<<

UCLA UCLA Previously Published Works

Title An assessment of marking techniques for odonates in the family

Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1m5071bs

Journal Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 141(3)

ISSN 00138703

Authors Anderson, Christopher N Cordoba-Aguilar, Alex Drury, Jonathan P et al.

Publication Date 2011-12-01

DOI 10.1111/j.1570-7458.2011.01185.x

Peer reviewed

eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California DOI: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.2011.01185.x TECHNICAL NOTE An assessment of marking techniques for odonates in the family Calopterygidae

Christopher N. Anderson1*, Alex Cordoba-Aguilar1,JonathanP.Drury2 & Gregory F.Grether2 1Departamento de Ecologı´a Evolutiva, Instituto de Ecologı´a, Universidad Nacional Auto´noma de Me´xico, Circuito Exterior s ⁄ n, Apdo. Postal 70-275, Me´xico, D.F. 04510, Mexico, and 2Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California at Los Angeles, 621 Charles E. Young Drive South, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1606, USA

Accepted: 13 September 2011

Key words: , paint and ink marking, mark-capture, haemorrhoidalis, titia,damselfly

tion. These manipulations significantly influenced mating Introduction success (Grether, 1996a,b), foraging success (Grether & Marking effects (e.g., changes to behavior, survival, or Grey, 1996), survival (Grether, 1997), and territorial reproduction after manipulation) are likely to be com- aggression against conspecifics and, in some instances, mon, as researchers have found effects in a variety of taxa, against heterospecifics (Anderson & Grether, 2010, 2011). including mammals (Moorhouse & Macdonald, 2005), Therefore, using wing marking to identify individuals may birds (Burley et al., 1982; Hunt et al., 1997; Gauthier-Clerc be problematic for investigations of reproductive, territo- et al., 2004), and amphibians (McCarthy & Parris, 2004). rial, and foraging behavior of these species and their Prior to the advent of the felt-tipped marker, marking relatives. techniques for odonates ranged from India ink to various Herein, we describe an alternative, simple, and effective types of paint carefully applied to the wings or abdomen marking technique for calopterygid damselflies and evalu- (Borror, 1934; Bick & Bick, 1961; Cordero Rivera & Stoks, ate potential effects on biology and behavior. Briefly, 2008). In recent years, most odonate biologists mark indi- abdominal sections 3–6 are marked with unique combina- vidual odonates by writing a unique identification code tions of paint marks. We report on the use of this marking with permanent marker on the wings (Cordero Rivera & technique from two calopterygid populations: a popu- Stoks, 2008). Despite the recommendation to carefully test lation of Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis (Vander Linden) and marking techniques (Hagler & Jackson, 2001), few a population of (Drury) (both Odonata: researchers describe more than a qualitative assessment of Calopterygidae). For each population, we investigate potential marking effects. Furthermore, few researchers whether specific colors painted on the abdomen affect the have compared alternative marking techniques for probability of resighting. Furthermore, in a subsequent odonates (but see Bennett & Mill, 1995). study, the C. haemorrhoidalis population was marked Natural variation in the extent of odonate wing colora- using the common technique of writing a number on the tion may influence inter- and intra-sexual interactions wing with a marker. This allowed us to directly compare (Tynkkynen et al., 2004; Anderson & Grether, 2010), as the two aforementioned odonate marking techniques. To well as foraging success (Grether & Grey, 1996) and the investigate the effects of abdominal coloring on territorial likelihood of predation by avian predators (Svensson & aggression, we utilized an experimental approach to com- Friberg, 2007). Prior studies in the calopterygid damselfly pare aggression toward manipulated and unmanipulated Hetaerina used markers to experimentally increase territorial intruders in the H. titia population. the extent of red (Grether, 1996a,b; Grether & Grey, 1996) and black (Anderson & Grether, 2010, 2011) wing colora- Materials and methods Localities and behavioral observations *Correspondence: Christopher N. Anderson, Departamento de Eco- Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis observations were carried out logı´a Evolutiva, Instituto de Ecologı´a, Universidad Nacional Auto´no- ma de Me´xico, Circuito Exterior s ⁄ n, Apdo. Postal 70-275, Me´xico, along a 200-m transect near a small stream near Ponteve- D.F. 04510, Mexico. E-mail: [email protected] dra, Spain (42.406N, 8.684W) in May 1996. In the first

2011 The Authors Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 1–4, 2011 Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 2011 The Netherlands Entomological Society 1 2 Anderson et al. investigation conducted from May 2 to 6, 80 C. haemor- Territorial intrusion tests with Hetaerina titia rhoidalis individuals were captured with aerial nets and To determine whether abdominal marking per se (as held carefully by the wing while marked on the abdomen opposed to any individual color) influenced territorial with a unique combination of four colored marks. behavior, we carried out experimental territorial intru- Marked individuals were resighted for four subsequent sions. During a territorial intrusion test, an experimental days after marking. The four colored marks were a unique or control tethered male was presented using 0.3 m of combination of four Humbrol Pinturas acrylic paint col- fine transparent thread and a modified fishing pole to a ors (white: #22, orange: #18, yellow: #69, and lime: #38; territory holder. During the 2-min test, an observer Humbrol LTD, Kingston-upon-Hull, UK). Paint was recorded observations on a continuously running audio applied by immersing the tip of a sharpened pencil into recorder. A total of 14 H. titia territory holders were the paint and gently applying a dot (ca. 1.2 mm diame- tested consecutively against control and experimental ter) upon consecutive abdominal segments. Colors were tethered intruders within a 20–30 min window (so that, applied in a predetermined random order. Randomiza- each territory holder was tested twice). Experimental tion was completed by listing all possible color combina- individuals were given three colored marks on the abdo- tions and then applying the RAND function in Microsoft men, whereas control individuals received similar han- Excel in an adjacent column. We then sorted both col- dling, but remained uncolored. During a tethering test, umns by the random column. In a subsequent investiga- movement of the modified fishing pole ensured continu- tion conducted from May 4 to 8 at the same site, 80 ous flight from the tethered male. A minimum of 5 min C. haemorrhoidalis individuals were marked using a white elapsed between consecutive tests on a territorial male, Sharpie Brand marker (Extra fine point; Sanford, Oak and the order of treatments was varied systematically Brook, IL, USA). The left hindwing of each captured indi- (trial order has not significantly influenced the degree of vidual was marked with a unique three-digit numbered aggressive response in similar prior investigations; Ander- code selected randomly between 001 and 300. As with the son & Grether, 2010). Tethered males were obtained from first investigation, resighting continued for 4 days after stream sections adjacent to the study transect and used marking. for at most two tests before release outside the transect. Hetaerina titia observations were carried out in July– Following the tests, both control and experimental males August 2010 along the Otapa River southeast of La Tinaja, were uniquely marked to prevent use in future tests and Mexico (18.687N, 96.394W). Two study transects, (each returned to their site of origin. All simulated intruder 200 m) ca. 1 km apart were established along riverbanks. tests were carried out between 10:00 and 18:00 hours Damselflies were captured with aerial nets, photographed, under sunny or lightly cloudy conditions (territory and individually marked with a unique combination of defense wanes under heavy cloud cover). four colored marks on the abdomen using paint pens (200-S Fine Point, Marvy Decocolor Paint Marker; Uchida Statistical analysis of America, Torrance, CA, USA).The colored marks encir- We used contingency table analyses (v2 tests) to com- cled abdominal segments 3–6 and typically were as thick as pare the probability of resighting for individuals within the marker tip (ca. 1.2 mm diameter). The four colored a population marked with a specific color to the proba- marks were randomly selected from a set of seven colors bility of resighting for all other individuals in the popu- (white: #0, yellow: #5, orange: #7, light blue: #10, light lation (for a similar approach, see Hagan & Reed, 1988). green: #11, rosemarie: #59, and hot purple: #79. A prede- A contingency table approach was also used to compare termined random order was obtained as above. In total, between marking methods (abdomen coloring vs. wing 416 H. titia received unique colored marks. numbering). We used the Wilcoxon matched pairs During a behavioral census, we recorded each individ- signed-rank test to compare the response of territory ual’s perch location relative to the transect line to the near- holders (rate of aggressive attacks) to intruders with col- est 0.1 m. A male was classified as a territory holder if it ored abdomens vs. unmanipulated intruders. Statistical was consistently seen at the same location (+1.5 m) on at analyses were conducted in Stata 10 (StataCorp, College least two consecutive days. Following marking, the H. titia Station, TX, USA). population was censused for eight consecutive days. All individuals, territorial or not, were used for the inves- Results tigation of color effects on the probability of resighting. Individuals identified as territorial were subject to For H. titia, probabilities of resighting, given a specific paired intrusions to investigate the effects of marking on color ranged from 46.4 to 51.6%. No single color signifi- territorial behavior (see below). cantly affected the probability of resighting (Table 1). In Marking techniques for odonates 3

Table 1 The probabilities of resighting for white, pink, orange, was not significantly different from the rate of attack yellow, green, blue, and violet-colored Hetaerina titia damselflies toward unmanipulated intruders (signed-rank test: z = 1.193, P = 0.23; n = 14). Probability n of resighting P Discussion White 198 0.510 0.84 Non-white 218 0.500 Our results suggest that marking the abdomen with opa- Pink 183 0.464 0.15 que paint is a viable alternative to wing numbering for Non-pink 233 0.536 behavioral investigations of calopterygid damselflies. Our Orange 179 0.497 0.79 results do not concur with prior claims that calopterygids Non-orange 237 0.511 show negligible effects when populations are uniquely Yellow 201 0.493 0.63 marked by wing numbering (Cordero Rivera & Stoks, Non-yellow 215 0.516 Green 192 0.516 0.68 2008). A direct comparison of these two techniques Non-green 224 0.496 showed that abdominal painting was associated with Blue 178 0.500 0.87 higher resighting rates in the C. haemorrhoidalis popu- Non-blue 238 0.508 lation. We failed to detect any significant effect of any Violet 184 0.516 0.68 particular color on the probability of resighting – despite Non-violet 232 0.496 having robust sample sizes (H. titia,n=417;C. haemor- rhoidalis,n=80). Individuals with one or more abdominal mark(s) of the focal color are compared with all other individuals without the Several reasons may account for why painting the abdo- focal color; n, number of damselflies. P-values are from v2 men appears to be associated with an increased resighting contingency tests. rate over marking the wings. First, wing marking may affect conspecific or interspecific aggression and thus affect territory occupancy time. Wing pigmentation is an indica- C. haemorrhoidalis, probabilities of resighting given a spe- tor of fighting ability in several calopterygid species (Cor- cific color ranged from 70.9 to 77.4%. As with H. titia,no doba-Aguilar, 2002; reviewed by Suhonen et al., 2008) so color significantly affected the probability of resighting in that a change in the wing pigmented area may change the C. haemorrhoidalis study (Table 2). A significantly other males’ perception of the marked male’s fighting abil- higher proportion of C. haemorrhoidalis was resighted in ity. Second, wing marking may increase detection by pre- the census following abdomen marking (73.8%) than in dators, as natural variation in odonate wing color may 2 the census following wing marking (55.0%; v =6.13, affect predation (Svensson & Friberg, 2007). Abdomen d.f. = 1, P = 0.013). painting could also affect how odonates are perceived by In the H. titia territory intrusion tests, the average rate potential competitors and predators; however, and this of attack toward abdomen marked conspecific intruders needs to be investigated. In the H. titia population, we compared rates of aggres- sion by territory holders against tethered intruders with Table 2 The probability of resighting for white, yellow, orange, painted and unmanipulated abdomens. No significant dif- and green-colored Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis damselflies ferences were found in measurements of aggression against the two types of intruders. In contrast, prior studies using Probability an identical tethering protocol found effects of wing spot n of resighting P size manipulations on the extent of territorial aggression White 53 0.774 0.30 elicited (Anderson & Grether, 2010). Abdominal painting Non-white 27 0.667 appears to be a preferable method of individually marking Yellow 53 0.755 0.62 H. titia, at least when the scope of the study includes an Non-yellow 27 0.704 investigation of territorial behavior. Orange 55 0.709 0.39 We conclude that painting abdomens of individual Non-orange 25 0.800 calopterygid damselflies with unique paint color combina- Green 56 0.732 0.87 tions is a simple, effective method of individual identifica- Non-green 24 0.750 tion. Abdominal painting yielded resighting proportions Individuals with one or more abdominal mark of the focal color of close to 50% for H. titia damselflies, and close to 75% are compared with all other individuals without the focal color; n, for C. haemorrhoidalis damselflies. One explanation for number of damselflies. P-values are from v2 contingency tests. these different resighting rates is a difference in site fidelity 4 Anderson et al. between the two species. Damselflies in the family Calo- Burley N, Krantzberg G & Radman P (1982) Influence of color- pterygidae (also known as ‘broad-winged damselflies’) are banding on the conspecific preferences of zebra finches. Ani- among the most robust odonates in the suborder Zygo- mal Behaviour 30: 444–455. ptera (Corbet, 1999). Rings of opaque paint may not be Corbet PS (1999) Dragonflies: Behavior and Ecology of Odonata. appropriate for damselflies in other families (e.g., Coena- Comstock Publishing Associates, Ithaca, NY, USA. Cordero Rivera A & Stoks R (2008) Mark-recapture studies and grionidae), as the added weight to the abdomen may affect demography. Dragonflies and Damselflies: Model Organisms the efficacy of flight. Furthermore, while we did not find for Ecological and Evolutionary Research (ed. by A Cordoba- any effect of any individual color for calopterygids, Aguilar), pp. 7–20. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. researchers using the method for other odonates could Cordoba-Aguilar A (2002) Wing pigmentation in territorial male consider whether the colors used should preferably be damselflies, Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis: a possible relation to ‘camouflage colors’ (e.g., green, brown) instead of ‘warn- sexual selection. Behaviour 63: 759–766. ing colors’ (e.g., bright yellow, red). We concur with the Gauthier-Clerc M, Gendner JP, Ribic CA, Fraser WR, Woehler EJ recommendation of Hagler & Jackson (2001) to carefully et al. (2004) Long-term effects of flipper bands on penguins. test marking techniques prior to behavioral study. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 271: S423–S426. Grether GF (1996a) Sexual selection and survival selection on wing coloration and body size in the rubyspot damselfly Hetae- Acknowledgements rina americana. Evolution 50: 1939–1948. Grether GF (1996b) Intrasexual competition alone favors a sexu- We thank Patrick Green and Andrew Chao for assis- ally dimorphic ornament in the rubyspot damselfly Hetaerina tance with fieldwork, and Neil Losin for comments on americana. Evolution 50: 1949–1957. the manuscript. CNA was supported by a CONACyT Grether GF (1997) Survival cost of an intrasexually selected orna- postdoctoral fellowship and a UCMEXUS-CONACyT ment in a damselfly. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Lon- postdoctoral fellowship during this project. An NSF don B 264: 207–210. Graduate Research Fellowship supported JPD during Grether GF & Grey RM (1996) Novel cost of a sexually selected the completion of this investigation. Parts of this pro- trait in the rubyspot damselfly Hetaerina americana: conspicu- ject were supported by NSF grant DEB-1020586, ousness to prey. Behavioral Ecology 7: 465–473. UCMEXUS-CONACyT collaborative grant CN-10-402, Hagan JM & Reed JM (1988) Red color bands reduce fledgling and a UCLA Academic Senate Grant. The authors success in red-cockaded woodpeckers. Auk 105: 498–503. Hagler JR & Jackson CG (2001) Methods for marking : agree to make the data used in the preparation of this current techniques and future prospects. Annual Review of manuscript available upon reasonable request. Entomology 46: 511–543. Hunt S, Cuthill IC, Swaddle JP & Bennett ATD (1997) Ultraviolet References vision and band-colour preferences in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behaviour 54: 1383–1392. Anderson CN & Grether GF (2010) Interspecific aggression and McCarthy MA & Parris KM (2004) Clarifying the effect of toe character displacement of competitor recognition in Hetaerina clipping on frogs with Bayesian statistics. Journal of Applied damselflies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 277: Ecology 41: 780–786. 549–555. Moorhouse TP & Macdonald DW (2005) Indirect negative Anderson CN & Grether GF (2011) Multiple routes to reduced impacts of radio-collaring: sex ratio variation in water voles. interspecific territorial fighting in Hetaerina damselflies. Journal of Applied Ecology 42: 91–98. Behavioral Ecology 22: 527–534. Suhonen J, Rantala MJ & Honkavaara J (2008) Territoriality in Bennett S & Mill PJ (1995) Pre-maturation and post-maturation odonates. Dragonflies and Damselflies: Model Organisms for survival in adults of the damselfly Pyrrhosoma nymphula (Zy- Ecological and Evolutionary Research (ed. by A Cordoba- goptera, Coenagrionidae). Journal of Zoology 235: 559–575. Aguilar), pp. 203–217. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Bick GH & Bick JC (1961) An adult population of Lestes disjunc- Svensson EI & Friberg M (2007) Selective predation on wing tus australis Walker (Odonata: Lestidae). Southwestern Natu- morphology in sympatric damselflies. American Naturalist ralist 6: 111–137. 170: 101–112. Borror DJ (1934) Ecological studies of Argia moesta Hagen Tynkkynen K, Rantala MJ & Suhonen J (2004) Interspecific (Odonata: Coenagrionidae) by means of marking. Ohio aggression and character displacement in the damselfly Calo- Journal of Science 34: 97–108. pteryx splendens. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 17: 759–767.