Phonology: the Function and Patterning of Sounds

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Phonology: the Function and Patterning of Sounds three PHONOLOGY: THE FUNCTION AND PATTERNING OF SOUNDS Michael Dobrovolsky A person's tongue is a twisty thing, there are plenty of words there of every kind, and the range of words is wide, and their variation. - HOMER, The Iliad, 20 We saw in Chapter 2 that there are a large number of speech sounds that human beings can produce and perceive. No human language exploits all of these possibil- ities. Furthermore, the sounds of all languages are patterned and organized in such ways that linguists can discover some system that underlies their appearance. Linguists generally assume that speakers have (at least) some subconscious knowl- edge of this system. For example, we saw in Chapter 1 that English speakers recog- nize that forms like slish and screnk are acceptable, while forms like srish and screpk are not. But speakers can do more than note that certain forms are unnatural in their system; they can also correct such forms to make them acceptable in their own lan- guage. Without knowing exactly why, most English speakers would pronounce a form like srish as [sari/] rather than saying [nj] or [si/]. It is this largely subconscious knowledge of sound patterns that phonologists are interested in uncovering. This chapter is about phonology, the component of a grammar made up of the elements and principles that determine how sounds vary and pattern in a language. Phonologists attempt to make explicit formal statements about the sound patterns of individual languages in order to discover something about the linguistic knowl- edge that people must have in order to use these patterns. Even more broadly, the study of phonology attempts to discover general principles that underlie the pat- terning of sounds in human language. The existence of patterns in language depends on the organization of certain basic elements or units that combine to make up these patterns. Three major units of analysis will be presented in this chapter. We are already acquainted with the idea that the flow of speech can be divided into segments. In this chapter, we investigate 64 CHAPTER THREE the patterned variation of segments. A second unit of phonological analysis is the feature. Features may be thought of as the smallest building blocks of phonologi- cal structure, corresponding as they do to articulatory or acoustic categories such as [voice] or [strident]. We will also learn how segments are built up from features and how this fact is connected with phonological patterning. We will also investigate the properties of syllables in phonology. A syllable is a unit of linguistic structure that consists of a syllabic element—usually a vowel—and any segments that are associ- ated with it. (The word segment itself can be divided into two syllables: seg and ment.) These units—feature, segment, and syllable—interact with the processes we investi- gated in the previous chapter and with certain general principles to produce the sound patterns of language. The existence of patterned elements implies that there must be some principles of organization. Features, segments, and syllables are organized into hierarchical lev- els; each level is composed of elements at the level beneath it. In Figure 3.1, the word-level unit segment is represented by the abbreviation Wd. This word consists of two syllables, each of which is represented with the Greek letter a (sigma). Each syl- lable is made up of a number of segments (the internal structure of syllables is also hierarchical, and is treated in Section 5 of this chapter). Each segment, in turn, is composed of features. A representation of segment is given in Figure 3.1. (For pur- poses of illustration, only a few features are provided for each segment.) Wd Word level Syllable level Segment level -syllabic +syllabic -syllabic -syllabic +syllabic -syllabic -syllabic Feature level -sonorant +sonorant -sonorant +sonorant +sonorant +sonorant -sonorant •9 '• +coronal -high +high +labial +reduced +nasal +coronal Figure 3.1 Partial phonological representation of segment Us ) In the next sections, we look at the phonological knowledge that enables speak- ers to distinguish among forms and to deal with the considerable phonetic variation Si found in the pronunciation of speech segments. £..;'• E:, 1 SEGMENTS IN CONTRAST All speakers know which segments of their language contrast. Segments are said to Ill: contrast (or to be distinctive or be in opposition) when their presence alone may dis- tinguish forms with different meanings from each other. The segments [s] and [z] contrast in the words sip and zip, as do the vowels of hit, hate, and hot. lit:: PHONOLOGY: THE FUNCTION AND PATTERNING OF SOUNDS 65 1.1 MINIMAL PAIRS A minimal pair consists of two forms with distinct meanings that differ by only one segment found in the same position in each form. The examples [sip] sip and [zipj zip given previously form a minimal pair and show that the sounds [s] and [z] contrast in English. A number of minimal pairs that demonstrate consonant contrasts for English are given in Table 3.1; remember it is on the basis of sound and not spelling that mini- mal pairs are established. In displaying contrasts, contrasting words are often placed along the horizontal axis with respect to their place of articulation, reading from left to right (labial, alveolar, and so on), and vertically with respect to manner of articu- lation, in order to show which places and manners of articulation are exploited by the language in question. Table 3.1 Contrasts among consonants in English Labial Interdental Alveolar Alveopalatal Velar Glottal Stops and affricates (noncontinuants) tap [p] pat [t] match ItJl pick [k] tab |b] pad [d] Madge Pig Igl Continuants leaf [f] thigh [9] sip [s| mesher in hip [h] . leave [v] thy [5] zip fz] measure bi Nasals sum [m] sun fn] sung [rjj Liquids and glides wet [w] yet lil leer [1] rear [r] The phonetic context in which a sound occurs is called its environment. Pairs that show segments in nearly identical environments, such as azure/assure or author/either, are called near-minimal pairs. They help to establish contrasts where no minimal pairs can be found. You may assume that two segments contrast once a minimal pair or a near- minimal pair has been established. It is in fact rare to find minimal pairs for all dis- tinctive sounds in all environments in a language, since the historical evolution of every language has led to some sounds being utilized more frequently than others, or being eliminated from some environments. For example, you will find no mini- mal pairs involving [h] and [n] in word-initial or word-final position in English, because there are no words that begin with [rj] or end in [h]. It is also difficult to find 66 CHAPTER THREE minimal pairs in English that have the phone [3], which occurs for the most part in words borrowed from French such as azure and mirage. Vowel contrasts in English Contrasts among English vowels can be established with a few sets of examples. For now, we will continue to assume that English vowel-glide sequences like [ij], [uw], [ow], and so on are single vowels. From this perspective, we can say that the vowels [ij] and [1], [ej[ and [e], and so on, contrast. Table 3.2 Vowel contrasts in American English beet Ibijt] [ij] bit [bit] [i] bait [bejt] [ejl bet [bet] [el bat [bat] l*\ cooed [k"uwdi [uw] 2 P could [k'-ud] [0] V code [khowd| [owj A caught [k»3t] t>] cot [k"at] [a] cut [k"At] [A| lewd [luwd] [uwj loud [lawd| [aw] lied [Iajd] [aj] Lloyd [lojd| [ojl 1.2 LANGUAGE-SPECIFIC CONTRASTS Contrasts are language-specific; sounds that are distinctive in one language will not necessarily be distinctive in another. For example, the difference between the two 2.1 vowels [e] and [ae] is crucial to English, as we can see from minimal pairs like Ben [ben] and ban [baen]. But in Turkish, this difference in pronunciation is not distinc- tive. A Turkish speaker may pronounce the word for 'I' as [ben] or [baen], and it will make no difference to the meaning. Table 3.3 Language-specific vowel contrasts: English versus Turkish English Turkish [ben] Ben [benl [baen] ban [baen] Conversely, sounds that do not contrast in English, such as long and short vowels, may be distinctive in another language. There are no minimal pairs of the type [haet]:[hae:t] or [luws]:[lu:ws] in English. But in Japanese and Finnish, short and long vowels contrast, as the next examples show. PHONOLOGY: THE FUNCTION AND PATTERNING OF SOUNDS 67 Table 3.4 Short/long vowel contrasts in Japanese and Finnish Japanese [tori] 'bird' [tori:] 'shrine gate' [kiboj 'scale' [kibo:] 'hope' Finnish [tuli] 'fire' [tu:li] 'wind' [haeta?] 'distress' [ha?:tae:] 'to evict' Establishing the contrasting segments in a language is a first step in phonological analysis. But in any language, there are many sounds that never contrast. The fol- lowing section deals with this aspect of phonological analysis. PHONETICALLY CONDITIONED VARIATION: PHONEMES AND ALLOPHONES Everyday speech contains a great deal of phonetic variation that speakers pay little or no attention to. Some of this variation arises from nonlinguistic factors such as fatigue, excitement, orthodontic work, gum chewing, and the like. This kind of vari- ation is not part of the domain of phonology. But much phonetic variation is sys- tematic. It occurs most often among phonetically similar segments and is conditioned by the phonetic context or environment in which the segments are found. This vari- ation occurs because segments are affected and altered by the phonetic characteristics of neighboring elements or the larger phonological context in which they occur.
Recommended publications
  • The Phonetic Nature of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic
    www.sciedupress.com/elr English Linguistics Research Vol. 4, No. 3; 2015 The Phonetic Nature of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic Mohammad Yahya Bani Salameh1 1 Tabuk University, Saudi Arabia Correspondence: Mohammad Yahya Bani Salameh, Tabuk University, Saudi Arabia. Tel: 966-58-0323-239. E-mail: [email protected] Received: June 29, 2015 Accepted: July 29, 2015 Online Published: August 5, 2015 doi:10.5430/elr.v4n3p30 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5430/elr.v4n3p30 Abstract The aim of this paper is to discuss the phonetic nature of Arabic consonants in Modern Standard Arabic (MSA). Although Arabic is a Semitic language, the speech sound system of Arabic is very comprehensive. Data used for this study were collocated from the standard speech of nine informants who are native speakers of Arabic. The researcher used himself as informant, He also benefited from three other Jordanians and four educated Yemenis. Considering the alphabets as the written symbols used for transcribing the phones of actual pronunciation, it was found that the pronunciation of many Arabic sounds has gradually changed from the standard. The study also discussed several related issues including: Phonetic Description of Arabic consonants, classification of Arabic consonants, types of Arabic consonants and distribution of Arabic consonants. Keywords: Modern Standard Arabic (MSA), Arabic consonants, Dialectal variation, Consonants distribution, Consonants classification. 1. Introduction The Arabic language is one of the most important languages of the world. With it is growing importance of Arab world in the International affairs, the importance of Arabic language has reached to the greater heights. Since the holy book Qura'n is written in Arabic, the language has a place of special prestige in all Muslim societies, and therefore more and more Muslims and Asia, central Asia, and Africa are learning the Arabic language, the language of their faith.
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry of Non-Distinctive Phonetic Differences Between Language Varieties *
    Mimicry of Non-distinctive Phonetic Differences Between Language Varieties * James Emil Flege Ro bert M. Ham mond Unil'l![sifyof Alabama. Birmingham Miami·Dade Community College 1\ delayed mimicry paradigm w.as used to assess speakers' ,awareness of non-dlSlLnCf!Vc phonetic differences which in part dislinguisn I:mguagcs, . The notion of " rho no logIcal fillu­ Lng" IffiroheS ttur second Lmsuagc learners may not be lIblc to perceIve: phonetic differences between thCIT native language and :01 fOrf:.gn langua~ unless the phonetic differences arc linguistically relevant in the nallve bngu2gc. If cross-bogulIgc phoncuc differences arc In fact perceIved poorly, It is unlikely [hal phon.::!;\; mod,ficallon , ..,II OCcur .n Ihc course of nalUu.hsllc second language acqu15iuon. In th.s study natlvC' English speakers familiar Wllh Spamsh·accentcd English ulempted to lead sentences With a Spamsh accent. Acoustic mC::l5Uremc:nts showed thai twO phonellc characlens!!cs of Engllsh-Ihe long VOT values aSSOC iated '''llh 'p, t, kI and hnal-syll:Jble lengthening- were altered In the direction of Spanuh anu Spanish.accented Enghsh. These results prOVide tentative eYldence that nOn -dlStlnCIIVe rhonetle differences between languages arc detecuble by I:lnguage learners and thus do nO! present an msupeuble bamer to phonetIC learning in second 1:lOgU .. gc "cqulsHlun For several decades phonologists have believed that "phonological fillering" con· tnbutes Importantly to the presence of foreign accent In the speech of second languagc learners ITrubct;:koy 1969:5 1 ffI. Having acquired the phonology of one language, the language learner will tend to mterpret sounds heard in a foreign language in terms of phonological umts found In the native language.
    [Show full text]
  • Gradient Phonemic Contrast in Nanjing Mandarin Keith Johnson (UC Berkeley) Yidan Song (Nanjing Normal University)
    UC Berkeley Phonetics and Phonology Lab Annual Report (2016) Gradient phonemic contrast in Nanjing Mandarin Keith Johnson (UC Berkeley) Yidan Song (Nanjing Normal University) Abstract Sounds that are contrastive in a language are rated by listeners as being more different from each other than sounds that don’t occur in the language or sounds that are allophones of a single phoneme. The study reported in this paper replicates this finding and adds new data on the perceptual impact of learning a language with a new contrast. Two groups of speakers of the Nanjing dialect of Mandarin Chinese were tested. One group was older and had not been required to learn standard Mandarin as school children, while the other younger group had learned standard Mandarin in school. Nanjing dialect does not contrast [n] and [l], while standard Mandarin does. Listeners rated the similarity of naturally produced non-words presented in pairs, where the only difference between the tokens was the medial consonant. Pairs contrasting [n] and [l] were rated by older Nanjing speakers as if two [n] tokens or two [l] tokens had been presented, while these same pairs were rated by younger Nanjing speakers as noticeably different but not as different as pairs that contrast in their native language. Introduction The variety of Mandarin Chinese spoken in Nanjing does not have a contrast between [n] and [l] (Song, 2015), while the standard variety which is now taught in schools in Nanjing does have this contrast. Prior research has shown that speech perception is modulated by linguistic experience. Years of research has shown that listeners find it very difficult to perceive differences between sounds that are not contrastive in their language (Goto, 1971; Werker & Tees, 1984; Flege, 1995; Best et al., 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • Introductory Phonology
    3 More on Phonemes 3.1 Phonemic Analysis and Writing The question of phonemicization is in principle independent from the question of writing; that is, there is no necessary connection between letters and phonemes. For example, the English phoneme /e}/ can be spelled in quite a few ways: say /se}/, Abe /e}b/, main /me}n/, beige /be}è/, reggae /cryge}/, H /e}tà/. Indeed, there are languages (for example, Mandarin Chinese) that are written with symbols that do not correspond to phonemes at all. Obviously, there is at least a loose connection between alphabetic letters and phonemes: the designers of an alphabet tend to match up the written symbols with the phonemes of a language. Moreover, the conscious intuitions of speakers about sounds tend to be heavily influenced by their knowledge of spelling – after all, most literate speakers receive extensive training in how to spell during child- hood, but no training at all in phonology. Writing is prestigious, and our spoken pronunciations are sometimes felt to be imperfect realizations of what is written. This is reflected in the common occur- rence of spelling pronunciations, which are pronunciations that have no historical basis, but which arise as attempts to mimic the spelling, as in often [cÑftvn] or palm [pwlm]. In contrast, most linguists feel that spoken language is primary, and that written language is a derived system, which is mostly parasitic off the spoken language and is often rather artificial in character. Some reasons that support this view are that spoken language is far older than writing, it is acquired first and with greater ease by children, and it is the common property of our species, rather than of just an educated subset of it.
    [Show full text]
  • Minimal Pair Approaches to Phonological Remediation
    Minimal Pair Approaches to Phonological Remediation Jessica A. Barlow, Ph.D.,1 and Judith A. Gierut, Ph.D.2 ABSTRACT This article considers linguistic approaches to phonological reme- diation that emphasize the role of the phoneme in language. We discuss the structure and function of the phoneme by outlining procedures for de- termining contrastive properties of sound systems through evaluation of minimal word pairs. We then illustrate how these may be applied to a case study of a child with phonological delay. The relative effectiveness of treat- ment approaches that facilitate phonemic acquisition by contrasting pairs of sounds in minimal pairs is described. A hierarchy of minimal pair treat- ment efficacy emerges, as based on the number of new sounds, the number of featural differences, and the type of featural differences being intro- duced. These variables are further applied to the case study, yielding a range of possible treatment recommendations that are predicted to vary in their effectiveness. KEYWORDS: Phoneme, minimal pair, phonological remediation Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the reader will be able to (1) analyze and recognize the con- trastive function of phonemes in a phonological system, (2) develop minimal pair treatment programs that aim to introduce phonemic contrasts in a child’s phonological system, and (3) discriminate between different types of minimal pair treatment programs and their relative effectiveness. Models of clinical treatment for children cognition given our need to understand how with functional phonological delays have been learning takes place in the course of interven- based on three general theoretical frameworks. tion. Still other approaches are grounded in Some models are founded on development linguistics because the problem at hand in- given that the population of concern involves volves the phonological system.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 the Phoneme
    11 The Phoneme B. Elan Dresher 1 Introduction The concept of the phoneme was central to the development of phonological theory. In the early twentieth century, phonological theory was all about the phoneme: how to define it, how to recognize it, how to discover it (see, for example, the articles selected for inclusion in Joos 1957 and Makkai 1972). The American structuralist term for phonology, phonemics, indicates to what extent the field was considered to be about the phoneme. Things have now changed. The phoneme, to all appearances, no longer holds a central place in phonological theory. Two recent and voluminous handbooks devoted to phonology, edited by Goldsmith (1995) and by de Lacy (2007), have no chapter on the phoneme. It is barely mentioned in the indexes. This does not mean that the phoneme plays no role in modern phonology; closer inspection reveals that the phoneme is far from dead. However, it is not much talked about, and when it is, it is more often to dispute its existence than to affirm it. Such a dramatic change in fortunes for a concept bears some looking into, and this chapter will be devoted to trying to understand what has happened to the phoneme in its journey into the twenty-first century, and what its prospects are for the future. 2 Origins of the term S. R. Anderson (1985: 38) cites Godel (1957) and Jakobson (1971) as locating the origin of the term phoneme in the French word phonème, coined in the early 1870s by the French linguist Dufriche-Desgenettes. He proposed the term to substitute for the German Sprachlaut (“speech sound”), so it did not have the modern sense of phoneme, but rather corresponded to what we would now call “speech sound” or “phone.” The term was taken up by Saussure (1879), who used it in yet a different sense, and from Saussure it was taken up by the Polish Kazan school linguists Jan Baudouin de Courtenay and MikoÓaj Kruszewski.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rhaeto-Romance Languages
    Romance Linguistics Editorial Statement Routledge publish the Romance Linguistics series under the editorship of Martin Harris (University of Essex) and Nigel Vincent (University of Manchester). Romance Philogy and General Linguistics have followed sometimes converging sometimes diverging paths over the last century and a half. With the present series we wish to recognise and promote the mutual interaction of the two disciplines. The focus is deliberately wide, seeking to encompass not only work in the phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, and lexis of the Romance languages, but also studies in the history of Romance linguistics and linguistic thought in the Romance cultural area. Some of the volumes will be devoted to particular aspects of individual languages, some will be comparative in nature; some will adopt a synchronic and some a diachronic slant; some will concentrate on linguistic structures, and some will investigate the sociocultural dimensions of language and language use in the Romance-speaking territories. Yet all will endorse the view that a General Linguistics that ignores the always rich and often unique data of Romance is as impoverished as a Romance Philogy that turns its back on the insights of linguistics theory. Other books in the Romance Linguistics series include: Structures and Transformations Christopher J. Pountain Studies in the Romance Verb eds Nigel Vincent and Martin Harris Weakening Processes in the History of Spanish Consonants Raymond Harris-N orthall Spanish Word Formation M.F. Lang Tense and Text
    [Show full text]
  • Arabic Phonology Janua Linguarum
    ARABIC PHONOLOGY JANUA LINGUARUM STUDIA MEMORIAE NICOLAI VAN WIJK DEDICATA edenda curat C. H. VAN SCHOONEVELD INDIANA UNIVERSITY SERIES PRACTICA 61 1970 MOUTON THE HAGUE · PARIS ARABIC PHONOLOGY An Acoustical and Physiological Investigation by SALMAN H. AL-ANI INDIANA UNIVERSITY 1970 MOUTON THE HAGUE . PARIS © Copyright 1970 in The Netherlands. Mouton & Co. N.V., Publishers, The Hague. No part of this book may be translated or reproduced in any form by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publishers. LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOG CARD NUMBER: 70-91210 Printed in The Netherlands by Mouton & Co., Printers, The Hague. This book is dedicated to the late Dr. Gordon E. Peterson whose untimely death occurred before its publication. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am most grateful to Dr. Wadie Jwaideh of Indiana University who encouraged me throughout my university years and to Dr. Fred W. Householder, also of Indiana University, who was my thesis director. To Dr. Charles J. Adams, Director of the Institute of Islamic Studies at McGill University, I owe a great debt of thanks for his help in securing financial aid and for his understanding and faith. Also at McGill, I wish to thank Dr. A. Rigault and Mr. J. Frydman for their able assistance with the acoustical work done there. I cannot too warmly express my appreciation to Dr. Gordon E. Peterson who allowed me to work at the Communication Sciences Laboratory at the University of Michigan and who helped me beyond measure. Also I am extremely grateful to Dr. June E. Shoup, of the Communication Sciences Laboratory, who spent many hours both in helping me prepare the data and in very carefully proofreading the first six chapters.
    [Show full text]
  • By Chiara Frigeni a Thesis Submitted in Conformity with the Requirements
    SONORANT RELATIONSHIPS IN TWO VARIETIES OF SARDINIAN by Chiara Frigeni A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Linguistics University of Toronto Copyright c 2009 by Chiara Frigeni Abstract Sonorant relationships in two varieties of Sardinian Chiara Frigeni Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Linguistics University of Toronto 2009 Phonological interactions among sonorant sounds, and between sonorants and obstruents, are widespread in Romance languages. In this dissertation, I examine in detail such interactions in two dialects of Sardinian (Italo-Romance), Campidanese and Nuorese, showing that sonorant relationships differentiate the synchronic grammars of these dialects. The synchronic patterning of nasals and liquids, and how these two sonorant subclasses interact with obstruents, is significantly different between the two dialects. In particular, nasals trigger phonological nasalization of vowels and of the rhotic in Campidanese but not in Nuorese. The arguments for a phonological analysis of vowel nasalization in Campidanese are reviewed, expanded, and tested against an acoustic study. The historical traces of interac- tion between /n/ and /r/ in this dialect are linked to the synchronic rhotic nasalization process highlighted by an acoustic study of fieldwork data. In Nuorese, on the other hand, /n/ does not initiate phonological nasalization either of vowels or of the rhotic, and it is the target of total assimilation when followed by any segments but an oral stop. Nasals in the two dialects thus pattern in two very different ways phonologically: nasals are process triggers in Campidanese and process targets in Nuorese. The rhotic also shows distinct patterns in the two dialects, interacting with /n/ in Campidanese and with /s/ in Nuorese.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Experience and the Perception of a Voicing Contrast in Fricatives: Infant and Adult Data
    Language Experience and the Perception of a Voicing Contrast in Fricatives: Infant and Adult Data Laura Bosch and Núria Sebastián-Gallés Departament de Psicologia Básica, Universitat de Barcelona Passeig Vall d’Hebron, 171, 08035 Barcelona (Spain) e-mail: [email protected], [email protected] ABSTRACT More critical to the present research are data concerning the perception of voicing contrasts between fricative sounds. Literature on young infants’ discrimination abilities for Only the [s] – [z] contrast has been studied and again some fricative consonants is controversial. The voicing controversy is present. Syllabic position appeared as a distinction between /s/ and /z/, a phonemic contrast present crucial variable in the first report on this issue: [as]-[az] but in Catalan but not in Spanish, was selected to analyze early not [sa]-[za] were discriminated by 3-month-olds using the discrimination capacities and the effect of language HAS procedure [5]. In a second study, using the head- experience in Catalan monolingual, Spanish monolingual turning paradigm, 6-month-olds reached discrimination for and Catalan-Spanish bilingual populations. By 4 months of [sa]-[za] and it was suggested that longer experience with age discrimination could only be reached in syllable initial the language was required to develop sensitivity to this position and for monosyllabic stimuli. By 12 months of age, contrast [3]. This interpretation has been challenged and it initial sensitivities had been modified and only infants has been argued that failure to observe discrimination at an exposed to Catalan showed discrimination. Bilinguals’ early age might be attributed to the testing procedures and results are in conflict with a language-experience account methods of analysis used in that experiment [6].
    [Show full text]
  • On the Perceptual Accuracy of Non-Native Phonemic Contrasts: a Case Study of Native Arabic Speakers
    Available online at www.jlls.org JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC STUDIES ISSN: 1305-578X Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 16(4), 2003-2023; 2020 On the perceptual accuracy of non-native phonemic contrasts: A case study of Native Arabic Speakers Bader Alharbi a 1 , Mohammad Aljutaily b a Department of English Language &Translation, College of Arabic Language & Social Studies, Qassim University, Buraidah, Saudi Arabia b Department of English Language &Translation, College of Arabic Language & Social Studies, Qassim University, Buraidah, Saudi Arabia APA Citation: Alharbi, B., & Aljutaily, M. (2020). On the Perceptual Accuracy of Non-Native Phonemic Contrast: A Case Study of Native Arabic Speakers. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 16(4), 2003-2023. Doi: 10.17263/jlls.851031 Submission Date:10/07/2020 Acceptance Date:25/08/2020 Abstract Research on native Arabic speakers’ perception of non-native English phonemic contrasts has manifested various conclusions. Some studies have revealed that native Arabic speakers experience difficulty in the perception of non-native English phonemic contrasts, where others have concluded that they largely do not experience such difficulty. Due to this discrepancy, the current study aimed to examine the overall perceptual accuracy of only the English labial categories /p, b, f, and v/ in onset positions performed by native Arabic speakers. The study also examined the potential effects of the phonetic contexts and the familiarity of the stimuli being tested on the participants’ perceptual performance. Twenty-four Saudi male participants completed consonantal phoneme identification and discrimination tasks. The findings revealed that the participants experienced difficulty in the perception of the categories /p/ and /v/, confusing them with their counterparts /b/ and /f/.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetic Description of a Three-Way Stop Contrast in Northern Paiute
    UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2010) Phonetic description of a three-way stop contrast in Northern Paiute Reiko Kataoka Abstract This paper presents the phonetic description of a three-way phonemic contrast in the medial stops (lenis, fortis, and voiced fortis stops) of a southern dialect of Northern Paiute. Qualitative and quantitative analysis of VOT, closure duration, and voice quality was performed on field recordings of a female speaker from the 1950s. The findings include that: 1) voiced fortis stops are realized phonetically as voiceless unaspirated stops; 2) the difference between fortis and voiced fortis and between voiced fortis and lenis in terms of VOT is subtle; 3) consonantal duration is a robust acoustic characteristic differentiating the three classes of stops; 4) lenis stops are characterized by a smooth VC transition, while fortis stops often exhibit aspiration at the VC juncture, and voiced fortis stops exhibit occasional glottalization at the VC juncture. These findings suggest that the three-way contrast is realized by combination of multiple phonetic properties, particularly the properties that occur at the vowel-consonant boundary rather than the consonantal release. 1. Introduction Northern Paiute (NP) belongs to the Western Numic branch of the Uto-Aztecan language family and is divided into two main dialect groups: the northern group, Oregon Northern Paiute, and the southern group, Nevada Northern Paiute (Nichols 1974:4). Some of the southern dialects of Nevada Northern Paiute, known as Southern Nevada Northern Paiute (SNNP) (Nichols 1974), have a unique three-way contrast in the medial obstruent: ‗fortis‘, ‗lenis‘, and what has been called by Numic specialists the ‗voiced fortis‘ series.
    [Show full text]