Cell Phones Silent Clickers on Physics 1240 Lecture 14

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cell Phones Silent Clickers on Physics 1240 Lecture 14 This is PHYS 1240 - Sound and Music Lecture 12 Professor Patricia Rankin TA: Tyler McMaken Cell Phones silent Clickers on Physics 1240 Lecture 14 Today: Scales, Tutorial Next time: Review for midterm physicscourses.colorado.edu/phys1240 Canvas Site: assignments, administration, grades Homework – HW7 Not due till Wed March 11th 5pm Homelabs – Hlab4 Not due till March 16th HW 6 review open-open pipe closed-open pipe 3. (avg score: 30%) What would happen to the frequency of the 푛 = 1: second mode (the next member of the harmonic series after the fundamental) of an open-open pipe 푛 = 2: if a cap was placed on one end? 푛 = 3: A) It would increase by a factor of 2 B) It would decrease by a factor of 2 푛 = 4: C) It would decrease by a factor of 3 D) It would stay the same E) It would change by some other 푛 = 5: factor 푣푠 푣 푓 = 푛 ∙ 푓 = 푛 ∙ 푠 푛 2퐿 푛 4퐿 Review • Consonance: tones have whole number frequency ratios • Dissonance: harsh sound when 2 tones (or upper harmonics) produce beats within the same critical band • Harmonic series → Pythagorean intervals perfect perfect major minor major minor octave fifth fourth third third second second (2/1) (3/2) (4/3) (5/4) (6/5) (9/8) (16/15) ………… Questions: 1) Why does a piano have 12 notes in each octave? 2) How do we tune those 12 notes (how do we decide what frequencies to assign to each note)? Pythagoras of Samos • 500s BCE • Founded school of numerology • Music of the spheres • Pythagorean Hypothesis: Consonant musical intervals are related to low integer ratios of frequencies Clicker 14.1 Two monochords are plucked to produce sound. One string is 50 cm long, and the other is 40 cm long. What is the musical interval between these plucked notes? A) octave B) tritone C) perfect fourth D) major third E) minor third Clicker 14.1 D Two monochords are plucked to produce sound. One string is 50 cm long, and the other is 40 cm long. What is the musical interval between these plucked notes? A) octave B) tritone C) perfect fourth D) major third E) minor third Clicker 14.2 A note is played at 100 Hz. Then, the pitch moves up by a perfect fifth, then it moves up by a perfect fourth. What is the new frequency? A) 100 Hz B) 133 Hz C) 150 Hz D) 180 Hz E) 200 Hz Clicker 14.2 E A note is played at 100 Hz. Then, the pitch moves up by a perfect fifth, then it moves up by a perfect fourth. What is the new frequency? A) 100 Hz B) 133 Hz C) 150 Hz D) 180 Hz E) 200 Hz 3 4 (100 Hz) × × = 200 Hz 2 3 The Piano Keyboard C D E F G A B The Piano Keyboard C# / D♭ F# / G♭ D# / E♭ G# / A♭ A# / B♭ Half step or semitone Half step Half step Whole step or whole tone whole step = two half steps Whole step Intervals on the Piano Keyboard octave # of half fourth Interval Frequency fifth ratio steps Octave 2/1 12 Perfect fifth 3/2 7 Perfect fourth 4/3 5 Major third 5/4 4 whole step half step “whole tone” “semitone” Minor third 6/5 3 Scale #1: Just Tuning • Based on lowest integer frequency ratios 1 9 5 4 3 2 Ratio to C: ? ? 1 8 4 3 2 1 Scale #1: Just Tuning • For A, go up a perfect fourth then up a major third: 4 5 5 × = 3 4 3 • For B, go up a perfect fifth then up a major third: 3 5 15 × = 2 4 8 or, from A, go up a major second: 5 9 15 × = 3 8 8 1 9 5 4 3 5 15 2 Ratio to C: ? ? 1 8 4 3 2 3 8 1 Scale #1: Just Tuning • Benefits: sounds pure • Drawbacks: only works in one key (not all fifths are perfect 3/2 ratios) Circle of fifths: 1 9 5 4 3 5 15 2 Ratio to C: 1 8 4 3 2 3 8 1 Scale #2: Pythagorean Tuning • Goal: make all the perfect fifths within the scale pure (3/2) • Problem: Pythagorean comma ⇒ Impossible to tune a piano perfectly with this system 1 9 81 4 3 27 243 2.03 Ratio to C: ? 1 8 64 3 2 16 128 1 Scale #3: Equal Temperament • Solution: temper the fifths (split the leftover frequency among other intervals to make them each slightly out of tune) • Equal temperament: • All 12 half step intervals are the same frequency ratio 1 12 • Each half step is a factor of 2 = 212 ≈ 1.05945 • Any song can be played in any key without going out of tune (since everything is already “equally out of tune”) Scale #3: Equal Temperament 1 12 • Each half step is a factor of 2 = 212 ≈ 1.05945 Clicker 14.3 # of half In an equal-tempered 12-note scale, what is the Interval Frequency frequency ratio corresponding to a major third? ratio steps A) 5/4 Octave 2/1 12 B) 81/64 1 Perfect 3 3/2 7 C) (212) ≈ 1.189 fifth 1 4 D) (212) ≈ 1.260 Perfect 4/3 5 E) 12/4 fourth Major 5/4 4 third Minor 6/5 3 third Clicker 14.3 D # of half In an equal-tempered 12-note scale, what is the Interval Frequency frequency ratio corresponding to a major third? ratio steps A) 5/4 Octave 2/1 12 B) 81/64 1 Perfect 3 3/2 7 C) (212) ≈ 1.189 fifth ퟏ ퟒ D) (ퟐퟏퟐ) ≈ ퟏ. ퟐퟔퟎ Perfect 4/3 5 E) 12/4 fourth Major 5/4 4 third Minor 6/5 3 third Scale #3: Equal Temperament 1 12 • Each half step is a factor of 2 = 212 ≈ 1.05945 • Now a tune can sound alright when played in any key • Equal temperament didn’t take hold until around the time of Mozart – why not sooner? • Hard to tune this way with just a tuning fork • None of the intervals are purely consonant; they’re just “good enough” • Just Tuning: uses only pure, harmonic intervals • Pros: all pure consonances for intervals from same note • Cons: can only play in one key • Pythagorean Tuning: makes all fifths in any key pure (3/2) • Pros: all pure consonances for fifths • Cons: thirds are dissonant; Pythagorean comma • Equal Temperament: same interval for all adjacent notes • Pros: can play in any key • Cons: all intervals are very slightly dissonant Note C D E F G A B C name: 1 9 5 4 3 5 15 2 Just 1 8 4 3 2 3 8 1 1 9 81 4 3 27 243 2.03? Pythagorean Frequency 1 8 64 3 2 16 128 1 ratio to C: Equal- 1 1 2 1 4 1 5 1 7 1 9 1 11 2 Tempered 1 212 212 212 212 212 212 1 Tutorial What is the frequency ratio of a Pythagorean comma? A) 1.01364 B) 1.1524 C) 1.5 D) 1.11111 E) 1.
Recommended publications
  • Third Report to the Thirteenth Session of the General Assembly
    UNITED COpy ADVISORY COMMITTEE ON ADMINISTRATIVE AND BUDGETARY QUESTIONS ·THIRD REPORT TO THE THIRTEENTH SESSION OF THE GENERAL ASSEMBLY GENERAL ASSEMBLY OFFICIAL RECORDS : THIRTEPNfH· SESSION i SUPPLEMENT No. f {A/ 3860) \ / -,-------~/ NEW YORK, 1958 .I , (49 p.) UNITED NATIONS ADVISORY COMMITTEE ON ADMINISTRATIVE AND BUDGETARY QUESTIONS THffiD REPORT TO THE THIRTEENTH SESSION OF THE GENERAL ASSEMBLY GENERAL ASS'EMBLY OFFICIAL RECORDS : THIRTEENTH SESSION SUPPLEMENT No. 7 (Aj3860) Ner..o York, 1958 NOr.fE Symbols of United Nations documents are composed of capital letters combined with figures. Mention of such a symbol indicates a reference to a United Nations document. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page FOREWORD . v REPORT TO THE GENERAL ASSEMBLY ON THE BUDGET ESTIMATES FOR 1959 Chapter Paragraphs Page 1. ApPRAISAL OF THE BUDGET ESTIMATES FOR 1959 OF PERSONNEL Estimates for 1959 . 1-7 1 Comparison with 1958 appropriations . 8-10 2 Major items of increase in the 1959 initial estimates . 11-14 3 Assessments for 1958 and 1959 . 15-17 3 Form of presentation of the 1959 estimates . 18-28 4 Procedures for the Advisory Committee's budget examination . 29-32 5 Conferences and meetings . 33-40 6 Work and organization of the Secretariat . 41-45 7 Economic Commission for Africa . 46 7 Established posts. .............................................. 47-52 7 General expenses . 53-54 9 Public information expenses . 55 9 Administrative costs of technical assistance . 56-63 9 Appropriation resolution . 64-66 10 Working Capital Fund . 67-68 10 Comparative table of appropriations as proposed by the Secretary- General and recommended by the Advisory Committee . 11 Appendix I. Draft appropriation resolution for the financial year 1959 12 Appendix II.
    [Show full text]
  • The 17-Tone Puzzle — and the Neo-Medieval Key That Unlocks It
    The 17-tone Puzzle — And the Neo-medieval Key That Unlocks It by George Secor A Grave Misunderstanding The 17 division of the octave has to be one of the most misunderstood alternative tuning systems available to the microtonal experimenter. In comparison with divisions such as 19, 22, and 31, it has two major advantages: not only are its fifths better in tune, but it is also more manageable, considering its very reasonable number of tones per octave. A third advantage becomes apparent immediately upon hearing diatonic melodies played in it, one note at a time: 17 is wonderful for melody, outshining both the twelve-tone equal temperament (12-ET) and the Pythagorean tuning in this respect. The most serious problem becomes apparent when we discover that diatonic harmony in this system sounds highly dissonant, considerably more so than is the case with either 12-ET or the Pythagorean tuning, on which we were hoping to improve. Without any further thought, most experimenters thus consign the 17-tone system to the discard pile, confident in the knowledge that there are, after all, much better alternatives available. My own thinking about 17 started in exactly this way. In 1976, having been a microtonal experimenter for thirteen years, I went on record, dismissing 17-ET in only a couple of sentences: The 17-tone equal temperament is of questionable harmonic utility. If you try it, I doubt you’ll stay with it for long.1 Since that time I have become aware of some things which have caused me to change my opinion completely.
    [Show full text]
  • The Lost Harmonic Law of the Bible
    The Lost Harmonic Law of the Bible Jay Kappraff New Jersey Institute of Technology Newark, NJ 07102 Email: [email protected] Abstract The ethnomusicologist Ernest McClain has shown that metaphors based on the musical scale appear throughout the great sacred and philosophical works of the ancient world. This paper will present an introduction to McClain’s harmonic system and how it sheds light on the Old Testament. 1. Introduction Forty years ago the ethnomusicologist Ernest McClain began to study musical metaphors that appeared in the great sacred and philosophical works of the ancient world. These included the Rg Veda, the dialogues of Plato, and most recently, the Old and New Testaments. I have described his harmonic system and referred to many of his papers and books in my book, Beyond Measure (World Scientific; 2001). Apart from its value in providing new meaning to ancient texts, McClain’s harmonic analysis provides valuable insight into musical theory and mathematics both ancient and modern. 2. Musical Fundamentals Figure 1. Tone circle as a Single-wheeled Chariot of the Sun (Rg Veda) Figure 2. The piano has 88 keys spanning seven octaves and twelve musical fifths. The chromatic musical scale has twelve tones, or semitone intervals, which may be pictured on the face of a clock or along the zodiac referred to in the Rg Veda as the “Single-wheeled Chariot of the Sun.” shown in Fig. 1, with the fundamental tone placed atop the tone circle and associated in ancient sacred texts with “Deity.” The tones are denoted by the first seven letters of the alphabet augmented and diminished by and sharps ( ) and flats (b).
    [Show full text]
  • Standard for Data Processing of Measured Data (Draft) WP 3 Deliverable/Task: 3.3
    Joint Monitoring Programme for Ambient Noise North Sea 2018 – 2020 Standard for Data Processing of Measured Data (Draft) WP 3 Deliverable/Task: 3.3 Authors: L. Wang, J Ward, S Robinson Affiliations: NPL Date: February 2019 INTERREG North Sea Region JOMOPANS Project Full Title Joint Monitoring Programme for Ambient Noise North Sea Project Acronym Jomopans Programme Interreg North Region Programme Programme Priority Priority 3 Sustainable North Sea Region Colophon Name Niels Kinneging (Project Manager) Organization Name Rijkswaterstaat Email [email protected] Phone +31 6 5321 5242 This report should be cited: XXXXXXXXXXX Cover picture: NPL 2 INTERREG North Sea Region JOMOPANS Table of contents 1 Data quality assurance and pre-processing .......................................................................... 5 1.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................................... 5 1.2 Check for missing data and data consistency ....................................................................... 5 1.3 Removal of contaminated data ............................................................................................. 5 1.4 Checks for clipping and distortion ......................................................................................... 5 1.5 Checking for spurious signals ............................................................................................... 6 1.6 Software control ...................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Hexatonic Cycles
    CHAPTER Two H e x a t o n i c C y c l e s Chapter 1 proposed that triads could be related by voice leading, independently of roots, diatonic collections, and other central premises of classical theory. Th is chapter pursues that proposal, considering two triads to be closely related if they share two common tones and their remaining tones are separated by semitone. Motion between them thus involves a single unit of work. Positioning each triad beside its closest relations produces a preliminary map of the triadic universe. Th e map serves some analytical purposes, which are explored in this chapter. Because it is not fully connected, it will be supplemented with other relations developed in chapters 4 and 5. Th e simplicity of the model is a pedagogical advantage, as it presents a circum- scribed environment in which to develop some central concepts, terms, and modes of representation that are used throughout the book. Th e model highlights the central role of what is traditionally called the chromatic major-third relation, although that relation is theorized here without reference to harmonic roots. It draws attention to the contrary-motion property that is inherent in and exclusive to triadic pairs in that relation. Th at property, I argue, underlies the association of chromatic major-third relations with supernatural phenomena and altered states of consciousness in the early nineteenth century. Finally, the model is suffi cient to provide preliminary support for the central theoretical claim of this study: that the capacity for minimal voice leading between chords of a single type is a special property of consonant triads, resulting from their status as minimal perturbations of perfectly even augmented triads.
    [Show full text]
  • USCA11 Case: 19-14434 Date Filed: 04/21/2021 Page: 1 of 23
    USCA11 Case: 19-14434 Date Filed: 04/21/2021 Page: 1 of 23 [PUBLISH] IN THE UNITED STATES COURT OF APPEALS FOR THE ELEVENTH CIRCUIT ________________________ No. 19-14434 ________________________ D.C. Docket No. 8:19-cv-00983-TPB-TGW RICHARD HUNSTEIN, Plaintiff - Appellant, versus PREFERRED COLLECTION AND MANAGEMENT SERVICES, INC., Defendant - Appellee. ________________________ Appeal from the United States District Court for the Middle District of Florida ________________________ (April 21, 2021) Before JORDAN, NEWSOM, and TJOFLAT, Circuit Judges. NEWSOM, Circuit Judge: This appeal presents an interesting question of first impression under the Fair Debt Collection Practices Act—and, like so many other cases arising under USCA11 Case: 19-14434 Date Filed: 04/21/2021 Page: 2 of 23 federal statutes these days, requires us first to consider whether our plaintiff has Article III standing. The short story: A debt collector electronically transmitted data concerning a consumer’s debt—including his name, his outstanding balance, the fact that his debt resulted from his son’s medical treatment, and his son’s name—to a third- party vendor. The third-party vendor then used the data to create, print, and mail a “dunning” letter to the consumer. The consumer filed suit alleging that, in sending his personal information to the vendor, the debt collector had violated 15 U.S.C. § 1692c(b), which, with certain exceptions, prohibits debt collectors from communicating consumers’ personal information to third parties “in connection with the collection of
    [Show full text]
  • Interval Cycles and the Emergence of Major-Minor Tonality
    Empirical Musicology Review Vol. 5, No. 3, 2010 Modes on the Move: Interval Cycles and the Emergence of Major-Minor Tonality MATTHEW WOOLHOUSE Centre for Music and Science, Faculty of Music, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom ABSTRACT: The issue of the emergence of major-minor tonality is addressed by recourse to a novel pitch grouping process, referred to as interval cycle proximity (ICP). An interval cycle is the minimum number of (additive) iterations of an interval that are required for octave-related pitches to be re-stated, a property conjectured to be responsible for tonal attraction. It is hypothesised that the actuation of ICP in cognition, possibly in the latter part of the sixteenth century, led to a hierarchy of tonal attraction which favoured certain pitches over others, ostensibly the tonics of the modern major and minor system. An ICP model is described that calculates the level of tonal attraction between adjacent musical elements. The predictions of the model are shown to be consistent with music-theoretic accounts of common practice period tonality, including Piston’s Table of Usual Root Progressions. The development of tonality is illustrated with the historical quotations of commentators from the sixteenth to the eighteenth centuries, and can be characterised as follows. At the beginning of the seventeenth century multiple ‘finals’ were possible, each associated with a different interval configuration (mode). By the end of the seventeenth century, however, only two interval configurations were in regular use: those pertaining to the modern major- minor key system. The implications of this development are discussed with respect interval cycles and their hypothesised effect within music.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Techniques
    Musical Techniques Musical Techniques Frequencies and Harmony Dominique Paret Serge Sibony First published 2017 in Great Britain and the United States by ISTE Ltd and John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research or private study, or criticism or review, as permitted under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, this publication may only be reproduced, stored or transmitted, in any form or by any means, with the prior permission in writing of the publishers, or in the case of reprographic reproduction in accordance with the terms and licenses issued by the CLA. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside these terms should be sent to the publishers at the undermentioned address: ISTE Ltd John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 27-37 St George’s Road 111 River Street London SW19 4EU Hoboken, NJ 07030 UK USA www.iste.co.uk www.wiley.com © ISTE Ltd 2017 The rights of Dominique Paret and Serge Sibony to be identified as the authors of this work have been asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. Library of Congress Control Number: 2016960997 British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A CIP record for this book is available from the British Library ISBN 978-1-78630-058-4 Contents Preface ........................................... xiii Introduction ........................................ xv Part 1. Laying the Foundations ............................ 1 Introduction to Part 1 .................................. 3 Chapter 1. Sounds, Creation and Generation of Notes ................................... 5 1.1. Physical and physiological notions of a sound .................. 5 1.1.1. Auditory apparatus ............................... 5 1.1.2. Physical concepts of a sound .......................... 7 1.1.3.
    [Show full text]
  • The Augmented Sixth Chord
    CHAPTER24 The Augmented Sixth Chord Characteristics, Derivation, and Behavior The two excerpts in Example 24.1 are from different style periods, yet they share several features. In terms of form and harmony, both divide into two subphrases and close with strong half cadences. Further, the pre-dominant harmony in both examples is the same: an altered iv6 chord. Indeed, we hear not a Phrygian cadence (iv6-V), but rather some chromatic version, where the diatonic major sixth above the bass is raised a half step to create the strongly directed interval of the augmented sixth (+6). The new half-step ascent (#4-5) mirrors the bass's half-step descent (6-5). We refer to such chromatic pre-dominants as augmented sixth chords because of the characteristic interval between the bass 6 and the upper-voice #4. Listen to both excerpts in Example 24.1, noting the striking sound of the augmented sixth chords. EXAMPLE 24.1 A. Schubert, WaltzinG minor, Die letzte Walzer, op. 127, no. 12, D. 146 472 CHAPTER 24 THE AUGMENTED SIXTH CHORD 473 B. Handel, "Since by Man Came Death," Messiah, HWV 56 Example 24.2 demonstrates the derivation of the augmented sixth chord from the Phrygian cadence. Example 24.2A represents a traditional Phrygian half cadence. In Example 24.2B, the chromatic F# fills the space between F and G, and the passing motion creates an interval of an augmented sixth. Finally, Example 24.2C shows the augmented sixth chord as a harmonic entity, with no consonant preparation. EXAMPLE 24.2 Phrygian Cadence Generates the Augmented Sixth Chord Given that the augmented sixth chord also occurs in major, one might ask if it is an example of an applied chord or a mixture chord? To answer this question, consider the diatonic progression in Example 24.3A.
    [Show full text]
  • The Consecutive-Semitone Constraint on Scalar Structure: a Link Between Impressionism and Jazz1
    The Consecutive-Semitone Constraint on Scalar Structure: A Link Between Impressionism and Jazz1 Dmitri Tymoczko The diatonic scale, considered as a subset of the twelve chromatic pitch classes, possesses some remarkable mathematical properties. It is, for example, a "deep scale," containing each of the six diatonic intervals a unique number of times; it represents a "maximally even" division of the octave into seven nearly-equal parts; it is capable of participating in a "maximally smooth" cycle of transpositions that differ only by the shift of a single pitch by a single semitone; and it has "Myhill's property," in the sense that every distinct two-note diatonic interval (e.g., a third) comes in exactly two distinct chromatic varieties (e.g., major and minor). Many theorists have used these properties to describe and even explain the role of the diatonic scale in traditional tonal music.2 Tonal music, however, is not exclusively diatonic, and the two nondiatonic minor scales possess none of the properties mentioned above. Thus, to the extent that we emphasize the mathematical uniqueness of the diatonic scale, we must downplay the musical significance of the other scales, for example by treating the melodic and harmonic minor scales merely as modifications of the natural minor. The difficulty is compounded when we consider the music of the late-nineteenth and twentieth centuries, in which composers expanded their musical vocabularies to include new scales (for instance, the whole-tone and the octatonic) which again shared few of the diatonic scale's interesting characteristics. This suggests that many of the features *I would like to thank David Lewin, John Thow, and Robert Wason for their assistance in preparing this article.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 – Music Temperament and Pitch
    11 – Music temperament and pitch Music sounds Every music instrument, including the voice, uses a more or less well defined sequence of music sounds, the notes, to produce music. The notes have particular frequencies that are called „pitch“ by musicians. The frequencies or pitches of the notes stand in a particular relation to each other. There are and were different ways to build a system of music sounds in different geographical regions during different historical periods. We will consider only the currently dominating system (used in classical and pop music) that was originated in ancient Greece and further developed in Europe. Pitch vs interval Only a small part of the people with normal hearing are able to percieve the absolute frequency of a sound. These people, who are said to have the „absolute pitch“, can tell what key has been hit on a piano without looking at it. The human ear and brain is much more sensitive to the relations between the frequencies of two or more sounds, the music intervals. The intervals carry emotional content that makes music an art. For instance, the major third sounds joyful and affirmative whereas the minor third sounds sad. The system of frequency relations between the sounds used in music production is called music temperament . 1 Music intervals and overtone series Perfect music intervals (that cannot be fully achieved practically, see below) are based on the overtone series studied before in this course. A typical music sound (except of a pure sinusiodal one) consists of a fundamental frequency f1 and its overtones: = = fn nf 1, n 3,2,1 ,..
    [Show full text]
  • Music Notation, but Not Action on a Keyboard, Influences
    No Effect of Action on the Tritone Paradox 315 MUSIC NOTATION , BUT NOT ACTION ON A KEYBOARD , INFLUENCES PIANISTS’ JUDGMENTS OF AMBIGUOUS MELODIES BRUNO H. REPP Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; Proffitt, Haskins Laboratories 2006; Schütz-Bosbach & Prinz, 2007; Wilson & Knoblich, 2005). Almost all of these studies have been concerned ROBERT M. GOEHR K E with visual perception. However, Repp and Knoblich Yale University (2007, 2009) recently claimed to have found effects of action on auditory perception. PITCH INCREASES FROM LEFT TO RIGHT ON PIANO Their research was based on the fact that on a piano key- keyboards. When pianists press keys on a keyboard to board, the pitch of produced tones increases from left to hear two successive octave-ambiguous tones spanning a right. Pianists are continuously exposed to this relationship, tritone (half-octave interval), they tend to report hear- whereas other musicians and some nonmusicians merely ing the tritone go in the direction consistent with their know it but do not have sensorimotor experience with it. key presses (Repp & Knoblich, 2009). This finding has On each trial of their experiment, Repp and Knoblich been interpreted as an effect of action on perceptual (2009) showed participants a pair of two-digit numbers judgment. Using a modified design, the present study that corresponded to two keys on a labeled piano keyboard separated the effect of the action itself from that of the (a silent MIDI controller). The keys always spanned the visual stimuli that prompt the action. Twelve expert pia- musical interval of a tritone (half an octave), with the sec- nists reported their perception of octave-ambiguous ond key being either higher (i.e., to the right of) or lower three-note melodies ending with tritones in two condi- than (to the left of) the first key.
    [Show full text]