Perception & Psychophysics 2002, 64 (2), 266-278

Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: Support for a planning/control model of action

SCOTT GLOVER and PETER DIXON University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada

A distinction between planning and control can be used to explain the effects of context-induced il- lusions on actions. The present study tested the effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on the planning and control of the grip aperture in grasping a disk. In two experiments, the illusion had an effect on grip aperture that decreased as the hand approached the target, whether or not visual feedback was avail- able. These results are taken as evidence in favor of a planning/control model, in which planning is sus- ceptible to context-induced illusions, whereas control is not. It is argued that many dissociations be- tween perception and action may better be explained as dissociations between perception and on-line control.

The distinctionbetween the premovement planningof an Since Woodworth’s(1899) seminal study,much research actionand its on-linecontrolhas a long history(e.g., Jean- has gone into characterizingthese two stages of action (e.g., nerod, 1988; Keele & Posner, 1968; Woodworth, 1899). Abrams & Pratt, 1993;Elliot,Binsted,& Heath, 1999;Flash Here, we demonstrate that the earlier portions of a grasp- & Henis, 1991; Keele & Posner, 1968; Khan, Franks, & ing movement are more affected by the Ebbinghaus illu- Goodman, 1998; Meyer, Abrams, Kornblum, Wright, & sion than are the latter portions. These results provide fur- Smith, 1988; Pratt & Abrams, 1996), and some distinctions ther support for a planning/control model (Glover, 2001; between the two stages have been elucidated.For example, Glover & Dixon, 2001a,2001b, 2001d)in which planning planning appears to be a relatively slow and deliberate is more susceptible to illusions than control. The results process. The minimum time required to initiatea movement do not support a perception/action model (e.g., Aglioti, has been found to be around 250 msec (Stark, 1968). Con- DeSouza, & Goodale, 1995; Bridgeman, 1999; Bridge- versely, on-line control appears to operate relatively man, Peery, & Anand, 1997; Goodale & Milner, 1992; Mil- quickly.In contrast to the 400-msec lag between planning ner & Goodale, 1995), in which both planning and con- and control stages hypothesized by Woodworth on the trol are thought to be less susceptible to illusions than are basis of his original study, the benefits of visual and pro- perceptions. prioceptivefeedback during on-line control have more re- Woodworth (1899) was the first to demonstrate the dis- cently been found to occur in as little as 70–150 msec (e.g., tinctionbetween the premovement planningand the on-line Evarts & Vaughn, 1978; Lee & Tatton, 1975; Paulignan, control of action. Woodworth found that when participants MacKenzie, Marteniuk,& Jeannerod,1991; Smeets, Erke- were asked to draw lines of particular lengths, they re- lens, & van der Gon, 1990; Zelaznik, Hawkins, & Kissel- quired sufficient time to correct the actions on line. For ex- burgh, 1983). ample, accuracy was worse at a drawing rate of 400 msec We have hypothesized that another difference between per line than at slower rates. Furthermore, if the line- planning and control may be how each is affected by drawing task was done without vision, participants’perfor- context-induced visual illusions. In the planning/control mance at all speeds was just as poor as when the task was model (Glover, 2001; Glover & Dixon, 2001a, 2001b, done quickly with vision. Woodworth reasoned from this 2001d), context-induced illusions affect the planning that actions were composed of two stages: an “initial im- process, but not the on-linecontrolprocess. We use the term pulse” stage that reflected the premovement planning of context-inducedto refer to distortionsthat arise owing to the the action and a subsequent “current control” stage that re- visual context surrounding the target, as opposed to those flected the on-line correction of an action via feedback that result from othertypes of optical effects, such as some mechanisms. perceptualconstancymechanisms (e.g., saccadicsuppres- sion;Goodale,Pelisson,& Prablanc,1986;Prablanc & Mar- tin, 1992), exposure aftereffects (e.g., the McColloughef- Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to fect; McCollough,1965), or the wearing of light-refracting S. Glover, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, devices (e.g., prism goggles; Redding & Wallace, 1994, 541 Moore Bldg., University Park, PA 16402 (e-mail: [email protected]). 1997).

Copyright 2002 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 266 DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 267

The different effects of visual illusionson the planning will be affected by illusionsonly when the perception and and control of actions are assumed to be due to the fact that action modules interact prior to movement initiation. each stage of action uses its own visual representation. These interactions are said to be necessary when infor- The crucial difference between the two representations mation related to the target’sidentity (such as its function, pertainsto the visual context surroundingthe target. On the weight, or texture) is required in order to carry out an one hand, the visual representation used in planning in- action. corporates the visual context surrounding the target. Infor- The perception/action model makes many predictions mation about the context is crucial in planning movements similar to those of the planning/control model and is thus that avoid obstacles and/or depend on affordances involv- also supported by many of the same results (e.g., Aglioti ing the context. On the other hand, the visual representa- et al., 1995; Bridgeman et al., 1997; Daprati & Gentilucci, tion used in control operates relativelyindependentlyof the 1997; Haffenden & Goodale, 1998). However, other re- context. This allows it to operate quickly and with a high sults are more consistent with the planning/control model level of accuracy by focusing its computational power than with the perception/action model. For example, it has solely on the target itself. As a consequence of the differ- been found that illusioneffects on actionare smaller when ential role of the context in the two representations, plan- visual feedback is available to participants, as compared ning is affected by context-induced illusions, whereas with when it is not available (Gentilucci et al., 1996; control is largely immune to such effects. Glover & Dixon, 2001c; Westwood, Heath, & Roy,2000). This model provides a ready account of the reported pat- This supports the notion, inherent in the planning/control tern of illusion effects on action. For example, parameters model, that on-line control processes are responsible for of action that are the most affected by context-inducedil- the small illusion effects on actions. Conversely,any reduc- lusionsare also those that likely reflect planningprocesses, tion in illusion effects owing to the availability of visual such as reaction time (Smeets & Brenner, 1995), move- feedbackwouldseem to be inconsistentwith the perception/ menttime(Gentilucci,Chieffi, Daprati, Saetti,& Toni, 1996; action model, because both the planningand the control of Smeets & Brenner, 1995; van Donkelaar, 1999), posture actions are held to be relatively immune to illusions. choice (Glover & Dixon, 2001a), and initial hand orienta- Another line of evidencein favor of the planning/control tion (Glover & Dixon, 2001a, 2001b, 2001d). Conversely, model comes from studies examining the dynamic illusion parameters of action that are less affected by illusionsare effect (Glover & Dixon, 2001a, 2001b, 2001d). In these those that occur at or near the end of the movement and are studies, we measured the effect of an orientation illusion thus most likely to be influenced by on-line control on the orientation of the hand as participants reached out processes. These include pointing accuracy (Bridgeman, to grasp a bar. We found that the effect of the illusion on Gemmer, Forsman, & Huemer, 2000; Bridgeman, Lewis, the orientationof the hand was large early in the reach but Heit, & Nagle, 1979; Bridgeman et al., 1997), maximum decreased to near zero by the end of the reach, allowing grip aperture (Aglioti et al., 1995; Brenner & Smeets, participantsto grasp the bar without difficulty.According 1996; Daprati & Gentilucci, 1997; Haffenden & Goodale, to the planning/control model, the effect of the illusionon 1998; S. R. Jackson & Shaw, 2000; Otto-de Haart, Carey, hand orientation decreased because the illusion affected & Milne, 1999; but see Franz, Gegenfurtner, Bülthoff, & the initial planning of the movement, but not its subse- Fahle, 2000; Pavani, Boscagli, Benvenuti, Rabuffetti, & quent control. In contrast, a dynamic illusion effect would Farne, 1999), and final hand orientation(Glover & Dixon, not be predicted by a perception/action model. According 2001a, 2001b, 2001d). to this model, the illusion should have a small and consis- An alternative to the planning/control model is the tent effect throughoutthe reach, because bothplanningand perception/action model (Aglioti et al., 1995; Goodale & control make use of a context-independentaction represen- Milner, 1992; Haffenden & Goodale, 1998; Milner & tation. Goodale, 1995). This model is based on a dorsal-stream/ ventral-stream dichotomy in the path- Overview of the Present Study ways of the macaque brain (Mishkin, Ungerleider, & As a further test of the planning/control and perception/ Macko, 1983) and its putative homologue in the human action models, we here measured the effects of the Ebbing- brain. According to the perception/action model, illusion haus size-contrast illusion on grasping. Participants effects on action are often small or nonexistent because reached out and grasped a target disk lying flat in front of actions typically use a visual representation that only them, using a pincer grasp employing the thumb and fore- weakly encodes the context surrounding the target. This finger. During such an action,the grip aperture (i.e., the dis- context-independent action representation is thought to tance between the thumband the forefinger) first increases allow the motor system to accurately compute the charac- to a maximum somewhat larger than the actual size of the teristics of the target for both the planningand the control target, then decreases as the hand closes around the target of actions. Conversely, the perception module is thought (e.g., Jakobson & Goodale, 1991; Jeannerod, 1984; Marte- to includethe visualcontextsurroundingthe target for the niuk, MacKenzie, Jeannerod, Athenes, & Dugaas, 1987; purpose of helpingto identify objects and attach meaning Wing, Turton, & Fraser, 1986).In order to study the effect to them. According to the perception/action model, actions of a context-induced illusion on this action, the apparent 268 GLOVER AND DIXON

of a perception/action model (Aglioti et al., 1995; Haf- fenden & Goodale, 1998; Milner & Goodale, 1995). However, this pattern of results is also consistent with the planning/controlmodel. The principal dependentvari- able in these studies, the maximum grip aperture, usually does not occur until well into the second half of the movement (Jakobson & Goodale, 1991; Jeannerod, 1984; Marteniuk et al., 1987; Wing et al., 1986). Consequently, there is ample time in which on-line control mechanisms could operate (see Bock & Jungling,1999; Castiello,Ben- nett, & Stelmach, 1993; Paulignan, Jeannerod, MacKen- zie, & Marteniuk,1991). In our view, maximum grip aper- ture may not only represent how an action is planned, but also how it is controlled. In the present research, we measured the effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on grasping at a range of points throughoutthe reach, rather than at the point of the maxi- mum grip aperture only. Using this approach,we were able to distinguishthe effects of the illusion on how the grasps were planned (evident early in the movements) from its effects on how the grasps were controlled(evident later in the movements). Figure 1. The Ebbinghaus illusion used in the present study. The targets presented in the middle of each display are the same size. On the top, the contextual circles are large, making the tar- EXPERIMENT 1 get appear slightly smaller than it really is. On the bottom, the contextual circles are small, making the target appear slightly Experiment 1 tested the predictions of the planning/ larger than it really is. control and perception/action models by comparing the ef- fects of the Ebbinghausillusion at several points during the size of the target disk was manipulatedby surroundingthe reach. If the planning/control model is correct, the effect target with circles that were either larger or smaller than of the Ebbinghaus illusion on grip aperture should be the target (Figure 1). The resultingsize-contrastillusionis large early in the reach but decline as the hand approaches known as the Ebbinghaus (or Titchener) illusion (e.g., the target. This would reflect the use of a context-dependent Coren & Girgus, 1978; Coren & Miller, 1974) and has visual representation in planning and the use of a context- been extensively studied with regard to its effects on ac- independentvisualrepresentationin on-linecontrol.Con- tion (Aglioti et al., 1995; Franz et al., 2000; Haffenden & versely, if the perception/action model is correct, the ef- Goodale, 1998; Marotta,DeSouza, Haffenden, & Goodale, fect of the illusion on grip aperture should be small and 1998; Pavani et al., 1999). The Ebbinghaus illusion owes fairly stable throughoutthe course of the reach. This would its effects to the relation between the target and the con- reflect the use of a context-independent action represen- textual figures. Specifically, when the contextual figures tation in both the planning and the control of action. are larger than the target, the target appears to be smaller than it really is (Figure 1, top). Conversely, when the con- Method Participants. Twelve University of Alberta undergraduates par- textual figures are smaller than the target, the target ap- ticipated in the experiment in return for course credit. All 12 partic- pears larger than it really is (Figure 1, bottom). ipants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vi- Past studies have often found that the effect of the sion. All the participants were naive as to the exact purpose of the Ebbinghausillusionon grasping is dissociablefrom its ef- study. fects on perceptualjudgments(e.g., Agliotiet al., 1995;Haf- Apparatus. The participant sat on an adjustable chair at a 100 ´ ´ fenden & Goodale, 1998; but see Franz et al., 2000; Marotta 60 cm table, behind a 20 80 cm two-way mirror positioned ap- proximately 10 cm from the participant’s eyes. The top of the two-way et al., 1998; Pavani et al., 1999). For example, Aglioti et al. mirror was rotated 15º away from the participant in the frontal plane. found that the effect of the Ebbinghaus illusion on the The participant’s vision above and around the table top was restricted maximum grip aperture (i.e., the maximum distance be- by the testing apparatus, but he or she was able to see the table top, tween the thumb and the finger in grasping) was smaller including his or her reaching hand and arm, throughout each trial. than its effect on perceptual judgments indexed through Head and eye movements were not restricted. the method of comparison. A follow-up study by Haf- A system of lights was used to either block or allow vision through the two-way mirror. One light was positioned above the fenden and Goodale replicated this finding under visual table at a height of 52 cm, close to the participant’s end of the table. open-loop conditions, suggesting that the small effect on This light remained lit throughout the experiment. Two floodlights maximum grip aperture did not depend on visual feed- were set on the table to the left and right sides of the workspace, and back. These results have been taken as evidence in favor two more floodlights were positioned above the participant, to the DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 269

left and right. To allow vision through the two-way mirror, the lights on the table, making the stimulus array visible through the two-way on the table were lit, and the lights above the participant were extin- mirror. The participants were required to reach out and pick up the guished. To block vision through the mirror, the lighting arrange- disk, using the thumb and the forefinger, and then set the disk down ment was reversed. When the table top was visible, its illumination on the half of the table top nearest themselves. Instructions to the was 0.9 cd/m2 (measured from the participant’s side of the two-way participants did not emphasize speed. Six randomly determined mirror). When the table top was not visible, the participant saw only practice trials were run before the test trials to allow the participants the reflection of the ceiling in the two-way mirror. to familiarize themselves with the task. U.S. letter-sized (27.9 ´ 21.6 cm) pieces of paper with the Ebbing- Two contextual circles conditions were used. In half the trials, the haus circles illusion printed on them were placed on the table in the context consisted of the large circles; in the other half, it consisted center of the participant’s field of view. The paper occupied 29.9º of of the small circles. For each context condition, five disk sizes were visual angle in the depth plane and 23.1º of visual angle in the hor- used: 26, 28, 30, 32, and 34 mm. In each of two blocks, the partici- izontal plane. The large-circles version of the illusion (Figure 1, top) pants received seven repetitions of each context and disk combina- was made up of 6 evenly spaced circles (41-mm diameter, or 4.3º of tion, for a total of 120 trials. The participants were allowed a brief visual angle) surrounding the center of the paper. The edge of each rest between blocks if they so desired. large circle was 25 mm (2.7º of visual angle) from the center of the Data analysis. Data were analyzed by passing the position data paper. The small-circles version of the illusion (Figure 1, bottom) from the two ireds through a custom filter. The onset of each reach was made up of 12 evenly spaced circles (1.0-mm diameter, or 1.1º was set at the time when the movement velocity of the thumb ex- of visual angle) surrounding the center of the paper. The edge of ceeded 0.10 m/sec. The offset of the reach was set at the time when each small circle was 20 mm (2.1º of visual angle) from the center the thumb ceased to move in a forward direction. The thumb was of the paper. used for determining velocity, because most of the change in grip Small white plastic disks, ranging in 2-mm increments from aperture during a pincer grasp is due to flexion or extension of the 26 mm (2.8º of visual angle) to 34 mm (3.6º of visual angle), replaced finger; as a consequence, the thumb provides a relatively stable the target circles in the middle of the paper. Each disk was 3 mm thick. index of the overall position of the hand during the reach (Haggard The outside rims of the disks were painted black so that the disks & Wing, 1997; Wing & Fraser, 1983; Wing et al., 1986). Grip aper- would resemble the contextual circles as closely as possible. The ture was computed for each sampled position from reach onset to center of the disks was approximately 43 cm from the participant’s offset. For each measurement of grip aperture, the position of the midsection. A 1 3 5 cm cylindrical starting bar was taped to the table, ired that was not lit during that frame was interpolated between the using surgical adhesive tape, so that its length was parallel with the position measurements for the preceding and the following frames. participant’s frontal plane. The center of the starting bar was ap- Measurements were omitted when they implied a movement veloc- proximately 23 cm from the participant’s midsection and exactly ity greater than 1.0 m/sec or when the positions were outside the nor- 20 cm from the center of the disks. mal range of movement. Positions for these omitted observations The participant wore two infrared light-emitting diodes (ireds), were estimated by interpolation. Trials were excluded if either the re- one taped to the mediodorsal surface of the thumb, roughly halfway action time or the movement time was less than 250 msec or greater between the interphalangeal joint and the tip of the thumb, and the than 1,500 msec or if the starting position was outside a normal other to the laterodorsal surface of the index finger, roughly halfway range. Apart from 1 participant who was excluded owing to poor between the proximal interphalangeal and the distal interphalangeal recording, 96.6% of the trials from 11 participants were included in joints. If one imagines a participant’s hand lying on a table with the the final analysis. palm facing down, the ireds were worn on the skin near the base of On completion of the filtering process, the data for each trial were the nails of the thumb and forefinger, on the side to the right of the time normalized. This involved estimating the position and grip nail on the thumb and to the left of the nail on the finger. The ireds aperture by interpolation for 21 equally spaced points in time rang- were attached with wires to a computer, and the loose wire was fas- ing from the onset of the movement to its offset. Each time slice thus tened to the participant’s forearm with enough play that the partici- represented 5% of the duration of the movement. The normalized pant could move his or her arm and hand freely. None of the partic- trials for each background and disk condition were averaged for each ipants reported feeling any restriction or discomfort while wearing participant. the ireds. The magnitude of the illusion effect on grip aperture was ex- The table top was monitored with an infrared video camera fixed pressed relative to the effect of the (veridical) disk size. First, for to a stable mount at a height of 70 cm above the table. The two ireds each time slice, the ratio of disk size to grip aperture was computed, were alternately illuminated at 60 Hz, and the position of the lit averaging over participants and context condition. This size–aperture diode was detected electronically every video frame and recorded ratio represents the extent to which the grip varied with disk size at using an IScan tracking system. The IScan system output the posi- each time slice. Generally, the ratio starts out small at the beginning tion of the lit diode to a computer at 60 Hz, with a nominal resolu- of the reach and increases as the hand approaches the target disk. tion of 1.2 mm horizontally and 2.2 mm in depth. In order to assess the Second, the raw effect of the illusion was computed for each subject precision of the system for monitoring movements, we used a method and time slice by subtracting the grip aperture measured with the adapted from Haggard and Wing (1990). Two diodes were attached small-circles context from that with the large-circles context. Finally, to a plastic card 8 cm apart. The card was swept systematically across the scaled illusion effect for each participant and time slice was cal- different portions of the camera field of view at velocities compara- culated by dividing the raw illusion effect by the size–aperture ratio ble with those during reaching (about 0.5 m/sec). The distance be- for that time slice. The scaled illusion effect represents the magni- tween the diodes was reconstructed from the system output, with a tude of the illusion effect relative to the extent to which grip aper- standard deviation of 0.60 mm laterally and 1.04 mm in depth. There ture is tuned to the size of the disk. In other words, the scaled effect were no measurable deviations from linearity in the workspace using provides a measure of how much of a difference in actual disk size the IScan system. is represented by the raw illusion effect. The scaled effect of the il- Experimental procedure. The participants were required to lusion on grip aperture was used in the subsequent analysis. begin each trial by pinching the starting bar with the thumb and fore- Two issues were assessed statistically. First, was there an overall finger, keeping the other fingers curled underneath the hand. A trial effect of the context-induced illusion on grip aperture—that is, was began when the computer, at the experimenter’s instigation, simul- the average scaled illusion effect greater than zero? Second, did the taneously extinguished the lights behind the screen and lit the lights scaled illusion effect change over time? These questions were ad- 270 GLOVER AND DIXON

the p value found by testing the hypothesis of no effect (Dixon, 1998), and, if one were to do null hypothesis significance testing, the null hypothesis would generally be rejected if the likelihood ratio were 10 or greater.

Results Figure 2 shows the effect of disk size on grip aperture over time for the 11 participants who produced usable data. It can be seen in Figure 2 that the grip aperture grad- ually became more dependent on the disk size as the movements progressed. The maximum grip aperture oc- curred at around 65% of the movement, comparable with what has been typicallyfound (Jakobson & Goodale, 1991; Jeannerod, 1984; Marteniuk et al., 1987; Wing & Fraser, 1983). The scaled difference in grip aperture between the two contextualcircles conditionsis shown in Figure 3. The data were analyzed by comparing the scaled effect of the con- text at t = 0.4, 0.6, 0.8, and 1.0 (i.e., at the completion of 40%, 60%, 80%, and 100% of the reach). The scaled effect for the times prior to t = 0.4 were excludedbecause the ratio relating grip aperture to disk size was very small early in the reach and, as a consequence, the scaled illusion effect Figure 2. The effect of each of the five disk sizes on grip aper- ture in Experiment 1. Grip aperture is represented on the y-axis, tended to be excessivelyvariable at those times. Only four normalized time on the x-axis. pointswere used in the analysis to reduce the potentialfor dependence between observations at successive points in time. It can be seen in Figure 3 that there was a consistent dressed by fitting a series of nested linear models to the data for the decline in the effect of the illusion from t = 0.4 onwards, scaled illusion effect. In the first model, it was assumed that the and this was also supportedby the statisticalanalysis.The mean illusion effect was zero at all points in time—that is, likelihood ratio comparing a model that included a con- m t =0. stant effect of the illusion with a null model that included only an effect of participantswas l > 1,000. In other words, In the second model, it was assumed that there was a constant effect of the illusion—that is, the data were more than 1,000 times as likely on the as- sumption that an effect of the illusionwas present than on m a t = . the assumptionthat there was no effect of the illusion.The In the third model, an effect of time was added: likelihood ratio comparing a full model that included an m a b t = + t, where b = å t 0. t The relative fits of these three models were evaluated by computing maximum-likelihood ratios. The likelihood ratio indicates the like- lihood of the data given the best fit of one model divided by the like- lihood of the data given the best fit of the other. The likelihood ratio provides an index of the strength of the evidence for one model rel- ative to the other (Goodman & Royall, 1988). The likelihood of the data for a linear model with normally distributed error is closely re- lated to the residual variance not predicted by the model and can be computed as follows (Dixon & O’Reilly, 1999): n æ SSE ö 2 l = ç 2 ÷ , è ø SSE1 where SSE1 and SSE2 reflect the fit of the two models and n is the number of independent observations. Because the scaled illusion ef- fect is essentially a difference score, we assumed that the observed effects for each subject at each point in time were independent. We Figure 3. The scaled illusion effect on grip aperture in Exper- adopt the heuristic that a likelihood ratio of 10 or greater indicates iment 1. The effect is shown for the four points in normalized relatively clear evidence for one model over the other. Under many cir- time used in the data analysis. Error bars represent standard er- cumstances, the likelihood ratio thus obtained is closely related to rors of the mean. DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 271 effect of the illusion and an effect of time (consistent with loss of proprioception through pathologyhas detrimental the planning/controlmodel) with a model that includedan effects on motor performance, suggesting that proprio- effect of the illusion but no effect of time (consistent with ception is important for the control of action (Gentilucci, the perception/action model) was l = 14.1. In other words, Toni, Chieffi, & Pavesi, 1994; G. M. Jackson et al., 2000; the data were more than 14 times as likely on the assump- Jeannerod, Michel, & Prablanc, 1984; Lee & Tatton, 1975; tion that an effect of time was present than on the as- Sainburg, Poizner, & Ghez, 1993). A role for efference sumption that there was no effect of time. copy may also be present, on the basis of studiesthat show Mean reaction time across participants was 613 msec that localization of an actively moved limb (i.e., one the (range, 389–929 msec). Mean movement time across par- participant moved himself or herself) is superior to local- ticipants was 570 msec (range, 442–904 msec). There was ization of a passively moved limb (i.e., a limb moved by no indication that the illusion or the size of the disks had the experimenter; Eklund, 1972; Paillard & Brouchon, effects on either reaction times or movement times. 1968). Thus, there is evidence that both visual and nonvi- sual sources of information contribute to on-line control. Discussion On the basis of this, we predict that in the planning/ The fact that the effect of the Ebbinghausillusionon grip control model, illusion effects should decrease over time aperture decreased as the hand approached the target in whether or not visual feedback is available, although the Experiment 1 is consistent with the planning/control model. reduction in the illusion effect over time may be less sub- In this model, it is held that actions are planned using a stantial in a no-visionconditionthan in a visioncondition. context-dependentrepresentation that is susceptible to il- In contrast,the perception/actionmodel includesthe premise lusions but are controlled using a context-independent that actions are planned and controlled using a common representationthat is immune to visual illusions.The data visual representation that is less susceptible to illusions from Experiment 1 are less consistent with a perception/ than are perceptions. This would seem to imply that re- action model, in which it is held that actions are both moving vision should not affect the illusion effect on planned and controlled using a context-independent rep- grasping. Furthermore, as was discussed in connection resentation that is immune to illusions and separate from with Experiment 1, according to a proponent of the that used in perception.The dynamic illusioneffect might perception/action model, the effect of the illusion should be explained within the perception/action model by as- not change over the course of the reach, whether or not vi- suming that the two modules interact in the course of the sion is available. action, with the perception module making a substantial Experiment 2 also included a test of the perceptual ef- contribution to the planning of the movement. However, fects of the Ebbinghaus illusion. It is possible that the ef- since there would appear to be no a priori necessity for an fect of the illusion in our setup may have been different interaction of this sort, such an assumption would tend to from that in previous studies because the stimulus display undermine the distinction between perception and action was viewed from a seated position (rather than standing) that motivates this approach. and was thus slightly foreshortened. A perceptual task would thus provide measurements that could be directly EXPERIMENT 2 compared with the effect observed in grasping.

Although Experiment 1 revealed a dynamic effect of the Method Ebbinghausillusion on grasping, showing that the effects Participants. Sixteen University of Alberta undergraduates par- of this illusion on action could be corrected in flight, the ticipated in the experiment in return for course credit. All 16 partic- results do not indicate which sources of information were ipants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vi- sion. All the participants were naive as to the exact purpose of the crucial to the correction process. Information from any or study. allof visualfeedback,proprioception,or efference copymay Apparatus. The apparatus was identical to that used in Experi- have contributed.The aim of Experiment 2 was thus to dis- ment 1. sociate the contributions of visual and nonvisual sources Reaching task procedure. The participants performed two of information in the correction process by including a no- blocks of reaching trials and one block of perception trials. The vision conditionin which participantsreached without vi- reaching trials were divided into one vision and one no-vision block. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across participants, with sion of their hand or the target. A vision conditionlike that the perception block coming directly before the reaching blocks in used in Experiment 1 was also included for comparison. half of the participants and directly after the reaching blocks in the It is commonly understood that visual feedback aids in other half of the participants. The procedure for reaching trials in the accuracy of reaching movements (Elliot et al., 1999; the vision block was identical to that used in Experiment 1. Jeannerod,1988;Keele & Posner, 1968;Woodworth,1899). The no-vision trials differed from the vision trials in the follow- However, on-line adjustments have also been found to ing respects: In the no-vision trials, the participants were first given a preview of the display. After 2 sec, the participants’ view of the occur in the absence of visual feedback (e.g., Goodale et al., table top (including their hands) was blocked, and a tone signaled the 1986; Khan et al., 1998; Prablanc & Martin, 1992), demon- participants to reach. Other than the 2-sec preview, the tone, and the strating that nonvisualsources of information also play an lack of vision during the reach, the task was identical in all respects important role during on-line control. For example, the to that in the vision trials. The participants were given 6 randomly de- 272 GLOVER AND DIXON termined practice trials prior to beginning the vision task and 10 ran- repetitions were averaged. Poor recording resulted in the exclusion domly determined practice trials prior to beginning the no-vision of 5.6% of the trials. However, at least one trial for each condition task. for each participant was available, and no participants had to be ex- The context and disk conditions used in the reaching task were cluded for this reason. The scaled effect of the illusion on perceptual identical to those used in Experiment 1, except that only six repetitions estimates was calculated by dividing the raw illusion effect on man- of each condition were used in each of the vision and no-vision ual estimates by the ratio of the effect of disk size on manual esti- blocks. This resulted in 60 trials in each of the two reaching blocks, mates. for 120 trials in total. The participants were allowed a brief rest be- tween the two blocks of reaching trials. Results Perception task procedure. The setup for the perception task Figure 4 shows the effect of disk size on grip aperture was identical to that for the reaching tasks, but here the participant was required to match the size of the disks, using the thumb and over time for both the vision (left) and the no-vision(right) forefinger. Trials began with the experimenter’s setting up the dis- conditions.A clear dependenceof grip aperture on disk size play with the participant’s vision blocked. Once the display was set was present from the beginning of the movement in both up, the lights were switched, and the participant had to lift his or her conditions.It is clear from Figure 4 that the grip aperture hand up off the starting bar and indicate the size of the disk, using was generally larger and reached its peak slightly earlier the thumb and forefinger. He or she was instructed not to move the in the no-vision condition than in the vision condition. hand toward the disk (which could have allowed him or her to inter- pose the hand in the line of sight of the disk). A trial continued for This replicatesfindingsreported in other studiesof grasp- 5 sec, at which time a tone sounded. The participant was then required ing without vision (Jakobson & Goodale, 1991; Jean- to return the hand to the starting position and await the next trial. nerod, 1984). In half the perception trials, the context consisted of the large cir- The scaled illusion effect for the two vision conditions cles; in the other half, it consisted of the small circles. For each con- and the four analyzed time slices is shown in Figure 5. The text condition, three disk sizes were used: 26, 30, and 34 mm. The illusion effect in the two vision conditions tended to de- participants received two repetitions of each context and disk com- bination, for a total of 2 repetitions 3 3 disks 3 2 contexts = 12 tri- crease as the hand approached the target, and this decrease als. The order of presentation was random. was more evident in the no-visionconditionthan in the vi- Data analysis. Data from the reaching trials were analyzed in a sion condition.When a model that included a constant ef- fashion identical to that in Experiment 1. A total of 94.5 % of the fect of the illusion was compared with a null model, the reaching trials from 15 of the participants were included in the final likelihoodratio was large (l > 500). That is, the data were analysis. One participant was excluded owing to recording failure. more than 500 times as likely on the basis of the assump- Data from the perception trials were analyzed by dividing the tion that the illusion had an effect on grip aperture than on recordings into 11 measurements spaced at 100-msec intervals over the final 1,000 msec of each trial. Distance measures from these the assumptionthat the illusionhad no effect. When an ef- 11 measurements were averaged, and the mean distance was com- fect of time was added intothis model, the likelihoodratio puted for each trial for each participant. Distances across the two comparing it with the model that included only the effect

Figure 4. The effect of each of the five disk sizes on grip aperture in the vision (left) and no- vision (right) conditions of Experiment 2. Grip aperture is represented on the y-axis, normalized time on the x-axis. DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 273

Figure 5. The scaled effect of the illusion on grip aperture in the vision (left) and no-vision (right) conditions of Experiment 2. The effect in each condition is shown for the four points in normalized time used in the data analysis.Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. of the illusion was also large (l = 17.0), suggesting that results found previously. Consequently, we attribute the the illusion effect decreased as the hand approached the lack of clear evidence for an effect of time to the variabil- target. ity of the data. Although the pattern of means shown in Figure 5 sug- Mean reaction time across participants was 534 msec gests an interaction in which the illusion effect early in the (range, 341–759 msec) in the vision condition and 461 reach is larger without vision, the evidencefor such an in- msec (range, 287–686 msec) in the no-vision condition. teraction was not compelling. When a full model that Mean movement time across participants was 731 msec addedin the effects of visionconditionand the interaction (range, 564–1,041 msec) in the vision condition and 923 of time with vision was compared with a model that in- msec (range, 674–1,154 msec) in the no-vision condition. cluded only the effects of the illusion and time, the likeli- Reaction times were shorter in the no-vision condition than hood ratio was l = 7.0. To further examine this possibility, in the vision condition, presumably because the partici- we compared the largest illusion effects in the two vision pants in the no-vision conditionwere better able to antic- conditionsat t = 0.4. Here, the illusioneffect was 0.64 mm ipate the onset of each trial. Movement times were some- in the vision conditionand 1.83 mm in the no-vision con- dition. In this case, the 95% confidence interval for the difference was -1.54 to 3.32 mm and included 0. In sum, whereas there was relatively clear evidence for a constant effect of the illusion and time in Experiment 2, there was no strong evidence either for an effect of either vision con- dition or for an interaction between time and vision. The effect of the illusion and its interaction with time were also evident in the no-vision conditionwhen consid- ered by itself. For the no-vision data, when a null model was compared with a model that included a constant effect of the illusion, the likelihood ratio was large (l = 32.8); when the constant model was compared with a model that included an effect of time, the likelihood ratio was l = 19.3. This constitutes clear evidence that the effect of the illusionwas greater than zero, on average, and diminished as the hand approached the disk. A similar analysis of the data in the vision condition by itself revealed evidence for a constant effect of the illusion (l = 26.9) but no clear ev- Figure 6. The effects of background condition and disk size in l the perceptual matching task of Experiment 2. Estimates made idence that the effect varied over time ( = 1.2). However, with the small-circles background are represented by the small this conditionwas essentiallya replicationof Experiment1, circles; estimates made with the large-circles background are and the pattern of means is generally consistent with the represented by the large circles. 274 GLOVER AND DIXON what longer in the no-vision condition than in the vision ies have shown that it is more common for the early com- condition,a common finding in studies comparing reach- ponent of the action to be more affected by an optical il- ing with and without vision (Connolly & Goodale, 1999; lusion than are perceptually based judgments (Glover & Jeannerod,1984).There was no indicationthat background Dixon, 2001a, 2001d, 2002a). In a related study, printed or disk size affected reaction times or movement times. words were found to have large effects on planningbut no Figure 6 shows the results of the perception task in Ex- effects on either control or perceptions (Glover & Dixon, periment 2. It is evident from Figure 6 that the participants 2002b). gave larger estimates for the disks surrounded by small circles than for the disks surrounded by large circles. To as- Discussion sess the effects of the illusionon the estimates,we compared Experiment 2 replicated the dynamic illusion effect nested linear models. Comparing a model that includedan found in Experiment 1. The data in the no-vision condition effect of disk with a null model gave a large likelihood showed that continuous visual information was not re- ratio (l > 1,000). Comparing an additive model that in- quired for the on-linecorrectionof illusioneffects on grasp- cluded effects of both disk and illusion with the model that ing. These data support the planning/control model, in included the effect of disk only also gave a large likeli- which actions are planned using a context-dependent vi- hood ratio (l > 1,000). However, comparing the full model sual representation but are corrected on line, using a that included the effects of disk and illusion and their in- context-independentrepresentation.In contrast, these data teraction with the additive model yielded a small likeli- do not supporta perception/actionmodel in which actions hood ratio (l = 1.52). Thus, there were large effects of are both plannedand executedusing a context-independent both disk and illusion on the perceptual estimates, but action representation. According to this model, the illu- there was little indication of an interaction between these sion should have had a consistently small effect on grip two variables. aperture throughout the reach. Figure 7 summarizes the results of the two experiments. The findingthat the dynamicillusioneffect occurred in Shown are the effects of the illusion on grasping in the the no-visionconditionsupports the notionthat nonvisual early (t = 0.4) and late (t = 1.0) portions of the reach. Also mechanisms, such as proprioception and efference copy, shown is the illusion effect on perceptually based match- can be used to adjust reaching movementson line (Goodale ing. It can be seen in this figure that the effect of the illu- et al., 1986; Khan et al., 1998; Prablanc & Martin, 1992). sion tends to be smaller in the grasping task (even rela- The results of Experiment 2 also indicate that continuous tively early in the reaches) than in the perceptualjudgment visual information does not play an essential role in the task. Although this result suggests that illusions have correction of the effects of the Ebbinghausillusionon ac- smaller effects on actions than on perceptions,other stud- tion.However, this does not discountthe role of any visual

Figure 7. Summary of results of Experiments 1 and 2. Shown are scaled illusion effects for t = 0.4 and t = 1 for the grasping task in Ex- periment 1 and the two conditions of Experiment 2. Also shown is the scaled illusion effect for the perceptual judgment task of Experiment 2. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 275 information in on-line control: The control system may Another possible explanation for the dynamic illusion use stored visual information regarding the target even effect observed here and elsewhere is that proprioceptive when there is no feedback loop. information becomes more relevant during movement In previous reports, the magnitude of the Ebbinghausil- than prior to movement. On this account, illusion effects lusion on perception has been between 2.25 and 2.50 mm are corrected on line because proprioception operates as (Aglioti et al., 1995; Franz et al., 2000; Haffenden & the hand is in flight to correct any error in the planning of Goodale, 1998; Pavani et al., 1999), whereas in the present the movement. Although this suggestion is intuitively research, the effect was somewhat smaller (2.05 mm). The plausible,we note that the present study cannot be used to smaller perceptual effect may have been due to the partic- compare such a notion with the planning/control model. ipants’ slightly foreshortened view of the display. How- ever, despite the smaller perceptual effect, the illusion still Other Studies of the Ebbinghaus Illusion and had clear effects in both the reaching and the perception Grasping tasks. Furthermore, the perceptual effect of the illusion Several authors have argued that the apparentdifference was generally larger than the effect on grip aperture in the between the effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on action last 60% of the reach. As we mentioned earlier in regard and on perceptiondependson the manner in which the ac- to Figure 7, the illusioneffect on grip aperture at t =0.4was tion and the perception tasks are conducted.For example, nearly as large as its effect on perception, but by the end in the perception task used by Aglioti et al. (1995), par- of the reach the effect on grip aperture had become sub- ticipants compared the apparent sizes of two disks pre- stantially smaller than the effect on perception. sentedwith different contextualsurrounds,whereas in the grasping task, participants reached out to grasp only one GENERAL DISCUSSION of the disks. Franz et al. (2000) and Pavani et al. (1999) ar- gued that the effect of the illusion may have been reduced In the present study, we demonstrated a dynamic effect in the grasping task because participants were required to of the Ebbinghausillusionon grasping, in both full-vision attend only to one disk at a time. In support of this inter- and no-visionconditions.This result replicates and extends pretation, these authors found little evidence for a differ- a similar dynamic effect obtainedwith an orientation illu- ence between grasping and perceptual judgments when sion in reaching (Glover & Dixon, 2001a, 2001b, 2001d). both tasks involved only a single target and its surround- In previous studies, the Ebbinghaus illusion was found to ing context.Franz et al. concluded that perceptionand ac- have a smaller effect on the maximum grip aperture than tion use a common visual representationand that both are on perceptual judgments of target size (Aglioti et al., equally affected by the Ebbinghaus illusion, whereas Pa- 1995; Franz et al., 2000;Haffenden & Goodale,1998; Pa- vani et al. concluded that their results reflected an inter- vani et al., 1999). However, when we measured the effect action between perception and action. of the illusion on grip aperture throughout the course of Although the results of Franz et al. (2000) and Pavani the reach here, we found that the effect of the illusionwas et al. (1999) suggest a need for cautionwhen comparing the large early in the reach and decreased as the hand ap- results of perception and action tasks, their specific con- proached the target. Thus, the relativelysmall effect of the cerns would not seem to apply to the manner in which the illusion on maximum grip aperture may be due to the fact perception and the action tasks were matched in the Haf- that the maximum grip aperture occurs at a relatively late fenden and Goodale (1998) study. In that study, the effect point in the movement. Together, the results of Experi- of the illusion on perception was clearly larger than its ef- ments 1 and 2 suggest that, although the planning of the fect on the maximum grip aperture, even though the two grasp is affected by the illusion, the on-line control of the tasks were appropriatelymatched by the criterion of Franz grasp is not. This is consistent with the planning/control et al. and Pavani et al. Similarly,in Experiment 2 of the pres- model, but not with a perception/action model. ent study, the effect of the illusion on grip aperture was The dynamicillusioneffect found in the no-visioncon- generally less than its effects on perceptions,even though dition was expected on the basis of past studies that have the two tasks were appropriately matched. demonstrated the contributions of proprioception and/or Another factor that may have affected the results in the efference copy to on-linecontrol.Althoughvisual feedback grasping tasks of the present study was the presence of the may seem to be the most intuitivecandidate for the correc- annulus (ring of circles) around the target. Haffenden and tion of illusion effects on line, the present study suggests Goodale (1998) observed that the presence of the annulus that its role under the present circumstances may be rela- may have the effect of presenting “obstacles” to the grasp- tively minor. However, in other studies using the Müller- ing hand.Generally,the maximumgrip aperture was smaller Lyer illusion,the role of continuousvisionof the effectors in their study when the hand reached into the center of and targets has been more evident (Gentilucci et al., 1996; such an annulus than when no annulus was present. In the Glover & Dixon, 2001c; Westwood et al., 2000), and a present study, it is difficult to imagine how the presence of complete understandingof the relative importance of con- the annulus would have interacted with the decreasing ef- tinuous vision in the on-line correction of illusion effects fect of the illusion on grasping. We would suggest that, on action requires further research. being a contextual element, the annulus likely has its ef- 276 GLOVER AND DIXON fects mainly on the planningof the grasp. Indeed, if the an- system (Savelsbergh,Whiting,& Bootsma, 1991). Because nulus were the sole cause of the dynamic illusioneffect in these motor adjustments occur during the movements this case, it would seem strange that the effects of other il- themselves, they would seem to imply a specific dissoci- lusions also decrease over time (Glover & Dixon, 2001a, ation between perception and on-line control, rather than 2001b, 2001c, 2001d). a more general dissociation between perception and ac- tion. In contrast to this evidence for different visual infor- Extensions of the Planning/Control Model mation’s being used for perception and control, support In the present study, we have explained the effects of for the notion that the planning of movements is dissocia- the Ebbinghausillusion on action in terms of a dichotomy ble from perception is much less evident. between the planning and the on-line control of action. However, it is necessary to point out that the planning/ This was also the case for the results of our studies using control model may not account as well for eye movements an orientation illusion (Glover & Dixon, 2001a, 2001b, as it does for othertypes of movements. For example, Wong 2001d) and a size-contrast illusion (Glover & Dixon, and Mack (1981) observedthat participantswere able to ac- 2001d). Furthermore, a similar pattern of on-line correc- curately perform saccadic eye movements to a target de- tion was observed for the effects of printed words on grasp- spite illusory motion induced by a shift in a frame sur- ing (Glover & Dixon, 2002b). In all four paradigms, a roundingthe target (Roelef’seffect). Crucially,the context-inducedoptical illusion(or word) had a larger ef- made by the participantsin this studydid not originallygo fect on action in the earlier portion of the movement than in the direction of the perceived motion, as would be pre- in the latter portion. Thus, it would appear that the visual dicted by the planning/control model. Rather, the saccades information used during planning is more heavily influ- moved accurately to the target location. enced by cognitive and perceptualvariablesthan is on-line We suggest that this result may occur because eye and control.Indeed,inasmuch as effects of the illusionson con- limb movements operate under very different constraints. trol are demonstrablydifferent from their effects on planning On the one hand, saccades involve the two-dimensional and perceptions,the present results represent evidencefor rotation of the eyes in the orbit and have no need to take a dissociation not just between planning and control, but into account any obstacles or environmental affordances. also between perceptionand control.Next, we will describe On the other hand, a body movement (of which reaching how many other apparent dissociations between percep- and grasping is but one example) generally involves a tion and action may be better explained as dissociations three-dimensional trajectory through physical space and between perception and control. must account for obstacles and affordances. Thus, the con- One class of dissociation between perception and con- text of the target may be much less important for the plan- trol is related to the observation that the motor system can ning of saccades than it is for the planning of body move- accommodate changes in the target that occur outside of ments, and as a consequence,context-inducedillusionsmay conscious awareness. For example,a perturbationin a tar- have little effect on saccadic planning.Consistentwith this get’s position during a often goes unnoticed, a distinctionbetween eye and body movements, there is ev- phenomenon related to saccadic suppression. However, idence that the neural structures underlying the two types pointing movements directed at the target can compensate of movements are differently localized (Anderson et al., accurately for the change in position (Bridgeman et al., 1994; Deiber, Ibanez, Sadato, & Hallett, 1996) and rely 1979; Goodale et al., 1986; Prablanc & Martin, 1992). on separate efferent pathways (Kolb & Whishaw, 1995). Although these studies have been held as evidence for a dissociation between perception and action (Bridgeman Conclusions et al., 1979; Goodale & Milner, 1992; Milner & Goodale, The present study revealed a dynamic effect of the 1995), the adjustments to the new location of the target Ebbinghaus illusion on grasping. This effect occurred were being made in flight and, thus, presumably reflected whether or not vision was availableduring the reach. These the operation of the on-line control system. results support the planning/control model of action; in this Other results also support a distinction between per- model, planning uses a context-dependentvisual represen- ception and on-line control. For example, Castiello and tation, whereas control uses a context-independentrepre- Jeannerod (1991) reported that when a target’slocationwas sentation.As such, context-inducedopticalillusionsshould moved coincident with the onset of a reach, participants affect the planningof an action, but not its subsequent con- made adjustments in their trajectories well before they trol. In contrast, the results of the present study do not sup- were able to verbally respond to the change (on the order port a perception/action model, in which actions are both of 150 msec for motor adjustments vs. 300 msec for verbal planned and controlled using a visual representation that response). In fact, participants reported having the sensa- is context independent. tion that they had adjusted the trajectories of their reaches The planning/control model provides a compelling ex- before they had become aware of the change in the posi- planationof the effects of context-inducedillusionson ac- tion of the target. Similarly, a subtle change in the size of tions. It can also accommodate many of the findings that an approaching target that goes unnoticed by participants have typically been interpreted as dissociations between can be quicklyand accurately accommodatedby the motor perception and action. In sum, we believe that the plan- DYNAMIC ILLUSION EFFECTS IN GRASPING 277 ning/control model provides a valuable framework for in- Gentilucci,M., Chieffi, S., Daprati, E.,Saetti, M., & Toni, I. (1996). terpreting a range of phenomena related to the organiza- Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia, 34, 369-376. Gentilucci, M., Toni, I., Chieffi, S., & Pavesi, G. tion and function of the motor system. (1994). The role of proprioception in the control of prehension movements: A kinematic study in a peripherally deafferented patient and in normal subjects. REFERENCES Experimental Brain Research, 99, 483-500. Glover,S. (2001). Separate visual representations in the planning and Abrams, R. A., & Pratt, J. (1993). Rapid aimed limb movements: Dif- control of action: Studies in healthy and brain-damaged populations. ferential effects of practice on component submovements. Journal of Manuscript submitted for publication. Motor Behavior, 25, 288-298. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001a). Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast task: Evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and illusionsdeceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679-685. control of reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Anderson, T.J., Jenkins,I., Brooks,D., Hawken, M., Frackowiak,R., Perception & Performance, 27, 560-572. & Kennard, C. (1994). Cortical control of saccades and fixation in Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001b). Motor adaptation to an optical illu- man: A PET study. Brain, 117, 1073-1084. sion. Experimental Brain Research, 137, 254-258. Bock, O., & Jungling, S. (1999). Reprogramming of grip aperture in Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001c). The Müller-Lyer illusion and the ac- a double-step virtual grasping paradigm. Experimental Brain Re- curacy of lower limb movements. Manuscript in preparation. search, 125, 61-66. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001d). The role of vision in the on-line cor- Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. (1996). Size illusion influences how we lift rection of illusion effects on action. Canadian Journal of Experimen- but not how we grasp an object. Experimental Brain Research, 111, tal Psychology, 55, 96-103. 473-476. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2002a). The importance of contour discrimi- Bridgeman, B. (1999).Separate representations of visual space for per- nation in the on-line correction of illusion effects on action. Manu- ception and visually guided behavior. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bach- script in preparation. mann, & J. Muessler (Eds.), Cognitive contributionsto the perception Glover,S., & Dixon, P. (2002b). Semantics affect the planning but not of spatial & temporal events (pp. 4-13). Amsterdam: Elsevier. on-line control of grasping. Manuscript submitted for publication. Bridgeman, B., Gemmer, A., Forsman, T., & Huemer, V. (2000). Pro- Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. (1992). Separate visual pathways for cessing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 20-25. system. Vision Research, 40, 3539-3552. Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjust- Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., & Nagle, M. (1979). Relation be- ments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand tween cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position per- or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748-750. ception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Goodman, S. N., & Royall, R. (1988). Evidence and scientific re- Performance, 5, 692-700. search. American Journal of Public Health, 78, 1568-1574. Bridgeman, B., Peery, S., & Anand, S. (1997). Interaction of cogni- Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. (1998). The effect of pictorial illu- tive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception & Psychophysics, sion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 59, 456-469. 10, 122-136. Castiello, U., Bennett, K., & Stelmach, G. (1993). Reach to grasp: Haggard,P.,& Wing, A. (1990). Assessing and reporting the accuracy The natural response to perturbation of object size. Experimental of position measurements made with optical tracking systems. Jour- Brain Research, 94, 163-178. nal of Motor Behavior, 22, 315-321. Castiello, U., & Jeannerod,M. (1991). Measuring time to awareness. Haggard, P., & Wing, A. (1997). On the hand transport component of NeuroReport, 2, 797-800. prehensile movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 282-287. Connolly, J. D., & Goodale, M. (1999). The role of visual feedback Jackson, G. M., Jackson, S., Husain, M., Harvey, M., Kramer, T., & of hand position in the control of manual prehension. Experimental Dow, L. (2000). The coordination of bimanual prehension movements Brain Research, 125, 281-286. in a centrally deafferented patient. Brain, 123, 380-393. Coren, S., & Girgus, J. (1978). Seeing is deceiving: The psychology of Jackson, S. R., & Shaw, A. (2000).The affects grip force visual illusions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. but not grip aperture scaling during prehension movements. Journal Coren, S., & Miller, J. (1974). Size-contrast as a function of figural of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, similarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 355-357. 418-423. Daprati, E., & Gentilucci, M. (1997). Grasping an illusion. Neu- Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale,M. (1991).Factors affecting higher-order ropsychologia, 35, 1577-1582. movement planning: A kinematic analysis of human prehension. Ex- Deiber, M.-P.,Ibanez,V.,Sadato, N., & Hallett, M. (1996).Cerebral perimental Brain Research, 86, 199-208. structures participating in motor preparation in humans: A positron Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of natural prehension movements. emission tomography study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 233-247. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 235-254. Dixon, P. (1998). Why scientists value p values. Psychonomic Bulletin Jeannerod, M. (1988). The neural and behavioural organization of & Review, 5, 390-396. goal-directed movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Dixon, P.,& O’Reilly,T. (1999).Scientific versus statistical inference. Jeannerod, M., Michel, F., & Prablanc, C. (1984). The control of Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 133-149. hand movements in a case of hemianaesthesia following a parietal le- Eklund, G. (1972). Position sense and state of contraction. Journal of sion. Brain, 107, 899-920. Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 35, 606-611. Keele, S. W.,& Posner, M. (1968). Processing of visual feedback in Elliot, D., Binsted, G., & Heath, M. (1999). The control of goal- rapid movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 155- directed movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory. Human 158. Movement Science, 18, 121-136. Khan, M. A., Franks, I., & Goodman, D. (1998). The effect of prac- Evarts,E.V.,& Vaughn,W. (1978).Intended arm movements in response tice on the control of rapid aiming movements: Evidence for an inter- to externally producedarm displacements in man. In J. E. Desmedt (Ed.), dependency between programming and feedback processing. Quar- Cerebral motor control in man: Long loop mechanisms (Progress in terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 425-444. Clinical Neurophysiology,pp. 178-192). Basel: Karger. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. (1995). Fundamentalsof human neuropsychol- Flash, T., & Henis, E. (1991). Arm trajectory modifications during ogy. New York: Freeman. reaching towards visual targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, Lee, R.G.,& Tatton, W. (1975). Motor responses to sudden limb dis- 220-230. placements in primates with specific CNS lesions and in human pa- Franz, V.H., Gegenfurtner,K., Bülthoff, H., & Fahle,M. (2000). tients with motor system disorders. Canadian Journal of Neurologi- Grasping visual illusions:No evidence for a dissociation between per- cal Science, 2, 285-293. ception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20-25. Marotta, J. J., DeSouza, J., Haffenden, A., & Goodale, M. (1998). 278 GLOVER AND DIXON

Does a monocularly-presented size-contrast illusion influence grip and visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of aperture? Neuropsychologia, 36, 491-497. prism adaptation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 26, 257-266. Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C., Jeannerod,M., Athenes, S., & Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1997). Prism adaptation during tar- Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. get pointingfrom visible and nonvisible starting locations. Journal of Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365-378. Motor Behavior, 29, 119-130. McCollough, C. (1965). Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the Sainburg,R.L., Poizner, H., & Ghez, C. (1993). Loss of propriocep- human . Science, 149, 1115-1116. tion produces deficits in interjointcoordination.Journal of Neurophys- Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R., Kornblum, S., Wright, C., & Smith, K. iology, 70, 2136-2147. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of Savelsbergh,G. J., Whiting, H.,& Bootsma, R. (1991).Grasping tau. rapid aimed movements. Psychological Review, 95, 340-370. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Perfor- Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. mance, 17, 315-322. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Smeets, J. B., & Brenner, E. (1995). Perception and action are based Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L., & Macko, K. (1983). Object vision on the same visual information: Distinction between position and ve- and spatial vision:Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neurosciences, 6, locity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Per- 414-417. formance, 21, 19-31. Otto-de Haart, E. G., Carey, D. P., & Milne, A. B. (1999). More Smeets, J. B., Erkelens, C., & van der Gon, J. (1990). Adjustments thoughts on perceiving and grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Neu- of fast goal-directed movements in response to an unexpected inertial ropsychologia, 37, 1437-1444. load. Experimental Brain Research, 81, 302-312. Paillard,J., & Brouchon, M. (1968). Active and passive movements Stark, L. (1968). Neurological control systems: Studies in bioengineer- in the calibration of position sense. In S. J. Freedman (Ed.), The neu- ing. New York: Plenum. ropsychology of spatially oriented behavior (pp. 37-55). Homewood, van Donkelaar, P. (1999). Pointing movements are affected by size- IL: Dorsey. contrast illusions. Experimental Brain Research, 125, 517-520. Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., MacKenzie, C., & Marteniuk, R. Westwood, D. A., Heath, M., & Roy, E. (2000). The effect of a picto- (1991). Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension rial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Experimental movements: II. The effects of changingobject size. Experimental Brain Brain Research, 130, 545-548. Research, 87, 407-420. Wing,A.M., & Fraser,C. (1983).The contributionof the thumb to reach- Paulignan, Y., MacKenzie, C., Marteniuk, R., & Jeannerod, M. ing movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, (1991). Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension 297-309. movements: I. The effects of changing object position. Experimental Wing, A.M., Turton,A., & Fraser,C. (1986). Grasp size and accuracy Brain Research, 83, 502-512. of approach in reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 245-260. Pavani, F., Boscagli, I., Benvenuti, F., Rabuffetti, M., & Farne, A. Wong, E., & Mack, A. (1981). Saccadic programming and perceived lo- (1999). Are perception and action affected differently by the Titch- cation. Acta Psychologica, 48, 123-131. ener circles illusion? Experimental Brain Research, 127, 95-101. Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psy- Prablanc, C., & Martin, O. (1992). Automatic control during hand chological Review Monographs, 3 (2), 1-114. reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. Jour- Zelaznik,H. N., Hawkins, B., & Kisselburgh,L. (1983).Rapid visual nal of Neurophysiology, 67, 455-469. feedback processing in single-aiming movements. Journal of Motor Pratt, J., & Abrams, R. (1996). Practice and component submove- Behavior, 15, 217-236. ments: The roles of programming and feedback in rapid aimed limb movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 396-426. (Manuscript received October 30, 2000; Redding, G. M., & Wallace,B. (1994). Effects of movement duration revision accepted for publication May 1, 2001.)