Courtship Behavior of the Greater Bird of Paradise

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Courtship Behavior of the Greater Bird of Paradise COURTSHIP BEHAVIOR OF THE GREATER BIRD OF PARADISE JA•s J. D•s•o• I• September1909, 48 Greater Birds of Paradise (Paradisaeaapoda apoda) of unknownsex were releasedon 280-acreLittle Tobago Island (11o18' N, 60ø30' W) in the southernWest Indies (Ingram, 1911). The birds had been obtained on th'e Aru Islands south of New Guinea and were placedon Little Tobagoin the hope of establishinga population safe from the plume hunting that threatenedtheir existencein their native home. Augmentedby three morebirds by 1912 (Ingram, 1913), the pop- ulation apparentlyfluctuated in numbersfor the next 20 years,but no one censusedthem carefully (Ingram, 1917, 1918; Baker, 1923; Guppy, 1931). The islandbecame a sanctuaryin 1928,but by 1955 only 11 apoda were th'oughtto survive (Ingram, 1956). E. T. Gilliard (19'58, and MS field notes) thoughtperhaps 35 birds were presentin 1958 but he was certain of only 15. A hurricanein 1963 may have causeda further re- ductionin the population. Thus althoughthe Little Tobago population has survived,it has never thrived as originallyhoped. I spent 9 months on Little Tobago studying this speciesin 1965-66. In that time I locatedonly 7 apoda; 4 full-plumagedmales, 1 subadult male, and 2 birds in the chocolatebrown plumage worn by both females and young birds. At least one of these was a female. I watchedapoda on Little Tobago almost daily from 23 September 19'65to 4 July 1966. Roldan George,a residentof nearby Tobago,helped in the field work and continuedobservations through 30 September1966. The period reportedon here is 1 October 1965 through 30 September 1966. We worked mainly at severaldisplay groundsthat the birds used extensivelyand in particular one that seemedto be the centerof activity. We usednatural hiding placesand took extensivefield noteson the court- ship behavior. As apoda are extremelywary, I made no attempt to captureor mark any of them, but I could distinguishadult males by differencesin the color and length of the side plumes. I actually watched the birds for about 364 hours and securedadditional information over many hours when I could only hear them. In all I watched304 courtshipdisplays and heard an additional 168. Knowledgeof the courtshipof apodahas heretoforebeen based largely on a few brief descriptions.Wallace in his classicalaccount (1869: 466) describeshow 12 to 20 full-plumagedmales on the Aru Islands assembled to play and dancein lofty, large-leavedforest trees, 305 The Auk, 87: 305-321. April 1970 306 JAMESJ. D•NS•ORE [Auk, Vol. 87 raise up their wings, stretch out their necks, and elevate their exquisite plumes, keeping them in a continual vibration. Between whiles they fly acrossfrom branch to branch in great excitement, so that the whole tree is filled with waving plumes in every variety of attitude and motion.... the wings are raised vertically over the back, the head is bent down and stretched out, and the long plumes are raised up and expanded till they form two magnificent golden fans. No one sinceWallace has describedthe displayof apoda from the Aru Islands. Baker's description(1923) of its display on Little Tobago is similar to that of Wallace. Crandall (1936) photographedand briefly describedthe displayof captivebirds and Iredale (1950) also has brief noteson the display. Gilliard (MS field notes) wrote the best previousdescription of the displayof apodaon Little Tobago. He noted that malesgathered at edges of a display area, each on a secondaryperch. Here each bird called and rapidly flappedhis wingsagainst his sidesin short, rapid wingbeats. From there the birds movedto the main display limbs where, with body held upright, they extendedtheir wings forward and erectedthe plumes over the back. Then abruptly the tail was depressed,the head,wings, and body were loweredso that the body was closeto the limb, and the plumes were still held erect over the back. In this positionthe bird movedalong the limb, sometimesprancing on stiff legs and at other times charging rapidly. The bird then stoppedabruptly and assumeda frozen posture, the bill pointing down, wings spread,and plumeserect. Gilliard called this the "flower" positionand consideredit the climax of the display. Gilliard observedthat when no femaleswere present,the malescharged back and forth• on the limbs, often in synchrony. However, when females were present,they assumedthe "flower" position. This positionwas only assumedon the centralor main dancingstages (courts). He also thought that the male with the longestplumes tended to assumethe centralposi- tion in the court and other males assembledaround him, each with a private perch. DIS•,•,^¾ G•ou•:Ds I located four main display grounds (the tree or trees used for dis- playing by one or more males) on Little Tobago. I occasionallysaw dis- plays at trees outsideof these four, but the use of these trees was too irregular and infrequentto ascertainany exact usagepattern. Three of the display groundswere within 63 m of each other on a woodedhillside. Each seemedto have one plumed male in regular attendance. The three birds could easily hear and probablyoften see each other. They often visited each other'sdisplay grounds,but the regular male at each used a centrallylocated court (the limbsof treesused repeatedly for displaying) April 1970] Greater Bird oJ Paradise 307 while the others seemedto use peripheralperches and courts. The fourth adult male had short sideplumes and probably had just reachedmaturity. This bird apparentlyhad no court he attendedregularly and was seldom seen. The fourth display ground was 365 m from the others and seemed to be used transiently. The two most frequently used display grounds were on the west slope of a hill and thus were not exposedto the early morning sun. Elsewhere I have described in detail the limbs used as courts and/or perches(Dinsmore, 1967). In general the principal requirementsfor a court seem to be a horizontal or gently sloping leaflessbranch, 8 cm or less in diameter, under a fairly thick canopy cover. Perches seem to re- quire thicker canopy. When deciduoustrees dropped their leavesin the dry season,the birds desertedperches there and establishednew perches in thick-canopiedevergreen trees. Most courtsand percheswere in under- story trees such as Eugenia sp. and Mayepea caribaea. Three of 13 courts had lateral scratch marks or small holes on them, apparently from bill-wiping and pecking, but none was worn smooth of bark. I never saw birds clearing leaves from the courts. Rather they seemedto chooselimbs or parts of limbs that were naturally clear of leaves. CALLS The basic call note of apoda is a deep loud "wauk" or "wonk." This note is deliveredin a number of ca]ling patterns. Severalof thesepatterns Mong with a few other ca]Is are associatedwith courtship display. The courtship display, describedin the next section, consistsof five main phases(wing pose,pump, bow, dance,and mounting). Rising calL--In this call four or sometimesfive deliberate"wauks" are repeatedat about 1-secondintervals, the first two notesof equal intensity and the rest successivelylouder and higher. Birds give this call, at the rate of about one per minute, most often in early morning from near their display grounds. They often give a similar call in late afternoon before they roost. Rapid wauk call.--This call is a series of rapid, equal pitch "wauk" notes deliveredin short bursts of severalper second,usually accompanied by synchronousshort wingbeats, the wing tips beingmoved about 2 inches from the bird's sides and then flapped back against the sides with' each note. It is usually given when a female is near the display ground and, with the followingcall, accompaniesthe wing posephase of the display. Wing pose calL--This call, given with the wing pose phase of the dis- play, is a shrill sliding"eee-ak" note, often repeatedseveral times. While calling the bird throws the wings forward into the wing pose position. Birds often alternate this and the precedingcall, deliveringrapid "wauk" 308 J^•r•s J. D•sz4oR• [Auk, Vol. 87 notes with the wings held forward rather than flapped against their sides as previously described. Pump call.--This consistsof a series of very rapidly given "wauk" notes of equal pitch, even faster than the rapid "wauk" call described above. The notes often are run together so that it sounds more like "wa-wa-wa-wa." Lasting up to about 10 seconds,the pump call is given without break during the pump phaseof the display. Baa calL--At the condusionof the pump call, the bird usually gives one or several drawn-out, harsh nasal "baa" notes as it tips the body forward and down. Click call.--During the dance phase of the display, the birds utter a slow and rhythmic "click" at about 1-secondintervals, much like the soundmade when a personclicks his tongueagainst the roof of his mouth. Between "clicks" I sometimes heard a faint nasal "bonk" note as the bird raisedthe wings for anotherdance step. Nasal call.--Similar to the "baa" call but more abrupt and nasal, this call is given most commonlyafter a display, either as the bird bill-wipes or moves from tree to tree tearing off leaves. Chugich call.--This call, heard only a few times, is a rather harsh, guttural "chug'-ich," "chug'-a," or "chug'-a-la" note. It is given in two quite different contextsand may be two distinct but similar calls. Males give it while bouncing along the court prior to giving the "dick" call and starting the rhythmic bouncing of the dance. I also heard it just after a bird left a display groundbut was still nearby, usually as he was about to move to his evening roost. COURTSHIP DISPLAY I recognizefive main phasesin the courtshipdisplay of apoda. They are describedin the sequencein which they usually were given. A more thorough'description of the sequenceis presentedlater in Table 1. Al- though the term "dance" often has been used in referenceto the entire display of apoda, in this discussiondance refers to only the fourth phase of the display.
Recommended publications
  • Origins of Language in Relation to Sexual Selection: the Effect of M
    h [ { { h [ { { 9 b a { 9 [ / Ç" Ü $ 9 " ò t '(( h [ { { L " Ç" Ü $ 9 " t * " " + $ + + , - + " L " + $ -- . / !.{Çw!/Ç Language can be viewed as sexual displays from an evolutionary perspective. As both sexes in humans contribute significantly to parental care, we can expect that both males and females use language to display their favourable qualities in order to attract potential mates. In this study, 50 participants (22 male, 28 female) rated 30 vignettes, in which a male or female protagonist attempted to impress an opposite sex friend in a conversation. In both male-female and female-male flirtations, conversations reveal- ing the speaker’s positive character traits were the most highly rated while those ex- plicitly showing the speaker’s sexual interest in the potential partner were the most poorly rated. Despite the similarities, there were significant sex differences in the rat- ings of some individual vignettes and item-groups. Such differences would be chiefly discussed from an evolutionary perspective. L The origins of language surely deserve and require a great deal of discussion among researchers from a wide range of disciplines. Based on my previous essay about the sexually attractive features of language (Szeto 2009), I will look into the relationship between sexual selection and language origins in greater depth, with a special focus on the effect of mutual mate choice on sex differences in speech content. ' h [ { { Despite the prevalence of elaborate animal communication systems, it is essen- tially unequivocal that humans are the only species which possess language (Barrett, Dunbar and Lycett 2002; Hurford 2006; Workman and Reader 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Correlated Complexity in the Radically Different Courtship Signals of Birds-Of-Paradise
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/351437; this version posted June 20, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Evolution of correlated complexity in the radically different courtship signals of birds-of-paradise 5 Russell A. Ligon1,2*, Christopher D. Diaz1, Janelle L. Morano1, Jolyon Troscianko3, Martin Stevens3, Annalyse Moskeland1†, Timothy G. Laman4, Edwin Scholes III1 1- Cornell Lab of Ornithology, 159 Sapsucker Woods Rd, Ithaca, NY, USA. 10 2- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA. 3- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and Environmental Science, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE, UK 4- Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford St., Cambridge, MA 02138, USA 15 † Current address: Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, UK *Author for correspondence: [email protected] ORCID: Russell Ligon https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0195-8275 20 Janelle Morano https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5950-3313 Edwin Scholes https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7724-3201 [email protected] [email protected] 25 [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] 30 keywords: ornament, complexity, behavioral analyses, sensory ecology, phenotypic radiation 35 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/351437; this version posted June 20, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
    [Show full text]
  • Broad-Tailed Hummingbird Coloration and Sun Orientation 1
    1 Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 1 Two ways to display: male hummingbirds show different 2 color-display tactics based on sun orientation 3 Running header: Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 4 5 Richard K. Simpson1* and Kevin J. McGraw1 6 1School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-4501 7 *Corresponding Author. Email: [email protected]; Phone: (480) 965-2593 8 9 ABSTRACT 10 Animals exhibit a diversity of ornaments and courtship behaviors, which often co- 11 occur and are used for communication. The sensory drive hypothesis states that these 12 traits evolved and vary due to interactions with each other, the environment, and signal 13 receiver. However, interactions between colorful ornaments and courtship behaviors, 14 specifically in relation to environmental variation, remain poorly understood. We studied 15 male iridescent plumage (gorgets), display behavior, and sun orientation during courtship 16 flights (shuttle displays) in broad-tailed hummingbirds (Selasphorus platycercus), to 17 understand how these traits interact in both space and time to produce the perceived 18 coloration of males. We also tested how gorget coloration varies among males based on 19 their plumage, behavioral, and morphological characteristics. In contrast with previous 20 work on other animals, we found that displaying males did not directionally face the sun, 21 but instead displayed on a continuum of solar orientation angles. The gorgets of males 22 who tended to face the sun during their displays appeared flashier (i.e. exhibited greater 23 color/brightness changes), brighter, and more colorful, whereas the gorgets of males who 2 Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 24 tended to not face the sun were more consistently reflective (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Management and Breeding of Birds of Paradise (Family Paradisaeidae) at the Al Wabra Wildlife Preservation
    Management and breeding of Birds of Paradise (family Paradisaeidae) at the Al Wabra Wildlife Preservation. By Richard Switzer Bird Curator, Al Wabra Wildlife Preservation. Presentation for Aviary Congress Singapore, November 2008 Introduction to Birds of Paradise in the Wild Taxonomy The family Paradisaeidae is in the order Passeriformes. In the past decade since the publication of Frith and Beehler (1998), the taxonomy of the family Paradisaeidae has been re-evaluated considerably. Frith and Beehler (1998) listed 42 species in 17 genera. However, the monotypic genus Macgregoria (MacGregor’s Bird of Paradise) has been re-classified in the family Meliphagidae (Honeyeaters). Similarly, 3 species in 2 genera (Cnemophilus and Loboparadisea) – formerly described as the “Wide-gaped Birds of Paradise” – have been re-classified as members of the family Melanocharitidae (Berrypeckers and Longbills) (Cracraft and Feinstein 2000). Additionally the two genera of Sicklebills (Epimachus and Drepanornis) are now considered to be combined as the one genus Epimachus. These changes reduce the total number of genera in the family Paradisaeidae to 13. However, despite the elimination of the 4 species mentioned above, 3 species have been newly described – Berlepsch's Parotia (P. berlepschi), Eastern or Helen’s Parotia (P. helenae) and the Eastern or Growling Riflebird (P. intercedens). The Berlepsch’s Parotia was once considered to be a subspecies of the Carola's Parotia. It was previously known only from four female specimens, discovered in 1985. It was rediscovered during a Conservation International expedition in 2005 and was photographed for the first time. The Eastern Parotia, also known as Helena's Parotia, is sometimes considered to be a subspecies of Lawes's Parotia, but differs in the male’s frontal crest and the female's dorsal plumage colours.
    [Show full text]
  • Nesting Behavior of a Raggiana Bird of Paradise
    Wilson Bull., 106(3), 1994, pp. 522-530 NESTING BEHAVIOR OF A RAGGIANA BIRD OF PARADISE WILLIAM E. DAVIS, JR.’ AND BRUCE M. BEEHLER* ABSTRACT..-WC made observations of a nest of a Raggiana Bird of Paradise (Parudisaea raggiana) for 22 days. The single nestling was attended only by the female and was fed only arthropods until day 5, and thereafter a mix of arthropods and fruit. Evidence from regurgitation of seeds at the nest indicates that the parent subsisted largely on fruit. This dietary dichotomy conforms to that of other polygynous birds of paradise and accords with socioecological predictions concerning single-parent nestling care. Received 3 Aug. 1993, accepted 1 Feb. 1994. Many aspects of the life history of birds of paradise (Paradisaeidae) are at least superficially understood (Gilliard 1969, Cooper and Forshaw 1977, Diamond 1981, Beehler 1989). One notable exception is nesting biology which is inadequately documented for many paradisaeid species (Cooper and Forshaw 1977). In spite of recent contributions (Pruett-Jones and Pruett-Jones 1988; Frith and Frith 1990, 1992, 1993a, b; Mack 1992), the nests of 13 species remain undescribed, and 26 species have never been studied at the nest (Cooper and Forshaw 1977; Beehler, unpubl.). Here we provide the first detailed description of nesting behavior of the Raggiana Bird of Paradise (Parudisaea ruggianu) in the wild, one of the best-known members of the family, and Papua New Guinea’s national symbol. The Raggiana Bird of Paradise is a common, vocal, and widespread species of forest and edge that inhabits lowlands and hills of southern, central, and southeastern Papua New Guinea (Cooper and Forshaw 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • Avian Monogamy
    (ISBN: 0-943610-45-1) AVIAN MONOGAMY EDITED BY PATRICIA ADAIR GOWATY AND DOUGLAS W. MOCK Department of Zoology University of Oklahoma Norman, Oklahoma 73019 ORNITHOLOGICAL MONOGRAPHS NO. 37 PUBLISHED BY THE AMERICAN ORNITHOLOGISTS' UNION WASHINGTON, D.C. 1985 AVIAN MONOGAMY ORNITHOLOGICAL MONOGRAPHS This series, published by the American Ornithologists' Union, has been estab- lished for major papers too long for inclusion in the Union's journal, The Auk. Publication has been made possiblethrough the generosityof the late Mrs. Carll Tucker and the Marcia Brady Tucker Foundation, Inc. Correspondenceconcerning manuscripts for publication in the seriesshould be addressedto the Editor, Dr. David W. Johnston,Department of Biology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030. Copies of Ornithological Monographs may be ordered from the Assistant to the Treasurer of the AOU, Frank R. Moore, Department of Biology, University of Southern Mississippi, Southern Station Box 5018, Hattiesburg, Mississippi 39406. (See price list on back and inside back covers.) OrnithologicalMonographs,No. 37, vi + 121 pp. Editors of Ornithological Monographs, Mercedes S. Foster and David W. Johnston Special Reviewers for this issue, Walter D. Koenig, Hastings Reservation, Star Route Box 80, Carmel Valley, CA 93924; Lewis W. Oring, De- partment of Biology,Box 8238, University Station, Grand Forks, ND 58202 Authors, Patricia Adair Gowaty, Department of BiologicalSciences, Clem- son University, Clemson, SC 29631; Douglas W. Mock, Department of Zoology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK 73019 First received, 23 August 1983; accepted29 February 1984; final revision completed 8 October 1984 Issued October 17, 1985 Price $11.00 prepaid ($9.00 to AOU members). Library of CongressCatalogue Card Number 85-647080 Printed by the Allen Press,Inc., Lawrence, Kansas 66044 Copyright ¸ by the American Ornithologists'Union, 1985 ISBN: 0-943610-45-1 ii AVIAN MONOGAMY EDITED BY PATRICIA ADAIR GOWATY AND DOUGLAS W.
    [Show full text]
  • Short Communications Courtship Display and Mating of the Superb
    Short Communications Courtship Display and Mating of the Superb Bird of Paradise Lophorina superba D.W. & C.B. FRITH "Prionodura':Paluma via Townsville, Queensland 4816 Emu 88, 183-188 Received 17 May 1987, accepted 9 October 1987 The Superb Bird of Paradise Lophorina superba inhabits December 1977 and November 1985 and we compared rainforest and forest edge between approximately 1000 these with those of the wild birds and descriptions by other and 2250 m above mean sea level (amsl) throughout authors. mainland New Guinea. The adult male is velvet black with metallic-like oil-green iridescent plumage on the crown, an Initial dkplay activily (IDA) erectile iridescent breast shield, and with a very long erectile and spreadable cape of modified velvet black nape This was observed seven times and consists of a sleeked feathers. Peculiar horn-like naral tufts of erectile rather stiff pose, held for about five seconds, before movement of black feathers adorn the base of the upper mandible, above cape, breast and naral tuft plumage. The male slightly the nostrils, as does a similar tuft of feathers beneath the crouches with breast shield sleeked tightly back against lower mandible. The female is cryptically coloured in himself, cape held back and down against the back, wings browns and greys with distinct blackish ventral bamng, and tail held normally, head and bill pointed upwards with like many other female paradisaeids (see Gilliard 1969; eyes fixed on the female, and naral tufts projecting conspic- Cooper & Forshaw 1977). First year male plumage is like uously forward and bifurcate (Fig. 1). This pose is followed that of the female, the black and iridescent plumage of by a repeated, sudden, upward and outward extension of adult males being acquired gradually over several years the breast shield, with head and bill still pointing at the (Gilliard 1969).
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Leks Through Female Choice: Differential Clustering And
    BehavEcol Sociobiol (1992) 30:227-237 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology ? Springer-Verlag1992 The evolutionof leks throughfemale choice: differentialclustering and space utilizationin six sympatricmanakins Marc Thery C.N.R.S. - U.R.A. 1183, Mus6umNational d'Histoire Naturelle, Ecologie Generale,4 avenue du Petit-Chateau, F-91800 Brunoy, France Received October 1, 1990 / Accepted October 2, 1991 Summary.The degreeto which lekkingand non-lekking the increasingfemale home-rangesize could have led male manakinsselect display sites in order to maximise to the evolution of classicalleks. proximityto femaleswas examinedby contrastingmove- ments of females with male dispersion.Data on female visitingpatterns, male courtshipdisruption, and mating skew were also collected over three successivebreeding Introduction seasons. For the five lek-breedingspecies, female home- rangeswere 3-7 times largerthan those of adult males. Lek matingsystems, where males congregatefor the sole Femalemovements were concentrated around leks, fruit- purpose of attractingand courtingfemales, are typified ing places and streambathing sites. None of the females by a heavilybiased operational sex ratio and the inability monitoredby radio-trackingexpanded her normalrange of individualmales to economicallycontrol or monopo- in order to visit males on leks. On the contrary,feeding lize the resourcesessential for femaleacquisition (Emlen bouts of femalesfrequently preceded a visit to potential and Oring 1977). Across lek mating systems,male clus- mates at neighboringleks. Despite small sample sizes, teringis highly variableand can be dividedinto "classi- significant correlations were found between female cal leks" (Bradbury1977, 1981) also called "true leks" home-rangesize and male clustering(distances between (Oring 1982), where males are densely grouped within neighboring leks and distances between neighboring sight of each other, and "explodedleks" (Gilliard1963; males), as predicted by the female choice model and Snow 1970; Bradbury 1981; Foster 1983) or "quasi- the hotspot model.
    [Show full text]
  • Courtship Behavior in the Dwarf Seahorse, Hippocampuszosterae
    Copeia, 1996(3), pp. 634-640 Courtship Behavior in the Dwarf Seahorse, Hippocampuszosterae HEATHER D. MASONJONESAND SARA M. LEWIS The seahorse genus Hippocampus (Syngnathidae) exhibits extreme morpho- logical specialization for paternal care, with males incubating eggs within a highly vascularized brood pouch. Dwarf seahorses, H. zosterae, form monoga- mous pairs that court early each morning until copulation takes place. Daily behavioral observations of seahorse pairs (n = 15) were made from the day of introduction through the day of copulation. Four distinct phases of seahorse courtship are marked by prominent behavioral changes, as well as by differences in the intensity of courtship. The first courtship phase occurs for one or two mornings preceding the day of copulation and is characterized by reciprocal quivering, consisting of rapid side-to-side body vibrations displayed alternately by males and females. The remaining courtship phases are restricted to the day of copulation, with the second courtship phase distinguished by females pointing, during which the head is raised upward. In the third courtship phase, males begin to point in response to female pointing. During the final phase of courtship, seahorse pairs repeatedly rise together in the water column, eventually leading to females transferring their eggs directly into the male brood pouch during a brief midwater copulation. Courtship activity level (representing the percentage of time spent in courtship) increased from relatively low levels during the first courtship phase to highly active courtship on the day of copulation. Males more actively initiated courtship on the days preceding copulation, indicating that these seahorses are not courtship-role reversed, as has previously been assumed.
    [Show full text]
  • Correlates of Male Mating Success in a Lekking Bird with Male-Male
    Anim. Behav., 1989,37, 1007-1022 Correlates of male mating successin a lekking bird with male-male cooperation DAVID B. McDONALD. Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721, U.S.A. Abstract. Correlates of male mating success were examined in a population of long-tailed manakins, Chiroxiphia linearis, that included 270 colour-banded individuals. Long-tailed manakins have a let mating system and male-male cooperation in courtship display. Multivariate analysis of behavioural variables indicated that female visitation correlated with the number of unison 'toledo' calls given by male partners. Given a female visit, copulatory successwas correlated with the 'butterfly' display component of . the dual-male dance. Both 'toledo' output and dance display differed significantly between perch-zones. Only six to eight partnerships in a local population of as many as 55 males per season performed call displays at a level (75-335 toledos per h) that was correlated with any female visitation. Data on crown plumage of female visitors suggested that younger females may have been less discriminating than were older females. The relationship between v;1riance in mating successand the evolution of cooperative ma:le display is discussed. ' In let mating systems male-male competition birds, the former Old World and the latter New (intrasexual selection) or female choice, or both, World,lekmatingistherule.Lekmatingisperhaps could affect male mating success.Variance in male the clearest example of sexual selection and is mating success may be the single most important characterized by intense competition between component of fitness in such systems. Although males. It seems paradoxical, therefore, to find that high variance in mating successhas long been noted in one genus of manakins, Chiroxiphia, males for many let species (Lill 1974; Bradbury [981), cooperate in courtship display (Foster 1977, 1981).
    [Show full text]
  • Female Mate Choice Based Upon Male Motor Performance
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Eileen Hebets Publications Papers in the Biological Sciences 4-2010 Female mate choice based upon male motor performance John Byers University of Idaho, [email protected] Eileen Hebets University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected] Jeffrey Podos University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons Byers, John; Hebets, Eileen; and Podos, Jeffrey, "Female mate choice based upon male motor performance" (2010). Eileen Hebets Publications. 45. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/45 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eileen Hebets Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Animal Behaviour 79:4 (April 2010), pp. 771–778; doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009 Copyright © 2010 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour; published by Elsevier Ltd. Used by permission. Submitted October 12, 2009; revised November 19, 2009; accepted January 11, 2010; published online February 19, 2010. Female mate choice based upon male motor performance John Byers,1 Eileen Hebets,2 and Jeffrey Podos 3 1. Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, ID 83844-3051, USA 2. School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA 3. Department of Biology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA Corresponding author — J. Byers, email [email protected] Abstract Our goal in this essay is to review the hypothesis that females choose mates by the evaluation of male motor performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Complex Sexual Courtship Displays by Luminescent Male Marine Ostracods
    2252 The Journal of Experimental Biology 211, 2252-2262 Published by The Company of Biologists 2008 doi:10.1242/jeb.011130 Complex sexual courtship displays by luminescent male marine ostracods Trevor J. Rivers* and James G. Morin Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, NY 85201, USA *Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]) Accepted 21 April 2008 SUMMARY In the western Caribbean Sea, about an hour after the sun sets, a complex and ritualized light show of precise, vertically placed luminescent pulses erupts over shallow grassbeds. These are among the most complex displays known in marine systems. Displays consist of repeated trains of secreted bioluminescent pulses in a specific pattern ejected into the water column as courtship signals by male Vargula annecohenae, which are small (<2 mm) myodocopid ostracod crustaceans. Although these animals display in near darkness, we have used image intensification and infrared videography and three-dimensional analysis in the lab to demonstrate that each luminescent display train, which can be up to 60 cm long, consists of two distinct luminescent and swimming phases. The first, or ‘stationary,’ phase consists of three (usually) bright, longer pulses placed close together, with the male swimming in a looping pattern. We hypothesize that this pattern acts as an attention-grabbing signal for receptive females. The stationary phase is followed by the ‘helical phase,’ which consists of about a dozen evenly placed dimmer, shorter pulses secreted by an individual male rapidly spiraling upward in a helical pattern. We hypothesize that this phase, which has very uniform interpulse intervals and distances, helps an approaching female target and intercept the rapidly moving male.
    [Show full text]