Variation in Bower Decorating Style Among Male Bowerbirds

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Variation in Bower Decorating Style Among Male Bowerbirds Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 83, pp. 3042-3046, May 1986 Population Biology Animal art: Variation in bower decorating style among male bowerbirds Amblyornis inornatus (cultural rnlsson/geographic variadon/behavior) JARED DIAMOND Physiology Department, University of California Medical School, Los Angeles, CA 90024 Contributed by Jared Diamond, December 30, 1985 ABSTRACT Courtship bowers of the bowerbird Ambly- BACKGROUND ornis inornatus, the most elaborately decorated structures Bowers are avenues, huts, or towers of sticks decorated with erected by an animal other than humans, vary geographically objects such as fruits, flowers, mushrooms, and stones. and individually. Bowers in the south Kumawa Mountains are Males often hold a decoration in their bill while displaying to tall towers of sticks glued together, resting on a circular mat of a female, and experiments confirm that bower structure and dead moss painted black, and decorated with dull objects such decorations influence females' choice of mate (7). Whereas as snall shells, acorns, and stones. Bowers in the Wandamen males are polygynous and contribute nothing to the female Mountains are low woven towers covered by a stick hut with an after copulation, females perform the whole effort ofbuilding entrance, resting on a green moss mat and decorated with a nest and rearing the young. Species comparisons show that colorful objects such as fruits, flowers, and butterfly wings. male bowerbirds with duller ornamental plumage build fan- Young males build simpler bowers, and adult males differ cier bowers (4, 8). Thus, during bowerbird evolution the among themselves. Experiments with poker chips of seven female's attention has been transferred from ornaments ofthe colors offered as decorations showed that individual birds male's body to those ofhis bower. A. inornatus is the species prefer some colors over others, individuals and populations with the dullest plumage (males and females identical, nearly differ in these preferences, certain objects are placed in specific uniformly brown) but with the largest and most elaborately parts ofthe bower, and birds steal chips from neighbors. Bower decorated bower. style may be partly learned. Hence, geographically varying My studies in the Kumawa, Wandamen, and Fakfak bower styles may be a culturally transmitted trait, like human Mountains were performed in February-March 1981 and art styles. August-October 1983. Bower sites in these uninhabited mountains lay at elevations of1050-2075 m and were reached Male bowerbirds of New Guinea and Australia build deco- by cutting trails inland for 5-20 days from the coast. rated structures termed bowers to woo females. The most complex bower-in fact, the most elaborately decorated NATURAL STRUCTURE OF BOWERS structure built by an animal other than Homo sapiens-is that Geographic Variation. South Kumawa Mountains. Bowers of the Vogelkop Gardener bowerbird Amblyornis inornatus. were spaced several hundred meters apart on ridge crests at This species is confined to five remote mountains of Indo- sites well lit from the east, thus enhancing visibility during nesian New Guinea that are hard to reach for political and early-morning hours of peak display activity. Each bower logistical reasons: the Arfak, Tamrau, Wandamen, Kumawa, incorporated one to five saplings ("maypoles"), about each and Fakfak Mountains (1). Thus, only a few briefaccounts of ofwhich was erected a tower 2-3 m high ofinterwoven sticks bowers in two of these mountains (Arfak and Tamrau) have 20-90 cm long. Sticks were glued together at contact points been published (2-5). with a white sticky substance (saliva?). Surrounding the This paper describes bowers of the Wandamen population sapling(s) was an almost perfectly circular mat of black dead and of the recently discovered Kumawa and Fakfak popu- moss up to 15 cm thick, with a diameter of 1.3-2.1 m and with lations. Previous studies had shown that bower styles differ the coefficient of variation of diameter in a given bower only among the 14 bower-building species of bowerbirds (4, 6). It 3%. (How a bird only 25 cm long lays out such a perfect circle now turns out that styles also differ conspicuously between is unknown.) At the base of each sapling was a cone of the populations of A. inornatus. Within a population there are same moss used for the mat. smaller differences between the styles of adults and im- All bowers of adult males had six types of decorations on matures and of different adults. To confirm that these or near the mat and segregated by type: pandanus leaves, each up to 2 m long and weighing up to 60 g (the bird weighs differences arise from intrinsic differences among birds 120 g), dragged from a considerable distance and propped rather than merely local differences in objects available for against a maypole at an angle of 30-45° or else laid on the decorating bowers, I tested the decorating preferences of ground; black elytrae ofbeetles; separate piles ofhundreds of individual birds with standardized test objects: poker chips of dark brown acorns, grey or brown snail shells, and dark seven different colors. Birds' choice and placement of chips brown stones; and a pile of black sticks from tree ferns and agreed with many features oftheir use ofnatural decorations. a substory tree. A whole bower and its decorations weighed These bower style differences among populations beg the several tens ofkilograms. The piled sticks, elytrae, and moss question as to how an individual bird acquires the style mat and cone were naturally brown but in the bower were characteristic of its population. I suggest that bower style is painted glossy black, apparently with an oily black substance not wholly innate but is partly learned, hence constitutes a in the bird's excrement. [Bower painting has been described culturally transmitted trait. previously for six species with avenue-style bowers (4, 6) but not for species with maypole-style bowers.] The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge Central Kumawa Mountains. At a site 8 km by air north of payment. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" the bowers just described, and within the same mountain in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact. range, Franz Sadsuitubun found 25 bowers similar to the 3042 Downloaded by guest on September 24, 2021 Population Biology: Diamond Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA 83 (1986) 3043 FIG. 1. (Upper) A Wandamen bower, showing hut, door, moss mat, red leaves and green fruit on the mat, and black bracket fungi on moss-covered root in foreground. (Lower) Bower owner on pile of black fungi facing door and hut, with red leaf, orange flowers, and red fruit outside hut, yellow fruits inside hut. (Photographs by J.D. and John Ratcliffe.) south Kumawa bowers, except for being also decorated with described by local New Guineans familiar with them, they are colored fruits (red, yellow, green, blue, white, or black). similar in structure and decorations to the south Kumawa Fakfak Mountains. Bowers were not seen by me, but, as bowers and lack colored fruits. Downloaded by guest on September 24, 2021 3044 Population Biology: Diamond Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 83 (1986) Wandamen Mountains. Wandamen bowers (Fig. 1) dif- bowers, except for two acorns at one bower. Conversely, fered drastically from south Kumawa bowers. There were colored fruits, flowers, fungi, and leaves like those used in the only one or two towers only 0.4-0.8 m high, of sticks woven Wandamen Mountains were available within a few meters of but not glued together. Covering the towers was a woven south Kumawa bowers but were ignored. White karst stones stick hut 0.9-2.2 m in diameter with an opening ("door") that were widespread in the south Kumawa Mountains but were faced downhill regardless of the compass direction. The mat used at only one bower, whereas only two Wandamen birds and cones were of live green moss, not of dead moss painted used the widely available orange bracket fungi. Thus, deco- black. The sole decoration shared with south Kumawa rations are not a random sample ofavailable objects: different bowers was beetle elytrae. A large pile ofblack bracket fungi populations and individuals select on the basis of different was placed downhill from the door. Decorations of many rules. colors lay on the mat or inside the hut, but details differed among bowers (see below): orange or black fungi of several EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF BOWER types; red leaves; orange bark; red, orange, yellow, green, blue, grey, brown, or black fruits; red, orange, yellow, or DECORATION purple flowers; and red, yellow, green, or black butterfly As tools to identify decorating rules, natural objects have the wings. disadvantages of nonuniformity and hard-to-quantify avail- Multiples of the same object (e.g., red flowers) were ability. As standardized test objects of known availability, I loosely grouped, as were objects of similar color (e.g., orange used numbered, glossy, circular poker chips of hard plastic, fruits with orange fungi, red flowers with red leaves). Certain 4 cm in diameter, 7 g in weight, and of seven colors: red, decorations besides black bracket fungi tended to be at orange, yellow, blue, purple, lavender, and white. Bower- specific locations-e.g., red leaves, black fruits, and orange birds' preferences may depend not just on hue but also on flowers solely outside the door; butterflies solely and blue saturation, lightness, and surface characteristics. In addition, fruits mainly inside the hut. When I shifted the position of a their perceptions of color will depend on the spectrum of decoration, the bower owner either restored it to the original illumination, the background, and details of their visual position or else discarded it in the forest.
Recommended publications
  • Nest, Egg, Incubation Behaviour and Parental Care in the Huon Bowerbird Amblyornis Germana
    Australian Field Ornithology 2019, 36, 18–23 http://dx.doi.org/10.20938/afo36018023 Nest, egg, incubation behaviour and parental care in the Huon Bowerbird Amblyornis germana Richard H. Donaghey1, 2*, Donna J. Belder3, Tony Baylis4 and Sue Gould5 1Environmental Futures Research Institute, Griffith University, Nathan 4111 QLD, Australia 280 Sawards Road, Myalla TAS 7325, Australia 3Fenner School of Environment and Society, The Australian National University, Canberra ACT 2601, Australia 4628 Utopia Road, Brooweena QLD 4621, Australia 5269 Burraneer Road, Coomba Park NSW 2428, Australia *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] Abstract. The Huon Bowerbird Amblyornis germana, recently elevated to species status, is endemic to montane forests on the Huon Peninsula, Papua New Guinea. The polygynous males in the Yopno Urawa Som Conservation Area build distinctive maypole bowers. We document for the first time the nest, egg, incubation behaviour, and parental care of this species. Three of the five nests found were built in tree-fern crowns. Nest structure and the single-egg clutch were similar to those of MacGregor’s Bowerbird A. macgregoriae. Only the female Huon Bowerbird incubated. Mean length of incubation sessions was 30.9 minutes and the number of sessions daily was 18. Diurnal incubation constancy over a 12-hour day was 74%, compared with a mean of ~70% in six other members of the bowerbird family. The downy nestling resembled that of MacGregor’s Bowerbird. Vocalisations of a female Huon Bowerbird at a nest with a nestling
    [Show full text]
  • Male Courtship Vocalizations As Cues for Mate Choice in the Satin Bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus Violaceus)
    MALE COURTSHIP VOCALIZATIONS AS CUES FOR MATE CHOICE IN THE SATIN BOWERBIRD (PTILONORHYNCHUS VIOLACEUS) CHRISTOPHER A. LOFFREDO AND GERALD BORGIA Departmentof Zoology,University of Maryland,College Park, Maryland 20742 USA AI3STRACT.--MaleSatin Bowerbirds(Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) court femalesat specialized structurescalled bowers. Courtship includes a complexpattern of vocalizationsin which a broad-band,mechanical-sounding song is followed by interspecificmimicry. We studiedthe effect of male courtshipdisplays on male mating successin Satin Bowerbirds.Data from 2 yearsof field researchshowed low between-maledifferences in mechanicalcomponents of courtshipsong and high variability betweenmales in mimeticsinging. Older malessang longer and higher-qualitybouts of mimicry than did youngermales. In one year, courtship song featureswere correlatedwith male mating success.The resultssuggest that female SatinBowerbirds use male courtship vocalizations in their mate-choicedecisions. We discuss hypothesesabout assessment of male age and dominancefrom courtshipvocalizations and suggestthat thesesongs have evolvedas a result of selectionfor male displaycharacteristics that provide femaleswith information about the relative quality of prospectivemates. Re- ceived27 June1985, accepted20 September1985. MALESatin Bowerbirds(Ptilonorhynchus vio- ingnessto copulateor by flying away. Given laceus)build specializedstructures called bow- the complexity of male display and the atten- ers that are used as sites for courting females tion femalespay to displayingmales,
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 18 Evolution of Reproductive Behavior (1St Lecture)
    Chapter 18 Evolution of reproductive behavior (1st lecture) The Satin Bowerbird has an unusual courtship ritual Male constructs an female elaborate “avenue” bower, contaiing colorful objects that he has collected. When a female arrives, he performs a energetic dance while emitting a medley of male buzzes, screeches and imitations of other bird songs. The female assesses the male based on: bower attributes, the male’s dance, and location of the bower If the female decides to mate with him, then she will enter his bower, copulate, and fly away, never to see him again. She will incubate her eggs and raise young on her own. The male will continue in this manner across the 8-month breeding season, mating with as many females as possible. The design of the bower varies greatly, both among and within species Playhouse bower Different populations of Amblyornis inoratus construct different types of bower, perhaps reflecting different “aesthetic tastes” of females within each population Maypole bower Evolutionary relationships among 14 of the 19 species of bowerbird, based on similarities in their mitochondrial cytochrome b gene Note that 2 bowerbird species share the ancestral trait of not building a bower Maypole builders Presumably, the hypothetical species Y built a Avenue simple bower. builders The ensuing adaptive radiation illustrates a fantastic example of divergent evolution What are the features of the bowerbird courtship ritual that represent evolutionary conundrums? The males make absolutely no parental investment Each male mates multiple
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Colorful Display
    COMMENTARY doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00051.x EVOLUTION OF COLORFUL DISPLAY Gerald Borgia,1 Brian Coyle, and P. B. Zwiers Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742-4415 1E-mail: [email protected] Received February 27, 2006 Accepted November 27, 2006 How the displays of bowerbirds have evolved has attracted widespread interest. Endler et al. (2005) analyzed color use in display in a subset of bowerbird species and generalized their results to all bowerbirds. Here we discuss problems with their analysis that calls into question their conclusions. For example, they state that bowerbirds do not use decorations that match their background, but this is not supported by their results. They reconstruct historical patterns of sexual dimorphism in plumage display using questionable methodology. The high lability of these display traits makes these reconstructions unreliable and, using accepted methods and acknowledging the lability problem, we were unable to support their conclusions. Their claim that plumage differences between sympatric species are due to character displacement is not supported by the available data. Their focus is on visual contrast as the cause for display color and we offer additional hypotheses that may contribute to explaining color use. We support studies of spectral analysis of display traits but urge greater care in using this information to reach conclusions about how colorful displays have evolved. KEY WORDS: Character displacement, character-state reconstruction, mate recognition, rapid evolution, sexual selection. Bowerbirds are widely known for their multifaceted sexual dis- logeny suggest the following sequence of events: (1) increase of plays including a bower, bower decorations, and dancing and vo- contrast under sexual selection; (2) onset of increased predation cal elements.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal-Fungal Interactions 1: Notes on Bowerbird's Use of Fungi
    Animal-Fungal Interactions 1: Notes on Bowerbird’s Use of Fungi Todd F. ElliottA* and Peter A. MarshallB A Integrative Studies Department, Warren Wilson College, P.O. Box 9000, Asheville, North Carolina 28815-9000, United States of America B Terra Preta Truffles, P.O. Box 94, Braidwood, New South Wales 2622, Australia * Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] Key words: Bowerbirds, bowers, ornithomycological, mushrooms, fungi, mycophagy Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/38/1/59/1474054/az_2015_032.pdf by guest on 29 September 2021 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7882/AZ.2015.032 Reports of ornithomycological interactions are scattered (Species Fungorum 2015). We found other reports dating across multiple bird families, including: Acanthizidae, back to the 1800s of fungi, black bracket fungi, mushrooms, Alaudidae, Casuariidae, Corvidae, Dromaiidae, and puffballs being used by Amblyornis inornata (Vogelkop Glareolidae, Megapodiidae, Petroicidae, Prunellidae, Bowerbirds), Chlamydera maculata (Spotted Bowerbird), Psittaculidae, Strigopidae, and Upupidae, (Simpson 1998; and Ptilonorhynchus violaceus (Satin Bowerbird), but Simpson 2000; Beever & Lebel 2014). These records unfortunately these reports are too taxonomically vague document fungi as food for birds ranging from Cassowaries to determine what taxa of fungi were involved (Beccari to Siberian Jays; in some cases, fungi may comprise a 1878; Nubling 1921; Marshall 1934, Diamond 1986; seasonally significant portion of birds’ diets (Andreev
    [Show full text]
  • Age-Related Effects of Testosterone, Plumage, and Experience on Aggression and Social Dominance in Juvenile Male Satin Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus Violaceus)
    The Auk 109(3):422-434, 1992 AGE-RELATED EFFECTS OF TESTOSTERONE, PLUMAGE, AND EXPERIENCE ON AGGRESSION AND SOCIAL DOMINANCE IN JUVENILE MALE SATIN BOWERBIRDS (PTILONORHYNCHUS VIOLACEUS) KEN COLLIS AND GERALD BORGIA Departmentof Zoology,University of Maryland,College Park, Maryland 20742, USA ASSTRACT.--Weinvestigated the relationshipbetween age and dominancein the Satin Bowerbird(Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) to understandwhy youngmales delay maturation. Tes- tosteroneimplants were used to experimentallyadvance the expression of adultcharacteristics in juvenile-plumagedmales. Testosterone implants increased both aggression and dominance amongjuvenile-plumaged males. In theyear following implantation, treated males molted intofull adultplumage and maintained their dominance over controls at feedingsites even thoughtheir implants were probably empty. Despite the changesin circulatingandrogens, aggression,and plumagebrought on by the testosteronetreatment of juvenile-plumaged males,age-related differences remained in aggressionand socialdominance between the treatedand untreatedbirds. These age effects are consistentwith the hypothesisthat male experiencein maleaggressive encounters plays an important role in determiningdominance relationshipsand, ultimately, affects the developmentof maleadult-plumaged characters. Received12 September1990, accepted 13 January1992. THEEVOLUTION of exaggeratedmale display still in juvenile plumage(Marshall 1954),they traitscommon in manypolygynous species has do not acquirea full-adult blue plumageuntil attracted
    [Show full text]
  • Australia Queensland Outback 21St July to 4Th August 2020 (15 Days) Cape York Peninsula Extension 3Rd to 12Th August 2020 (10 Days)
    Australia Queensland Outback 21st July to 4th August 2020 (15 days) Cape York Peninsula Extension 3rd to 12th August 2020 (10 days) Spinifex Pigeon by Jonathan Rossouw Our Queensland Outback birding adventure explores the remote and scenic corners of Western Queensland and the Gulf Country where many of Australia’s most desirable and seldom-seen endemics await us. RBL Australia – Queensland Outback & Cape York Ext Itinerary 2 This comprehensive tour targets all the region’s specialties in the unique and fascinating spinifex- dominated, stunted-Eucalyptus woodland, arid plains and savannah that covers much of this part of the continent. The birding is truly fantastic and we’ll be searching for a number of rare and little- known gems like Carpentarian and Kalkadoon Grasswrens, Masked, Long-tailed, Painted, Gouldian and Star Finches, Spinifex Pigeon, Spinifexbird, Grey and Black Falcons, Black-chested Buzzard, Red-backed Kingfisher, Ground Cuckooshrike, Purple-crowned Fairywren, Pictorella Mannikin, Rufous-crowned Emu-wren, Hall’s Babbler and Chestnut-breasted Quail-thrush to mention just a few of the possible highlights we hope to encounter on this specialist Australian birding tour. THE TOUR AT A GLANCE… THE QUEENSLAND OUTBACK ITINERARY Day 1 Arrival in Cairns Day 2 Cairns to Georgetown Day 3 Georgetown to Karumba Day 4 Karumba area Day 5 Karumba to Burketown Day 6 Burketown to Boodjamulla National Park Day 7 Boodjamulla National Park Day 8 Boodjamulla National Park to Mount Isa Days 9 & 10 Mount Isa area Day 11 Mount Isa to Winton Day
    [Show full text]
  • P0203-P0206.Pdf
    Condor, 81:203-206 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society 1979 THE EVOLUTIONARY RE-ESTABLISHMENT OF A LOST ANCESTRAL MUSCLE IN THE BOWERBIRD ASSEMBLAGE ROBERT J. RAIKOW STEPHEN R. BORECKY AND SUSAN L. BERMAN The loss of anatomical structures during the the New Zealand Thrush (Turnugra capensis; course of evolution is a common phenomenon, Turnagridae), the bellies of the IFE and M. and has elicited much discussion (see Regal iliotrochantericus caudalis are completely 1977 for a review of various theories of fused over most of their length and are causation). Many cases have been reported separate only at their insertions (Borecky in which lost ancestral structures have re- 1977 ) . appeared as developmental anomalies, but it We can offer three possible explanations is less often suggested that such ancestral for the presence of this muscle in the bower- characters can become re-established as nor- bird group. None of them can be either mal components of descendant groups, long totally refuted or verified, but we believe after their original loss. Such reversional that one hypothesis is by far the most likely phenomena have been attributed vaguely to for the reasons given below. These explana- the reversibility of mutations (e.g., Rensch tions follow. 1959:124), but advances in our understanding (1) Ancestral retention. It is possible that of how genes control development offer the IFE was never lost in the history of the insight into possible mechanisms for such bowerbird group, and that its presence is events. We report herein the apparent merely the retention of an ancestral character. re-establishment of a lost ancestral muscle This appears unlikely, however, because the in birds and speculate on the possible genetic muscle is lost in nearly all passerines, at least basis for it.
    [Show full text]
  • Male Satin Bowerbird Problem-Solving Ability Predicts Mating Success
    Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) 809–817 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Male satin bowerbird problem-solving ability predicts mating success Jason Keagy a,*, Jean-François Savard b,1, Gerald Borgia a,b,1 a Program in Behavior, Ecology, Evolution and Systematics, University of Maryland, College Park b Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park article info Mate choice and mate attraction are important behaviours influencing the evolution of elaborate traits. It Article history: is possible that male general cognitive performance plays an important role in sexual attractiveness, but Received 11 May 2009 there has been no direct test of this hypothesis. Satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus, are an Initial acceptance 22 June 2009 excellent species for testing this hypothesis because their complex male courtship, including use of Final acceptance 9 July 2009 decorations of certain colours, suggests a selective advantage to individuals with superior cognitive Published online 13 August 2009 abilities. We used males’ strong aversion to red objects on their bowers to design two unique problem- MS. number: A09-00305 solving tests. We presented males with these problems to test the hypothesis that males that are better problem-solvers have higher mating success. We confirmed this prediction and demonstrate that neither Keywords: age nor motivational level significantly influenced problem-solving scores. Our findings suggest that cognitive evolution general cognitive performance is related to male mating success. This is the first evidence that indi- cognitive performance viduals with better problem-solving abilities are more sexually attractive. female choice male reproductive success Ó 2009 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • Great and Spotted Bowerbirds Chlamydera Nucha/Is and C
    VOL. 16 (2) JUNE 1995 49 AUSTRALIAN BIRD WATCHER 1995, 16, 49-57 Great and Spotted Bowerbirds Chlamydera nucha/is and C. maculata (Ptilonorhynchidae) Sympatric and Interacting at Each Other's Bowers by CLIFFORD B. FRITH!, DAWN W. FRITH1 and MARNIE McCULLOUGH2 1'Prionodura', P.O. Box 581, Malanda, Queensland 4885 2 'Bruslee', P.O. Charters Towers, Queensland 4820 Summary An area of sympatry between bower-attending Great and Spotted Bowerbirds Ch/amydera nucha/is and C. maculata (Ptilonorhynchidae) in northern Queensland is described. Great Bowerbirds maintained bowers within little more than a kilometre of riverine vegetation and Spotted Bowerbirds mostly beyond this and in more arid habitat. The two species meet where the two favoured habitats abut and thus do so linearly either side of the major river system of the study area. The two closest pairs of bowers of each species were 1020 m and 1200 m apart. Spotted Bowerbirds visited bowers of, and displayed to, Great Bowerbirds, and the latter species visited bowers of the former. Photographic evidence is presented. Mean nearest-neighbour distance of 10 active Spotted Bowerbird bowers was 870 m (range 645-1170 m) and of nine Great Bowerbird bowers was 1370 m (range 825-1920 m). Introduction Records of sympatry between congeneric species of polygynous and bower-building bowerbird species (Ptilonorhynchidae) are few . The few records of sympatry between congeneric Australian bowerbird species involve only odd vagrant individuals within the peripheral range of another. In the case of polygynously breeding bowerbird species, sympatry of bower-attending congeneric males is unknown. Instances of sympatry are of interest as they may provide opportunities to examine the function of bower form, the influence of sympatry upon bower structures and upon the behaviour and reproduction of individuals and populations of bird taxa involved (Gilliard 1969, Frith 1970, Schodde & McKean 1973).
    [Show full text]
  • Educator's Guide
    EDUCATOR’S GUIDE GRADES 4−8 INTRODUCTION This exhibition takes you and your students on an expedition to the New Guinea rain forest to witness the spectacular plumage and dazzling dance moves of the family of birds known as birds-of-paradise. You will meet and follow the adventures of two determined researchers—National Geographic photographer Tim Laman and Cornell Lab of Ornithology researcher Edwin Scholes. These two explorers made 18 trips to New Guinea over eight years to complete the first survey of all 39 known species of birds-of-paradise. The video and photographs they brought back, combined with the exhibition artifacts and interactive experiences, provide exciting learning opportunities for you and your students. Photograph by Tim Laman Photograph by Tim MAJOR TOPICS Sexual Dimorphism: Physical differences—such as size, color, and shape—between males and females of the same species. Evolution by Sexual Selection: The process by which females or males select mates based on features they find attractive, resulting in more offspring with those features and— sometimes—the development of new species. Scientific Research: Methods used by scientists in the past and today to study birds-of- paradise, including expeditions to observe these birds in the wild. New Guinea Geography and Culture: How New Guinea’s geography affected the evolution of birds-of-paradise, and the role birds-of-paradise play in New Guinea’s culture. NATIONAL STANDARDS A trip to the Birds of Paradise exhibition supports the following standards. Science Science as Inquiry • Abilities needed for scientific inquiry Life Science • Characteristics of organisms • Diversity and adaptations of organisms History and Nature of Science • Science as a human endeavor • History of science Geography Places and Regions • Standard 4: The physical and human characteristics of places 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Vocal Mimicry in the Spotted Bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus Maculatus
    Vocal mimicry in the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus Laura A. Kelley A thesis submitted in application for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University of Edinburgh 2010 i Declaration The work contained within this thesis is my own and has not been done in collaboration, except where otherwise stated. The text does not exceed 70,000 words, and no part of this thesis has been submitted to any other university in application for a higher degree. Laura Ann Kelley ii Abstract Vocal mimicry is well documented in songbirds, yet the function of this behaviour is poorly understood. I studied vocal mimicry in a wild population of male spotted bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus maculatus to determine whether there was any support for the proposed functional hypotheses invoked to explain this behaviour. I collected observational data to determine what species male bowerbirds mimicked and how their mimetic repertoires related to the acoustic environment. Spotted bowerbirds preferentially mimicked the vocalisations of aggressive species, which is consistent with mimicry acting to deter predators or competitors (Batesian mimicry). However, these sounds were also relatively simple in terms of their structure, and may be mimicked purely due to their simplicity and similarity to the species-specific hiss. A survey of mimetic repertoires at three geographically isolated populations revealed a similar pattern in model choice: mimetic repertoires were predominantly composed of aggressive and predatory species but these sounds were also structurally simple. To test whether mimicry was used in a Batesian context I determined what contexts mimicry was produced in. Consistent with predictions, I found that males did not increase their mimetic rate in the presence of conspecifics but did increase their mimetic rate in response to human activity around the bower.
    [Show full text]