Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of the Ohio State University

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of the Ohio State University FACTORS INVOLVED IN SUCCESS IN PROFESSIONAL CAREERS DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By EDWIN CLARK LEWIS, B.A., M.A. **##*# The Ohio State University 195? Approved bys ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to express his appreciation to Dr. Sidney L. Pressey for his guidance and suggestions, to Dr. Horace B. English and Dr. John Kinzer for their helpful comments, and to his wife for her unfailing support. ii TABLE OP CONTENTS INTRODUCTION Chapter I. HISTORY OF THE PROBLEM.................. Present interest in Intellectual resources Nature vs. nurture of genius Approaches to the problem Studies of prominent people Summary II. MATERIALS OP THE STUDY.................. The Ohio State Group Selection of the group and source of data ‘ Background information Present status Professional activities Appraisals and other information from The Ohio State faculty Other Materials Used Summary III. THE SEARCH FOR SIGNIFICANT FACTORS IN CAREER................................ Present Differences Present positions Present areas of interest Background Differences Size of birthplace Age of receiving degrees Areas in which doctorates were received First positions after doctorate Number of different positions after doctorate Background Factors Making Little Difference Sizes of undergraduate schools Number of different areas Number of children Summary TABLE OF CONTENTS (Cont'd. ) Chapter Page IV. THE SEARCH FOR SIGNIFICANT FACTORS IN CAREER: QUALITATIVE DIFFERENCES .................. 70 Faculty Ratings and Comments Factors Common to All Groups Personal influence Early interests Factors Previously Discussed Factors Not Found in All Groups Health Flexible education Achievement drive Prediction of Later Performance Summary V. DISCUSSION.................................. 83 Early start in career Flexible education Utilization of early interests Personal influence Opportunities in first position VI. SUMMARY...................... ............. 95 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................... 98 AUTOBIOGRAPHY .................................... 102 iv LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Size of Birthplace.......................... 28 2. Age of Receiving Bachelor's Degree ...... 30 3. Age of Receiving Doctorate.................. 32 4. Number of Years between Bachelor's Degree and Doctorate......... 33 5. Number Receiving Doctorates Each Year. .... 34 6 . Areas in Which Degrees Were Received........ 34 7. First Position after Receiving Doctorate . 35 8 . Number of Different Positions Held during C a r e e r .................................... 36 9. Age as of June 30, 1957...................... 37 10. Present Position................. 38 11. Areas of Present Major Interest. ....... 38 12. Convention Participation........... 42 13. Years in Which Doctorates Were Obtained by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.......................... 51 14. Age as of June 30, 1957, of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.............. 52 15. Present Positions of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.......... 53 16. Areas of Present Interest of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.............. 54 17. Size of Birthplace of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder. .......... 55 v LIST OF TABLES (Cont'd.) Table Page 18. Ages at Which Undergraduate Degrees Were Obtained by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.................... 56 19. Ages at Which Doctorates Were Obtained by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.............. ................. 5? 20. Number of Years between Undergraduate Degree and Doctorate for Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder. ......... 58 21. Areas in Which Doctorates Were Received by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.................................. 60 22. First Positions after Doctorate of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder .. 61 23. Number of Different Positions Held by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder .. 63 24. Number of Pages Published Two and Five Years after Doctorate by Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.................... 65 25. Sizes of Undergraduate Schools of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder......... 66 26. Number of Areas of Psychology of. Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder......... 67 27. Number of Children of Twenty Most and Least Prominent and Remainder.................... 6? vl INTRODUCTION The gifted or talented student Is still referred to by many writers as the most neglected person In education. However, a perusal of the literature on this subject Indi­ cates that there has been progress In recent years in the development of special educational programs for the gifted and In an Increased awareness of the need to provide special arrangements for them. Despite these advances, however, there has been relatively little study of the extent to which environ­ mental factors may play a part in the development of super­ ior talent or ability* Most of the work which has been done with the mentally superior seems to begin with the assumption that their abilities are so much a product of native constitution that the proper educational facilities release these potentialities but have little to do with their nature or amount. Becently there has been increased interest in the notion that a genius may be, to a considerable extent, the product of his environment, and that more attention should be paid to the possibilities of stimulating the realization of potential superior mental ability. Such approaches require, first of all, inquiry regarding environmental factors important in the development of superior persons in various fields. One method whioh has been used in attempts to isolate some of these important factors has been the study of men prominent in various fields to determine what factors seem to have been important in their success. In some studies, eminent men have been compared with those who are less-so, in an attempt to introduce some sort of control. The most recent example of the latter approach, and the most relevant to psychologists, is the study of America's Psychologists by Clark. One of the purposes of this Investigation was to determine factors which seemed to distinguish between those members of the American Psychological Association who were called *significant contributors" or "highly visible," and the psychologists-in-general (11). Although Clark's work was published after the writer's investigation was already underway, it was possible to revise the present study so as to take into account several deficiencies which seemed to be present in Clark's investigation and others of this type. One limitation of Clark's study was the narrowness of his criteria for selection of the highly visible people. An attempt was made to introduce broader criteria into the present study by having various members of the Ohio State department of psychology rate the individuals in the group studied as to present worth in the field, thus giving publication less weight in the total evaluation. Separate ratings were also made as to contributions to society or success in career, whether primarily in psychology or not. A second limitation was that the information used was primarily of an objective and often relatively super­ ficial nature. In what would seem to be a unique approach to this problem, this writer investigated those individuals who received the Ph.D. in psychology from Ohio State Univer slty prior to 1947. Not only could similar objective facts be obtained for the members of such a group, but it was also possible to obtain qualitative Information about these individuals from faculty members who had known them well as graduate students. The third disadvantage of the Clark study was that it was limited to psychologists of recent "vintage," with no attempt made to compare the results with information about other groups. In the present investigation, the Ohio State group was compared with two other groups of superior scientists: (1 ) the psychologists whose autobiographies are included in Volume TV of A HiBtory of Psychology in Autobiography (23), and the scientists whose early lives are described in Cox's Early Mental Traits of Three Hundred Geniuses (15). By comparing the information con­ cerning the early development and careers of these various groups, it was possible to Isolate factors which seem to have been influential in their success. CHAPTER I HISTORY OP THE PROBLEM Until recently, most of the attention paid to those with superior mental ability has concerned their childhood and youth. The leading work on this subject Is that of Terman, whose study of a large group of gifted children is one of the classics In the field (42), Among others who have published on this topic are Witty, who has edited a book on The Gifted Child (46) 5 DeHaan and Havighurst, who have written a new book on Educating Gifted Children (16); Worcester, who has been primarily interested in the early education of the gifted (49) j and Miles, who has written the chapter on the gifted In The Manual of Child Psychology (32). Present interest in intellectual resources.- Lately, however, the problem of the mentally superior has been made more insistent by its relation to the national security. More and more writers are stressing that this country is not using its ablest young people to the fullest advantage in the cold war with the Soviet Union, a country which is apparently using its brainpower more efficiently.
Recommended publications
  • 'Introspectionism' and the Mythical Origins of Scientific Psychology
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 634–654 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog ‘Introspectionism’ and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Alan Costall Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK Received 1 May 2006 Abstract According to the majority of the textbooks, the history of modern, scientific psychology can be tidily encapsulated in the following three stages. Scientific psychology began with a commitment to the study of mind, but based on the method of introspection. Watson rejected introspectionism as both unreliable and effete, and redefined psychology, instead, as the science of behaviour. The cognitive revolution, in turn, replaced the mind as the subject of study, and rejected both behaviourism and a reliance on introspection. This paper argues that all three stages of this history are largely mythical. Introspectionism was never a dominant movement within modern psychology, and the method of introspection never went away. Furthermore, this version of psychology’s history obscures some deep conceptual problems, not least surrounding the modern conception of ‘‘behaviour,’’ that continues to make the scientific study of consciousness seem so weird. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Introspection; Introspectionism; Behaviourism; Dualism; Watson; Wundt 1. Introduction Probably the most immediate result of the acceptance of the behaviorist’s view will be the elimination of self-observation and of the introspective reports resulting from such a method. (Watson, 1913b, p. 428). The problem of consciousness occupies an analogous position for cognitive psychology as the prob- lem of language behavior does for behaviorism, namely, an unsolved anomaly within the domain of an approach.
    [Show full text]
  • Women Psychologists and Gender Politics During World War II
    Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 42. No. I, 1986, pp. 157-180 “We would not take no for an answer”: Women Psychologists and Gender Politics During World War I1 James H. Capshew and Alejandra C. Laszlo University of Pennsylvania This essay explores the complex relationship among gender, professionalization, and ideology that developed as psychologists mobilized for World War It. Upon being excluded from mobilization plans by the male leaders of the profession, women psychologists organized the National Council of Women Psychologists to advance their interests. But while their male colleagues enjoyed new employment opportunities in the military services and government agencies, the women were confined largely to volunteer activities in their local communities. Although women psychologists succeeded in gaining representation on wartime commit- tees and in drawing attention to their professional problems, they were unable to change the status quo in psychology. Situated in a cultural milieu ihat stressed the masculine nature of science, women psychologists were hampered by their own acceptance of a professional ideology of meritocratic reward, and remained ambivalent about their feminist activities. Throughout the 20th century women have increasingly challenged the as- sumption that they are neither interested in nor qualified for scientific careers. It is also true that as women successfully breached this overwhelmingly male bastion, a growing body of biological, social, and cultural arguments were advanced to rationalize their usually restricted scientific role. This was especially evident during the Second World War, as social and economic forces strained existing professional relationships. Permission to quote from unpublished materials has been granted by Alice I. Bryan, Carnegie- Mellon University Libraries, Harvard University Archives, Stanford University Archives, and Yale University Library.
    [Show full text]
  • Looksmart's Findarticles - Journal of Social Issues: Gordon Willard All
    LookSmart's FindArticles - Journal of Social Issues: Gordon Willard All... http://www.findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_m0341/is_3_55/ai_58549253/... FindArticles > Journal of Social Issues > Fall, 1999 > Article > Print friendly Gordon Willard Allport: A Tribute Thomas F. Pettigrew Thomas F. Pettigrew [*] In tribute to Gordon Allport, this article discusses four interrelated topics. First, Allport's life at Harvard is briefly described. Next three interwoven features of his work are advanced to explain why his contributions to psychology are both unique and lasting. (1) His work offered a broadly eclectic balance of the many sides of psychology. (2) He repeatedly demonstrated the ability to formulate the discipline's central problems for the future and to propose innovative approaches to them. And (3) Allport's entire scholarly work presents a consistent, seamless and forceful perspective. These three points are then applied to his classic work The Nature of Prejudice. Though it was his most explicit attempt to influence public opinion, this famous volume also is balanced, ahead of its time, and elegantly written. It has organized the study of prejudice over the last half century. The article concludes with an explanation in personal terms for why those who knew him remember him so warmly 30 long years after h is death. With this issue, we honor the centennial of a great psychologist and a fine human being. As a doctoral student and colleague of Gordon Allport, I am pleased to provide my personal reflections on his life and work. This article discusses four interrelated topics. First, Allport's life at Harvard is sketched.
    [Show full text]
  • Book Reviews
    Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 42(3), 279–280 Summer 2006 Published online in Wiley Interscience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002 /jhbs.20173 © 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. BOOK REVIEWS D. Brett King and Michael Wertheimer. Max Wertheimer and Gestalt Theory. New Brunswick, NJ, and London: Transaction Publishers, 2005. 438 pp. $49.95. ISBN: 0- 7658-0258-9. To review this long-awaited volume is a delicate task. Full disclosure up front: I have known and liked Michael Wertheimer for more than 30 years, since I first began my own work on the history of Gestalt theory. I was privileged to work with the Max Wertheimer papers in the mid-1970s, when they were still stored in Michael Wertheimer’s home in Boulder, Colorado. At that time, Michael Wertheimer already planned to write a biography of his father. The finished book, written with the help of D. Brett King, can only be called a labor of love. This is a fascinating, comprehensive, generally well-written, and, above all, warm- hearted volume. The authors make every effort to give both the person and his creation their due. As might be expected, Wertheimer’s family life gets detailed attention. The authors make judicious use of an extended interview with Max Wertheimer’s former wife and Michael Wertheimer’s mother, Anni Wertheimer Hornbostel, and other family documents, including a newspaper “published” by the Wertheimer children. And Michael Wertheimer contributes his own memories of his father, helping to make him come alive on the page. Max Wertheimer’s
    [Show full text]
  • Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(S): J
    Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(s): J. G. Morawski Source: Isis, Vol. 77, No. 2 (Jun., 1986), pp. 219-242 Published by: University of Chicago Press on behalf of History of Science Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/232650 Accessed: 22-12-2015 00:42 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. History of Science Society and University of Chicago Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Isis. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 129.133.6.95 on Tue, 22 Dec 2015 00:42:52 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations By J. G. Morawski* IN 1929 JAMES ANGELL, president of Yale, announced plans for a unique teaching and research center for those fields "directly concerned with the problems of man's individual and group conduct. The purpose is to correlate knowledge and coordinate technique in related fields that greater progress may be made in the understanding of human life. The time has certainly come once again to attempt a fruitful synthesis of knowledge." The New York Times described the experiment as dismantling the disciplinary "Great Wall of China" and compared it with the Renaissance transformation of knowledge.1 The Insti- tute of Human Relations (IHR), as the center was named, received over $4.5 million from the Rockefeller Foundation for its first decade of operation.
    [Show full text]
  • Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology
    Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology Edited by Claude E. Buxton Department of Psychology Yale University New Haven, Connecticut 1985 ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers) Orlando San Diego New York London Toronto Montreal Sydney Tokyo Passages from the following are reprinted by permission of the publishers: Newell, Α., Duncker on Thinking, in S. Koch & D. Leary (Eds.), A Century of Psychology as Science. Copyright 1985 by McGraw-Hill. Neisser, U., Cognitive Psychology. © 1967 by Prentice-Hall. COPYRIGHT © 1985 BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Orlando, Florida 32887 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24-28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING IN PUBLICATION DATA Main entry under title: Points of view in the modern history of psychology. Includes indexes. 1. Psychology— History. I. Buxton, Claude E. BF81.P57 1985 150\9 85-4010 ISBN 0-12-148510-2 (alk. paper) PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 85 86 87 88 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Mitchell G. Ash (295), Department of History, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242 William Bevan (259), John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, Chicago, Illinois 60603 Arthur L. Blumenthal (19, 51), Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts at Boston, Boston, Massachusetts 02125 Claude E.
    [Show full text]
  • HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY of PSYCHOLOGY
    HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Irving B. Weiner John Wiley & Sons, Inc. HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY VOLUME 1 HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Volume Editor Irving B. Weiner Editor-in-Chief John Wiley & Sons, Inc. This book is printed on acid-free paper. ➇ Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. All rights reserved. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 750-4470, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008, e-mail: [email protected]. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials.
    [Show full text]
  • Jung and the Making of Modern Psychology: the Dream of a Science
    Jung and the Making of Modern Psychology Occultist, Scientist, Prophet, Charlatan – C. G. Jung has been called all these things and after decades of myth making is one of the most misunderstood figures in Western intellectual history. This book is the first comprehensive study of the formation of his psychology, as well as providing a new account of the rise of modern psychology and psy- chotherapy. Based on a wealth of hitherto unknown archival materials it reconstructs the reception of Jung’s work in the human sciences, and its impact on the social and intellectual history of the twentieth century. This book creates a basis for all future discussion of Jung, and opens new vistas on psychology today. is a historian of psychology and a Research As- sociate of the Wellcome Trust Centre for the History of Medicine at University College London. His most recent book Cult Fictions: C. G. Jung and the Founding of Analytical Psychology won the Gradiva Prize for the best historical and biographical work from the World Association for the Advancement of Psychoanalysis. Jung and the Making of Modern Psychology The Dream of a Science Sonu Shamdasani Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo Cambridge University Press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge , United Kingdom Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York www.cambridge.org Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521831451 © Sonu Shamdasani 2003 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provision of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press.
    [Show full text]
  • Moral Economies and Codes of Conduct: the Social Organization of Canadian Experimental Psychology Jordan Richard Schoenherr
    Document generated on 10/01/2021 6:11 p.m. Scientia Canadensis Canadian Journal of the History of Science, Technology and Medicine Revue canadienne d'histoire des sciences, des techniques et de la médecine Moral Economies and Codes of Conduct: the Social Organization of Canadian Experimental Psychology Jordan Richard Schoenherr Volume 41, Number 1, 2019 Article abstract Psychological science has flourished in North America since the late 19th URI: https://id.erudit.org/iderudit/1065958ar century. As laboratories multiplied, professional organizations began to DOI: https://doi.org/10.7202/1065958ar emerge to facilitate communications through journals and conferences. This article examines the development of the moral economy of professional and See table of contents scientific psychology in Canada. While the general features of the social organization of psychological science in Canada and the United States have followed similar trajectories, important differences are also evident. In Publisher(s) particular, these differences are apparent in the values and conventions outlined by the two largest professional organizations in Canada (CPA) and the CSTHA/AHSTC United States (APA). ISSN 1918-7750 (digital) Explore this journal Cite this article Schoenherr, J. R. (2019). Moral Economies and Codes of Conduct: the Social Organization of Canadian Experimental Psychology. Scientia Canadensis, 41(1), 31–54. https://doi.org/10.7202/1065958ar All Rights Reserved © Jordan Richard Schoenherr, 2019 This document is protected by copyright law. Use of the services of Érudit (including reproduction) is subject to its terms and conditions, which can be viewed online. https://apropos.erudit.org/en/users/policy-on-use/ This article is disseminated and preserved by Érudit.
    [Show full text]
  • THE NEW BEHAVIORISM Mind, Mechanism and Society
    THE NEW BEHAVIORISM Mind, Mechanism and Society John Staddon CONTENTS Part 1: History Chapter 1: Early Behaviorism Chapter 2: Behaviorism and Learning Psychology Part 2: Radical Behaviorism Chapter 3: Radical Behaviorism, I: Method Chapter 4: Radical Behaviorism, II: Explanation Chapter 5: Skinner and Theory Chapter 6: Variation and Selection Chapter 7: Behavior-Evolution Parallels Chapter 8: Rationality Chapter 9: Truth, Science and Behaviorism Chapter 10: Free Will and Utopia Chapter 11: Values Chapter 12: Skinner and Mental Life Part 3: The New Behaviorism Chapter 13: Cognitivism and The New Behaviorism i Chapter 14: Internal States: The Logic of Historical Systems Chapter 15: Consciousness and Theoretical Behaviorism Postscript to Parts 1-3: Alchemy of the Mind Part 4: Behaviorism and Society Chapter 16: Law, Punishment and Behaviorism Chapter 17: The Health-Care Schedule Chapter 18: Reinforcement and ‘Socialized’ Medicine Chapter 19: Teaching ii Preface to the Second Edition This edition is almost completely rewritten. It is about 45% longer than the first. I cover two new social issues and also devote more space to the philosophy of cognitivism and the science behind theoretical behaviorism. B. F. Skinner figures less prominently in this edition than the last, but his work is a theme that still runs through many chapters – because his influence has been so great and his writings raise so many provocative issues that are identified with behavior- ism. But, as readers of the first edition detected, my position is far from Skinnerian. Many reviewers thought well of the first edition but, naturally, I want to attend to those who did not.
    [Show full text]
  • Special Issue Foundations for Experimental Social Psychology: Requiem for Classic Social Psychology
    Special Issue Foundations for Experimental Social Psychology: Requiem for Classic Social Psychology Robert D. Mather Savannah Brand Special Issue Editors University of Central Oklahoma The field of social psychology is relatively young, and has been touched by many different areas, including experimental psychology, sociology, cognitive psychology, and neuroscience. During the course of the field’s development, many perspectives have contributed to the work of individual researchers in exciting ways. However, most introductory social psychology textbooks give very little coverage to the stories behind the classic social psychologists. This is not an indictment of introductory social psychology textbooks. They serve their purpose and do so very well—to survey the research and theories in the field and to place it in a broader context of application. This issue is dedicated to providing a readable digest of information that may be of interest to our readers. Kurt Lewin earned the title of “Father of Experimental Social Psychology” by attempting to understand the connection between one’s self and their environment, along with looking at how a group of individuals think when together. Leon Festinger contributed important research with his theory of cognitive dissonance, which allows psychologists to understand how individuals can soothe themselves when they are dealing with an uncomfortable emotion and how individuals deal with difficult decision-making. Floyd Allport helped to move the psychology field away from mere speculation by insisting upon the use of objective methodology and experimentation, allowing the field to become more science based. And his brother, Gordon Allport, focused on making personality psychology its own sub-field, and had an influential impact on psychology, as a whole.
    [Show full text]
  • Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done? Caitlin Martin-Wagar [email protected]
    The University of Akron IdeaExchange@UAkron Psychology from the Margins April 2018 Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done? Caitlin Martin-Wagar [email protected] Please take a moment to share how this work helps you through this survey. Your feedback will be important as we plan further development of our repository. Follow this and additional works at: http://ideaexchange.uakron.edu/psychologyfromthemargins Part of the History Commons, Multicultural Psychology Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Martin-Wagar, Caitlin (2018) "Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done?," Psychology from the Margins: Vol. 1 : Iss. 1 , Article 3. Available at: http://ideaexchange.uakron.edu/psychologyfromthemargins/vol1/iss1/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by IdeaExchange@UAkron, the institutional repository of The nivU ersity of Akron in Akron, Ohio, USA. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology from the Margins by an authorized administrator of IdeaExchange@UAkron. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Martin-Wagar: Inequity for Women in Psychology All along the question of marriage interferes with the woman's assured planning. Can a woman become a fanatic in her profession and still remain marriageable? Yes, she can, for I know some, but I think a woman must be abnormally bright to combine charm with concentration. These women make the synthesis by being charmingly enthusiastic. The Woman Problem comes up again after the professional woman has acquired a husband and a couple of children, with the culture pressing to give her a heavy responsibility in the home, with her husband noting, perhaps, that his own success demands his own job-concentration.
    [Show full text]