011). Criminology theory: Context and consequences (5th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

252

Liska, A. E., Krohn, M. D., & Messner, S. F. (1989). Strategies and requisites for theoretical integration in the study of crime and deviance. In S. F. Messner, M. D. Krohn, & A. E. Liska (Eds.), Theoretical integration in the study of deviance and crime (pp. 1-19). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. J. (1987). Antisocial and delinquent youths: Methods for their dearly identification. In J. D. Burchard & S. Burchard (Eds.), Prevention of delinquent behavior (pp. 75-89). Newbury Park, CA: Sage

Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. Crime and Justice, 7, 29-149.

Loehlin, J. C., & Nichols, J. (1976). Heredity, environment, and personality. Austin, TX:University of Texas Press.

Lombroso, C. (2006 [1876]). Criminal man. Translated by M. Gibson & N. H. Rafter. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Lopez, J. M. O., Redondo, L. M., & Martín, A. L. (1989). Influence of family and peer group on the use of drugs by adolescents. Substance Use & Misuse, 24, 1065-1082.

Lykken, D. T., & Tellegen, A. (1993). Is human mating adventitious or the result of lawful choice? A twin study of mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 56-68.

Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys' conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, 683-697.

Maimon, D., Antonaccio, O., & French, M. T. (2012). Severe sanctions, easy choice? Investigating the role of school sanctions in preventing adolescent violent offending. Criminology, 50, 495-524.

Maldonado-Molina, M. M., Reingle, J. M., & Jennings, W. G. (2011). Does alcohol use predict violent behaviors? The relationship between alcohol use and violence in a nationally representative longitudinal sample. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 9, 99-111.

Manning, W. D., Longmore, M. A., & Giordano, P. C. (2005). Adolescents' involvement in non- romantic sexual activity. Social Science Research, 34, 384-407.

Marcos, A. C., Bahr, S. J., & Johnson, R. E. (1986). Test of a bonding/association theory of adolescent drug use. Social Forces, 65, 135-161.

Mare, R. D. (1991). Five decades of educational assortative mating. American Sociological Review, 56, 15-32.

253

Mascie-Taylor, C. G. N., & Vandenberg, S. G. (1988). Assortative mating for IQ and personality due to propinquity and personal preference. Behavior Genetics, 18, 339-345.

Mason, D. A., & Frick, P. J. (1994). The heritability of antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 16, 301-323.

Massey, J. L., & Krohn, M. D. (1986). A longitudinal examination of an integrated social process model of deviant behavior. Social Forces, 65, 106-134.

Matsueda, R. L., Kreager, D. A., & Huizinga, D. (2006). Deterring delinquents: A rational choice model of theft and violence. American Sociological Review, 71, 95-122.

Matza, D. (1964). Delinquency and drift. New York, NY: John Wiley.

Matza, D. (1969). Becoming deviant. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

McCarthy, J. D., & Hoge, D. R. (1984). The dynamics of self-esteem and delinquency. American Journal of Sociology, 90, 396-410.

McCord, J. (1991). Family relationships, juvenile delinquency, and adult criminality. Criminology, 29, 397-417.

McGue, M., Osler, M., & Christensen, K. (2010). Causal inference and observational research: The utility of twins. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 546-556.

Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Mealy, L. (2000). Sex differences: Developmental and evolutionary strategies. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Mears, D. P., Cochran, J. C., & Beaver, K. M. (2013). Self-Control Theory and Nonlinear Effects on Offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 29, 447-476.

Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science, biometric theory, and twin studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis, 17, 191-214.

Mednick, S. A., Gabrielli, W. F., & Hutchings, B. (1984). Genetic influences in criminal convictions: Evidence from an adoption cohort. Science, 224, 891-894.

Menard, S. (1995). A developmental test of Mertonian anomie theory. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 32, 136-174.

Merton, R. K. (1938). Social structure and anomie. American Sociological Review, 3, 672-682.

Merton, R. K. (1957). Priorities in scientific discovery: A chapter in the sociology of science. American Sociological Review, 22, 635-659. 254

Merton, R. K. (1959). Social conformity, deviation, and opportunity structures: A Comment on the contributions of Dubin and Cloward. American Sociological Review, 24, 177-189.

Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Messner, S. F. (1988). Merton's “social structure and anomie”: The road not taken. Deviant Behavior, 9, 33-53.

Messner, S. F., & Rosenfeld, R. (1994). Crime and the American dream. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 207-217.

Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100: 674-701.

Moffitt, T. E. (2005a). The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: gene-environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 533-554.

Moffitt, T. E. (2005b). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviors: Evidence from behavioral–genetic research. Advances in Genetics, 55, 41-104.

Morley, K. I. & Hall, W. D. (2003). Is there a genetic susceptibility to engage in criminal acts? Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice, 263, 1-6.

Morenoff, J. D., Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology, 39, 517- 558.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Nagin, D. S. (1998). Criminal deterrence research at the outset of the twenty-first century. Crime and Justice, 23, 1-42.

Nagin, D. S. (2007). Moving choice to center stage in criminological research and theory: The American Society of Criminology 2006 Sutherland Address. Criminology, 45, 259-272.

Nagin, D. S. (2013). Deterrence in the twenty-first Century. Crime and Justice, 42, 199-263.

Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (1991). The preventive effects of the perceived risk of arrest: Testing an expanded conception of deterrence. Criminology, 29, 561-587.

Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (1993). Enduring individual differences and rational choice theories of crime. Law & Society Review, 27, 467-496.

255

Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (1994). Personal capital and social control: The deterrence implications of a theory of individual differences in criminal offending. Criminology, 32, 581-606.

Nagin, D. S., & Pogarsky, G. (2001). Integrating celerity, impulsivity, and extralegal sanction threats into a model of general deterrence: Theory and evidence. Criminology, 39, 865- 892.

Nagin, D. S., & Pogarsky, G. (2003). An experimental investigation of deterrence: Cheating, self-serving bias, and impulsivity. Criminology, 41, 167-194.

Natsuaki, M. N., Leve, L. D., Harold, G. T., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Ganiban, J., ... & Reiss, D. (2013). Transactions between child social wariness and observed structured parenting: Evidence from a prospective adoption study. Child Development, 84, 1750- 1765.

Neale, M. C. (2009). Biometrical models in behavioral genetics. In Y. K. Kim (Ed.) Handbook of behavior genetics (pp. 15-34). New York, NY: Springer.

Neale, M. C., & Maes, H. H. M. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Paternoster, R. (1987). The deterrent effect of the perceived certainty and severity of punishment: A review of the evidence and issues. Justice Quarterly, 4, 173-217.

Paternoster, R., & Iovanni, L. (1986). The deterrent effect of perceived severity: A reexamination. Social Forces, 64, 751-777.

Paternoster, R., & Piquero, A. (1995). Reconceptualizing deterrence: An empirical test of personal and vicarious experiences. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 32, 251-286.

Paternoster, R., & Pogarsky, G. (2009). Rational choice, agency and thoughtfully reflective decision making: The short and long-term consequences of making good choices. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 25, 103-127.

Paternoster, R., Pogarsky, G., & Zimmerman, G. (2011). Thoughtfully reflective decision making and the accumulation of capital: Bringing choice back in. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 27, 1-26.

Paternoster, R., & Simpson, S. (1996). Sanction threats and appeals to morality: Testing a rational choice model of . Law & Society Review, 30, 549-583.

Pike, A., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (1996). Using MZ differences in the search for nonshared environmental effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 695-704.

256

Pinker, S. (2002). : The modern denial of human nature. New York, NY: Penguin Books.

Piquero, A. R., Farrington, D. P., & Blumstein, A. (2003). The criminal career paradigm. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice (Vol. 30, pp. 359-506). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Piquero, A., & Paternoster, R. (1998). An application of Stafford and Warr's reconceptualization of deterrence to drinking and driving. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 35, 3-39.

Piquero, A. R., Paternoster, R., Pogarsky, G., & Loughran, T. (2011). Elaborating the individual difference component in deterrence theory. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 7, 335-360.

Piquero, A. R., & Pogarsky, G. (2002). Beyond Stafford and Warr's reconceptualization of deterrence: Personal and vicarious experiences, impulsivity, and offending behavior. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 39, 153-186.

Piquero, A., & Tibbetts, S. (1996). Specifying the direct and indirect effects of low self-control and situational factors in offenders' decision making: Toward a more complete model of rational offending. Justice Quarterly, 13, 481-510.

Plomin, R. (1990). The role of inheritance in behavior. Science, 248, 183-188.

Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1-16.

Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2013). Behavioral genetics (6th ed.). New York, NY: Worth Publishers.

Pogarsky, G. (2007). Deterrence and individual differences among convicted offenders. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 23, 59-74.

Pogarsky, G. (2002). Identifying “deterrable” offenders: Implications for research on deterrence. Justice Quarterly, 19, 431-452.

Pogarsky, G., & Piquero, A. R. (2003). Can punishment encourage offending? Investigating the “resetting” effect. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 40, 95-120.

Pogarsky, G., Piquero, A. R., & Paternoster, R. (2004). Modeling change in perceptions about sanction threats: The neglected linkage in deterrence theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 20, 343-369.

257

Prado, G., Huang, S., Schwartz, S. J., Maldonado-Molina, M. M., Bandiera, F. C., de la Rosa, M., & Pantin, H. (2009). What accounts for differences in substance use among US-born and immigrant Hispanic adolescents? Results from a longitudinal prospective cohort study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 118-125.

Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi's general theory of crime: A meta analysis. Criminology, 38, 931-964.

Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2005).‐ Assessing macro-level predictors and theories of crime: A meta-analysis. Crime and Justice, 32, 373-450.

Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Sellers, C. S., Winfree Jr, L. T., Madensen, T. D., Daigle, L. E., ... & Gau, J. M. (2010). The empirical status of social learning theory: A meta analysis. Justice Quarterly, 27, 765-802. ‐ Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Blevins, K. R., Daigle, L. E., & Madensen, T. D. (2006). The empirical status of deterrence theory: A meta-analysis. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins (Eds.), Taking stock: The status of criminological theory (pp. 367-396). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Prinstein, M. J., & Wang, S. S. (2005). False consensus and adolescent peer contagion: Examining discrepancies between perceptions and actual reported levels of friends’ deviant and health risk behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 293-306.

Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene–environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554-571.

Rafter, N. H. (2008). Criminology's darkest hour: Biocriminology in Nazi Germany. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology 41:287-306.

Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime: Criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Rankin, J. H., & Kern, R. (1994). Parental attachments and delinquency. Criminology, 32, 495- 515.

Rebellon, C. J., Piquero, N. L., Piquero, A. R., & Tibbetts, S. G. (2010). Anticipated shaming and criminal offending. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38, 988-997.

Reckless, W. C. (1961). The crime problem (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Reiss, A. J. (1951). Delinquency as the failure of personal and social controls. American Sociological Review, 16, 196-207.

Reitveld, M.J.H., Van derValk, J.C., Bongers, I.L., Slagboom, P.E., & Boomsma, D.I. (2000). Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Research, 3,134-141.

258

Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490- 529.

Rhule-Louie, D. M., & McMahon, R. J. (2007). Problem behavior and romantic relationships: Assortative mating, behavior contagion, and desistance. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 53-100.

Ridley, M. (1999). Genome: The autobiography of a species in 23 chapters. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

Robinson, M. B. & Beaver, K. M. (2009). Why crime: An interdisciplinary approach to explaining criminal behavior (2nd ed.). Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.

Rodgers, J. L., & Kohler, H. P. (2005). Reformulating and simplifying the DF analysis model. Behavior Genetics, 35, 211-217.

Rodgers, J. L., Rowe, D. C., & Li, C. (1994). Beyond nature versus nurture: DF analysis of nonshared influences on problem behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 30, 374-384.

Rovine, M. J. (1994). Estimating nonshared environment using sibling discrepancy scores. In E. M. Hetherington, D. Reiss, and R. Plomin (Eds.), Separate social worlds of siblings: The impact of nonshared environment on development (pp.33-62). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and behavior. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Rowe, D. C. (2001). Do people make environments or do environments make people? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 935, 62-74.

Rowe, D. C., & Farrington, D. P. (1997). The familial transmission of criminal convictions. Criminology, 35, 177-202.

Rowe, D. C., & Gulley, B. L. (1992). Sibling effects on substance use and delinquency. Criminology, 30, 217-234.

Rowe, D. C., & Jacobson, K. C. (1998). National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Pairs code book.

Rujescu, D., Giegling, I., Gietl, A., Hartmann, A. M., & Möller, H. J. (2003). A functional single nucleotide polymorphism (V158M) in the COMT gene is associated with aggressive personality traits. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 34-39.

Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1-12.

259

Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., & Eaves, L. (2001). Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 291-324.

Salem, R. G., & Bowers, W. J. (1970). Severity of formal sanctions as a deterrent to deviant behavior. Law & Society Review, 5, 21-40.

Sampson, R. J. (2013). The Place of Context: A Theory and Strategy for Criminology's Hard Problems. Criminology, 51, 1-31.

Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review, 64, 633-660.

Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918-924.

Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype→ environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424-435.

Schoemaker, P. J. (1982). The expected utility model: Its variants, purposes, evidence and limitations. Journal of Economic Literature, 20, 529-563.

Schreck, C. J., Fisher, B. S., & Miller, J. M. (2004). The social context of violent victimization: A study of the delinquent peer effect. Justice Quarterly, 21, 23-47.

Schwartz, J. A., & Beaver, K. M. (2011). Evidence of a gene × environment interaction between perceived prejudice and MAOA genotype in the prediction of criminal arrests. Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 378-384.

Schwartz, J. A., & Beaver, K. M. (2013). A Partial Test of Moffitt’s Developmental Taxonomy: Examining the Role of Genetic Risk. Justice Quarterly, Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/07418825.2013.805798

Segal, N. L. (2012). Born together—reared apart: The landmark Minnesota twin study. Harvard University Press.

Sellers, C. S., & Winfree, L. T. (1990). Differential associations and definitions: A panel study of youthful drinking behavior. Substance Use & Misuse, 25, 755-771.

Shackelford, T. K., Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 447-458.

Shaw, C. & McKay, H. D. (1942). Juvenile delinquency and urban areas. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

260

Shin, S. H., Edwards, E. M., & Heeren, T. (2009). Child abuse and neglect: relations to adolescent binge drinking in the national longitudinal study of Adolescent Health (AddHealth) Study. Addictive Behaviors, 34, 277-280.

Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Burt, C. H., Brody, G. H., & Cutrona, C. (2005). Collective efficacy, authoritative parenting and delinquency: A longitudinal test of a model integrating community- and family-level processes. Criminology, 43, 989-1029.

Smith, K. B., & Hatemi, P. K. (2013). OLS is AOK for ACE: A regression-based approach to synthesizing political science and behavioral genetics models. Political Behavior, 35, 383-408.

Snyder, J. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). Individual differences in social aggression: A test of a reinforcement model of socialization in the natural environment. Behavior Therapy, 26, 371-391.

Spivak, A. L., Fukushima, M., Kelley, M. S., & Jenson, T. S. (2011). Religiosity, delinquency, and the deterrent effects of informal sanctions. Deviant Behavior, 32, 677-711.

Stafford, M. C., & Warr, M. (1993). A reconceptualization of general and specific deterrence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30, 123-135.

Stewart, E. A. (2003). School social bonds, school climate, and school misbehavior: A multilevel analysis. Justice Quarterly, 20, 575-604.

Stitt, B. G., & Giacopassi, D. J. (1992). Trends in the connectivity of theory and research in criminology. The Criminologist, 17, 3-6.

Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of criminology (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.

Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in action: Symbols and strategies. American Sociological Review, 51, 273-286.

Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664-670.

Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1031-1039.

Thornberry, T. P. (1989). Reflections on the advantages and disadvantages of theoretical integration. In S. F. Messner, M. D. Krohn, & A. E. Liska (Eds.), Theoretical integration in the study of deviance and crime (pp. 198-235). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

261

Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. (2000). The self-report method for measuring delinquency and crime. Criminal Justice, 4, 33-83.

Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D., Farnworth, M., & Jang, S. J. (1994). Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology, 32, 47-83.

Tibbetts, S. G., & Herz, D. C. (1996). Gender differences in factors of social control and rational choice. Deviant Behavior, 17, 183-208.

Tibbetts, S. G., & Myers, D. L. (1999). Low self-control, rational choice, and student test cheating. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 23, 179-200.

Tittle, C. R., & Rowe, A. R. (1973). Moral appeal, sanction threat, and deviance: An experimental test. Social Problems, 20, 488-498.

Toby, J. (1957). Social disorganization and stake in conformity: Complementary factors in the predatory behavior of hoodlums. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology & Police Science, 48, 12-17.

Tucker Drob, E. M., & Harden, K. P. (2012). Intellectual interest mediates Gene × Socioeconomic Status interaction on adolescent academic achievement. Child ‐Development, 83, 743-757.

Tunnell, K. D. (1990). Choosing crime: Close your eyes and take your chances. Justice Quarterly, 7, 673-690.

Tunnell, K. D. (1992). Choosing crime: The criminal calculus of property offenders. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall Publishers.

Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 160-164.

Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Nonshared environment: a theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 78-108.

Turner, C. F., Ku, L., Rogers, S. M., Lindberg, L. D., Pleck, J. H., & Sonenstein, F. L. (1998). Adolescent sexual behavior, drug use, and violence: increased reporting with computer survey technology. Science, 280: 867-873.

Vaughn, M. G., DeLisi, M., Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2009). DAT1 and 5HTT are associated with pathological criminal behavior in a nationally representative sample of youth. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 1113-1124.

Vila, B. (1994). A general paradigm for understanding criminal behavior: Extending evolutionary ecological theory. Criminology, 32, 311-360.

262

Vinkhuyzen, A. A., van der Sluis, S., Maes, H. H., & Posthuma, D. (2012). Reconsidering the heritability of intelligence in adulthood: Taking assortative mating and cultural transmission into account. Behavior Genetics, 42, 187-198.

Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Arseneault, L. (2009). The discordant MZ-twin method: One step closer to the holy grail of causality. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 376-382.

Volavka, J. A. N., Bilder, R., & Nolan, K. (2004). Catecholamines and aggression: the role of COMT and MAO polymorphisms. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036, 393-398.

Walby, K., & Carrier, N. (2010). The rise of biocriminology: Capturing observable bodily economies of ‘criminal man’. Criminology and Criminal Justice, 10, 261-285.

Walden, B., McGue, M., Burt, S. A., & Elkins, I. (2004). Identifying shared environmental contributions to early substance use: The respective roles of peers and parents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 440-450.

Waldo, G. P., & Chiricos, T. G. (1972). Perceived penal sanction and self-reported criminality: A neglected approach to deterrence research. Social Problems, 19, 522-540.

Walsh, A. (2000). Behavior genetics and anomie/strain theory. Criminology, 38, 1075-1108.

Walsh, A. (2002). Biosocial criminology: Introduction and integration. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Publishing Co.

Walsh, A. (2009). Criminal behavior from heritability to epigenetics: How genetics clarify the role of the environment. In A. Walsh & K. M. Beaver (Eds.), Biosocial criminology: New directions in theory and research (pp. 29-49). New York, NY: Routledge.

Walsh, A. (2011). Feminist criminology through a biosocial lens. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.

Walsh, A. (2014). Criminology theory: Assessing philosophical assumptions. Waltham, MA: Anderson Publishing.

Wang, S. N., & Jensen, G. E. (2011). Explaining delinquency in Taiwan: A test of social learning theory. In R. L. Akers & G. F. Jensen (Eds.), Social learning theory and the explanation of crime: A guide for the New Century. Advances in criminology Theory, Vol. 11 (pp. 65- 84). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Warr, M. (1993). Age, peers, and delinquency. Criminology, 31, 17-40.

Warr, M. (2002). Companions in crime: The social aspects of criminal conduct. Cambridge University Press.

263

Weerman, F. M., & Smeenk, W. H. (2005). Peer similarity in delinquency for different types of friends: A comparison using two measurement methods. Criminology, 43, 499-524.

Weisburd, D., & Piquero, A. R. (2008). How well do criminologists explain crime? Statistical modeling in published studies. Crime and Justice, 37, 453-502.

Welch, M. R., Tittle, C. R., & Grasmick, H. G. (2006). Christian religiosity, self-control and social conformity. Social Forces, 84, 1605-1623.

Wiatrowski, M. D., Griswold, D. B., & Roberts, M. K, (1981). Social control theory and delinquency. American Sociological Review, 46, 525-541.

Widom, C. S., & Brzustowicz, L. M. (2006). MAOA and the “cycle of violence:” Childhood abuse and neglect, MAOA genotype, and risk for violent and antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 684-689.

Wiesner, M. Capaldi, D. M., & Patterson, G. (2003). Development of antisocial behavior and crime across the life-span from social interactional perspective: the coercion model. Social learning theory and the explanation of crime: A guide for the New Century. Advances in criminology Theory, Vol. 11 (pp. 317-338). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Williams, K. R., & Hawkins, R. (1989). The meaning of wife assault. Criminology, 27, 163-182.

Winfree, L. T., Mays, G. L., & Vigil-Bäckström, T. (1994). Youth gangs and incarcerated delinquents: Exploring the ties between gang membership, delinquency, and social learning theory. Justice Quarterly, 11, 229-256.

Wolfgang, M. E., Figlio, R. M., & Sellin, T. (1972). Delinquency in a birth cohort. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Wright, B. R. E., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1999). Low self-control, social bonds, and crime: Social causation, , or both? Criminology, 37, 479-514.

Wright, J. P., & Beaver, K. M. (2005). Do parents matter in creating self-control in their children? A genetically informed test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s theory of low self- control. Criminology, 43, 1169-1202.

Wright, J. P., Beaver, K. M., DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Boisvert, D., & Vaske, J. (2008). Lombroso's legacy: The miseducation of criminologists. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 19, 325-338.

Wright, J. P., & Boisvert, D. (2009). What biosocial criminology offers criminology. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 1228-1240.

264

Wright, J. P., & Cullen, F. T. (2012). The future of biosocial criminology beyond scholars’ professional ideology. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 28, 237-253.

Wright, J. P., Cullen, F. T., Agnew, R. S., & Brezina, T. (2001). “The root of all evil”? An exploratory study of money and delinquent involvement. Justice Quarterly, 18, 239-268.

Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal. Why we are the way we are: The new science of evolutionary psychology. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

Young, J. T., Barnes, J. C., Meldrum, R. C., & Weerman, F. M. (2011). Assessing and explaining misperceptions of peer delinquency. Criminology, 49, 599-630.

Zahn, M. A. (1999). Thoughts on the future of criminology: the American Society of Criminology 1998 Presidential Address. Criminology, 37, 1-16.

Zhang, L., & Messner, S. F. (1995). Family deviance and delinquency in China. Criminology, 33, 359-387.

Zheng, Y., & Cleveland, H. H. (2013). Identifying gender-specific developmental trajectories of nonviolent and violent delinquency from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 371-381.

Zimring, F. E., & Hawkins, G. J. (1973). Deterrence: The legal threat in crime control. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

265

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Joseph A. Schwartz

Joseph Schwartz received his Bachelor’s and Master’s degrees in criminal justice from

California State University, San Bernardino. His research interests include behavior genetics, biosocial criminology, and additional factors involved in the etiology of criminal behavior.

During his time at Florida State University (2010-2014), he published widely on a diverse set of topics related to behavior, intelligence, and the biosocial perspective. In the fall of 2014, he will join the faculty in the School of Criminology and Criminal Justice at University of Nebraska at

Omaha.

266