THELOOKSOFAWIER:

BEAUTY,GEDER,ADELECTORALSUCCESS

iclasBerggren PanuPoutvaara

TheRatioInstitute UniversityofandCEBR

HenrikJordahl

ResearchInstituteofIndustrialEconomics(IFN)

Abstract Westudytheroleofbeautyinpoliticsusingcandidatephotosthatfiguredprominentlyinelectoralcampaigns.Ourinvestiga tion is based on visual assessments of 1,929 Finnish political candidates from 10,011 respondents (of which 3,708 were

Finnish).Ashasaproportionalelectoralsystemwitharelativelylargenumberoffemalecandidates,weareableto perform a systematic study of gender differences, and to compare the electoral success of nonincumbent candidates representingthesameparty.Anincreaseinbeautybyonestandarddeviationisassociatedwithanincreaseof17–20%inthe number of votes for the average nonincumbent candidate. The relationship is virtually always statistically significant for femalecandidates,andinmostspecificationsalsoformalecandidates.Wheninterpretingourresults,wealsoevaluatealter nativeexplanationsofwhybeautymatters.

______

Acknowledgments: TheauthorswishtothankBryanCaplan,MikaelElinder,JustinaFischer,DanielHamermesh,Daniel

Klein,ClausThustrupKreiner,MarkkuLanne,MikaelPriksandRoopeUusitalo;participantsatthe2006IIPFconference, the2007AnnualMeetingofFinnishEconomists,the2007WorldMeetingofthePublicChoiceSociety,the2007CESifo

PublicSectorAreaConferenceandthe22 nd congressoftheEuropeanEconomicAssociation;andparticipantsatseminarsat

Bocconi University, Copenhagen Business School,ETH Zürich,George Mason University, HUI Stockholm, KU Leuven,

LundUniversity,theRatioInstitute,StockholmUniversity,TUDresden,UmeåUniversity,UniversityofGävle,University ofHelsinkiandUppsalaUniversityforhelpfulcommentsandsuggestions,allrespondentsandthosewhohelpedusattract them(especiallyourcolleagues,whoadvertisedthestudytotheirstudentsinseveralcountries,andseveralbloggers),aswell as Otto Kässi for excellent research assistance, Karl Bengtsson for outstanding technical assistance, and the Torsten and

RagnarSöderbergFoundations(BerggrenandJordahl),theJanWallanderandTomHedeliusFoundation(Jordahl)andthe

YrjöJahnssonFoundation(Poutvaara)forfinancialsupport.

Email addresses: Berggren: [email protected]; Jordahl: [email protected]; Poutvaara: [email protected] 1.Introduction

Aregoodlooksanadvantageinpolitics?Forseveralreasonstheycouldbe.Ifgoodlooking peoplearemorepersuasive,aretreatedbetterinsocialinteractionandachievehigheroccupa tionalsuccess–asevidencedinametastudybyLangloisetal.(2000)–theymightdobetter also in politics. In a recent contribution, Mobius and Rosenblat (2006) find that beautiful peoplereceivehigherwagesinanexperimentallabormarket,eventhoughperformancewas notcorrelatedwithbeauty.Partofthedifferenceisexplainedbyhigherconfidenceandbetter communicationskillsbygoodlookingpeople,while partofthe explanationrelatestoem ployersperceivinggoodlookingpeopleasmoreskilledthantheyreallyare.

In the vocabulary of Ambady and Rosenthal (1992), good looks could function as a

“thinslice”ofinformationorasaheuristicindecisionmaking.AlreadyDowns(1957)pro posedthatmanyvotersareratheruninformedaboutthedetailsofpolitics,aviewthatisfur thersupportedbyBartels(1996).Aconsequenceofthiscouldbethatvotersfocusonpersonal characteristicsofthecandidatesratherthanonpoliticalprograms,asWattenberg(1991)ar guesisthecase.Orforthatmatter,peoplemightjustprefertolookatbeautifulpeopleassug gestedbytheimportanceoflooksintheentertainmentindustry.Againstthisbackground,we investigateifvisualassessmentsofpoliticalcandidatescanexplainelectionoutcomes.

Ourmainresultisthatbeautyhelps.Wefindthatanincreaseinbeautybyonestandard deviationisassociatedwitha17to20percentincreaseinthenumberofvotesfortheaver age nonincumbent. Beauty is more strongly correlated with success than either perceived competenceortrustworthiness.Ourempiricalanalysisalsosuggeststhatbeautymattersmore forfemalecandidates. 1

Ourstudyisbasedonfourwebsurveyswithover1,900facialphotosofFinnishpoliti calcandidates.Altogether,wecollectedassessmentsfrom10,011respondents.About2,800 nonFinnishandabout3,700Finnishrespondentsweretoldthatthepersonsinphotosarepo liticalcandidates.About3,500respondentsfromoutsideofFinlandwerenottoldanything

1Throughoutthepaper,weusetheterms“men”and“women”todenoterespondents,i.e.thosewhoparticipatedinourstudy byevaluatingpoliticalcandidates,and“male”andfemale”todenotepoliticalcandidates.

2 aboutthepersonsinphotos.Inthesethreesurveyswithalargenumberofrespondents,each respondentwasshownarandomselectionofphotosandwasaskedtoassessthecandidates’ beauty,aswellasperceivedcompetence,trustworthiness,likabilityandintelligence.Inthe fourthsurveywith16respondents,eachrespondentassessed all 504photosofcandidatesin theHelsinkimunicipal election. For eachsurvey, we investigate to what extent the candi dates’individualbeautyscores(relativetotheaveragebeautyofcompetingcandidates)are associatedwiththeirelectoralsuccessinthe2003parliamentaryor2004municipalelections.

Our main contributionscan be summarized in three points. First, we are the first to study the effects of facial appearance on the success of political candidates who compete againstothercandidatesfromthesameparty.Second,weareabletofocusoncompetition betweenalargenumberofnonincumbentcandidates−aboutmostofwhomvoterscanbe expectedtohavelittleornoinformationotherthanparty,occupation,educationandvisual cues. Both of these advances are made possible by the proportional electoral system in

Finland. 2Andwhilewefocusoncompetitionbetweennonincumbentcandidates,wecanalso analyzeincumbencyeffects.Finnishvoterswhoareunsatisfiedwithanincumbentcanvote forachallengerfromthesameparty.Ourthirdcontributionconsistsofasystematicinvestiga tionoftheroleofgender.Ifbeautymattersforelectoralsuccess,thenanimportantquestionis ifitconfersdifferentialadvantagesonmaleandfemalecandidates.Finnishelectionsareun usuallysuitedforgenderanalysis,sincethereisasizablenumberofbothmaleandfemale candidatesinalldistricts.Wealsoinvestigatewhethermenandwomendifferintheirassess mentofcandidates’beautyandothertraits.

Amajorbenefitoffocusingonwithinpartycompetitionisthatweavoidproblemsof reverse causality which may plague studies based on betweenparty competition in one memberdistricts.Politicalpartiesaremorelikelytoattractmorepopular(e.g.morebeautiful) candidatesindistrictsinwhichtheyhaveanelectoral advantage.Thisproblemcould con foundtheintriguingfindingbyTodorovetal.(2005),thatquickphotoassessmentsofcompe tencehelppredicttheoutcomesofelectionstotheU.S.Congress.Unlikestudiesofbetween

2AccordingtoReynolds,Reilly,andEllis(2005),thereareproportionalelectoralsystemswithpartylistsin68countries includingFinland.

3 partycompetition,weareabletoconstructourelectoralsuccessvariableinsuchaway–ba sicallyasthevoteshareonalistfeaturingcompetitionagainstcandidatesfromthesameparty

–thattherelationshipbetweenexpectedelectoraloutcomesofvariouspartiesandcandidate selectionisunlikelytoinfluencetheresults.

Instudyingwithinratherthanbetweenpartycompetition,wealsoautomaticallycon trolfortheeffectofideologyonvoterchoice,ascandidatesofthesamepartyinFinlandare ideologicallyquitehomogeneous,unlikecandidatesofdifferentparties.IntheFinnishelec tionstudyfromthe2003parliamentaryelection,mostvoterssaidthatpoliticalopinionsand partywerecrucialfortheirchoiceofcandidate.Evenso,personalappearanceandstylewas importantforonethirdofthevoters.Asmorethanhalfofthevotersconsideredacandidate’s politicalexperienceimportantandasmorethanathirdvaluedacandidate’seducation,most votersclearlyalsothinkthatacandidate’spersonalcharacteristicsandexpectedcompetence, andnotjustparty,matter(seeBengtssonandGrönlund2005).

StudyingwithinpartycompetitioninFinlandoffersinterestinginsightsalsoforcoun trieswithonememberelectoraldistricts,liketheUnitedStates.Mostobviously,partyprima riesareanimportantstageinAmericanfederalandstatelevelelections.Ourstudyprovides reliableestimatesontherelativeimportanceofseveralaspectsofcandidateappearanceatthis stage of the electoral process. As ideological considerations are more important in general elections,ourresultsarguablygiveanupperboundfortheeffectsofvariousaspectsofcandi dateappearanceinbetweenpartycompetition.However,thereisnoreasontoexpecttherela tiveimportanceofvariousaspectsofpersonalappearancetodifferbetweenwithinpartyand betweenpartycompetition.Withorwithoutideologicalcompetition,votersprefercompetent, trustworthyandlikablepoliticians.3

Wealsothinkthatsomeaspectsofourresearchdesignformacontribution.Byhaving respondentsfromFinlandandfrommanyothercountriesandbystudyingtheirassessments separately,weareabletosaythattheresultsholdirrespectiveofthenationalityoftherespon

3 Forexample,Besley(2004),CaselliandMorelli(2004),MessnerandPolborn(2004),andPoutvaaraandTakalo(2007) studyelectoralcompetitionbetweencandidateswhodifferintheircompetenceorhonesty;twononideologicaltraitsthat voterscareabout.

4 dents(who,inthecaseofFinns,mayrecognizethecandidates).Oursurveywhere16respon dentsassessedallphotosofcandidatesfortheHelsinkimunicipalelectionshowsthatsurveys withasmallnumberofrespondentsmayproduceunstableresultswhentherespondentscome fromthesamecountryasthepersonswhoseappearancetheyassess.Inaddition,oursurvey whererespondentswerenottoldthatthephotosdepictpoliticalcandidatesprovidesinforma tionaboutwhetherknowingthisaffectstheassessment.

Importantly,weusephotosthatthepoliticalpartiesdisplayedontheircampaignpost ers.Finnishmunicipalitiesareobligedtoprovideeachpoliticalpartywiththesamenumberof standsonwhichthepartiescanpresenttheirposters.Allpartiesmakeextensiveuseofposters thatdisplaythenamesandphotosofallcandidatesinthedistrict.Asallpartieshavealarge numberofsuchpostersoutdoorsduringtheelectoralcampaignandasthesamephotosare alsodisplayedinnewspaperads,itislikelythatalargemajorityofvotershaveseenmostor allofthecandidatephotosfromthepartiesthattheyconsidervotingfor.Inordertoguarantee auniformstyleontheelectoralposters,itiscommonpracticeforFinnishpartiestouseone photographerwhotakesphotosofallcandidatesonagivenlist.Usingofficialcandidatepho tosfromelectoralcampaigns,ratherthanphotosfromthepressorphotossuppliedbycandi dates individually, reduces empirical problems of reverse causality, omitted variables and measurementerrorsubstantially.

A tentative interpretation of our findings is that beauty helps either because good lookingpeoplearemoresuccessfulinsocialinteraction,orbecausevotersliketowatchgood lookingpoliticians.Theinterpretationisbasedonthreepiecesofevidence.First,assessments offourothertraits(amongthemcompetenceandintelligence)allowsustodemonstratethat beautyhasanindependenteffect,ratherthanworkingasasignalthroughothertraitassess ments.Second,thecandidates’educationandoccupation,asreportedonelectorallists,also serveassignalsofcompetence,andbyincludingthisinformationwedemonstrateinanother waythatbeautyhasaneffectthatisindependentofitssignalingcompetence.Third,thefact thatbeautyappearstobeasimportantforincumbentsasforchallengersalsosupportsthein terpretationthatsocialinteractionorthepleasureoflookingatbeautifulpeopleexplainwhy betterlookingcandidatesaremoresuccessful.

5 Inordertoseehowreliableourfindingsare,weconductanextensivesensitivityanaly sisalongseveraldimensions.Thesensitivityanalysisconfirmsthebasicresultthatbeautyis positivelyrelatedtoelectoralsuccess,andmoresoforfemalecandidates.

2.TheLiterature

Alargebodyofresearchhasestablishedthatitisgoodtobebeautiful.Inametaanalysisof

102studies,Langloisetal.(2000)reportthatthelooksofpeopleinfluencehowtheyareper ceivedandtreatedbyothers,evenbythosewhoknowthem. 4Furthermore,goodlookscould serveasasignalofbetterhealth.Jackson,Hunter,andHodge(1995)andKanazawaandKo var(2004)arguethattheycouldalsobeasignalofhigherintelligence.Asforgender,Lan gloisetal.(2000,p.399)say:

Themetaanalysesshowedthat,bothwithinandacrosscultures,peopleagreedaboutwhoisandisnotattractive.Fur

thermore,attractivenessisanadvantageinavarietyofimportant,reallifesituations.Wefoundnotasinglegender

differenceandsurprisinglyfewagedifferences,suggestingthatattractivenessisasimportantformalesasforfemales

andforchildrenasforadults. Economic research has demonstrated similar substantial benefits in the labor market.

Beautifulpeoplereceivehigherwages,abeautypremium.AccordingtoHamermeshandBid dle’s(1994)seminalstudy,workersofaboveaveragebeautyearnabout10to15%morethan workers of belowaverage beauty. Other studies obtain similar results: see e.g. Biddle and

Hamermesh (1998), Harper (2000), Pfann et al. (2000), Hamermesh, Meng, and Zhang

(2002),French(2002),andMocanandTekin(2006).Asforgender,HamermeshandBiddle

(1994,p.1187)concludethatthereisan“absenceofsignificantlylargerpenaltiesandpremia, especiallythelatter,forwomenthanformen.”

Inarecentcontribution,MobiusandRosenblat(2006)findabeautypremiuminanex perimentallabormarketinvolvingamazesolvingtask,despitethefactthatperformancewas

4Cf.Eaglyetal.(1991)andFeingold(1992a).

6 uncorrelatedwithbeauty.MobiusandRosenblatdecomposethebeautypremiumintothree parts.First,goodlookingworkersaremoreconfidentandhigherconfidenceincreaseswages.

Second,foragivenlevelofconfidence,goodlookingworkersarewronglyconsideredmore ablebyemployers.Third,goodlookingworkersbenefitfromhavingbettercommunication andsocialskills.Ascommunication,socialskillsandconfidenceareallimportantinpolitics, onecouldexpectasubstantialbeautypremiuminelections.

Theroleofbeautyinpoliticshasattractedacademicinterestonlyrecently.Hamermesh

(2006)looksatelectionstothehighofficesoftheAmericanEconomicAssociation,andhis resultsindicatethatthereisalargeandalmoststatisticallysignificanteffectofbeautyonthe electoralsuccessofmalecandidates;butthatthereisvirtuallynoeffectforfemalecandidates.

Rosar, Klein, and Beckers (forthcoming) study the role of attractiveness evaluations in a

German state election in onemember districts. Their results indicate that an increase in beautycanincreaseacandidate’svotesharebyatmost4percentagepoints.KingandLeigh

(2007)studybeautyinAustralianelectionsandreportthatitmatters:aonestandarddeviation increase in beauty raises a candidate’s vote share by 0.7–1.8 percentage points. (Note that thesenumberscannotreadilybecomparedwithourfindingssincethesettingsofelectoral competition are different and since we define electoral success relative to list size.) While

Rosar,Klein,andBeckers(forthcoming)andKingandLeigh(2007)studycompetitionbe tweencandidatesrepresentingdifferentpartiesinonememberdistricts,wefocusonwithin party competition in a proportional electoral system with several candidates being elected fromeachdistrict.Furthermore,asweaskourrespondentstoassessthecompetence,intelli gence,likabilityandtrustworthinessofthecandidatesweareabletotestwhetherbeautyhas anindependentroleofitsown,inadditiontoitspotentialroleasasignalforothertraits.

Ourworkisalsorelatedtovotingresearchontheroleofheuristics,informationshort cuts,stereotyping,andthinslicesofinformation.Downs(1957)stressestheuncertaintyof voterdecisionmakingandregardspartiesandideologiesasdevicesusedtoattractvoterswho arenotallthatfamiliarwithdetailedpolicies.LauandRedlawsk(2001)findthatvoterslow inpoliticalsophisticationusecandidateappearanceasaheuristic.5Amongmorerecentstud

5Cf.BudesheimandDePaola(1994,p.339)andRedlawskandLau(2003).

7 ies,Todorovetal.(2005)findthatinferencesof competencefromphotoshelppredictthe outcomesofelectionstotheU.S.Congress(71.6%ofSenateracesand66.8%ofHouserac es).BenjaminandShapiro(2006)reportthatabout20%ofthevariationoftheactualvote sharesinU.S.gubernatorialelectionscanbeexplainedbypredictionsbasedonvideoclips.

Whiletheseauthorsanalyzephotosorvideoclipsastheonlythinsliceofinformation,we alsostudyoccupationandeducation,asreportedonelectorallists.Themetastudyby Am badyandRosenthal(1992)furtherconfirmsthatpeople–whetherrightorwrong–oftenform assessmentsandactonthebasisofthinslicesofinformation. 6

3.InstitutionalFacts,Surveys,andData

3.1. InstitutionalFacts

ThepoliticalsettingforthisstudyisFinland,anditselectoralsystemisproportional. 7Finland has a onechamber legislature, and the country is divided into fourteen mainland districts electingintotal199legislatorsandtheautonomousprovinceofÅlandelectingone.Elections areheldeveryfouryears.ThenumberofMPselectedfromthe14mainlanddistrictsvaries betweensevenand32.

In eachparliamentary district,partiespresent listsoftheircandidates,typicallyinal phabeticalorderbutsometimeswithincumbentslistedfirst,andeachvoterchoosesonecan didateononelist.Thenumberofcandidatesthatapartycanpresentequalsthenumberof representativeselectedfromthedistrict,ifthisis14ormore.Insmalldistrictswithlessthan

14seats,partiescanpresent14candidates.Thelegislatureseatsofagivendistrictareallo catedbasedonpartyvotesharestothecandidatesinaccordancewiththeir“competitivein

6 This conclusion is supported further by Kahneman, Slovic, and Tversky (1982), Simon (1985), Lupia (1994), Macrae,

Milne, and Bodenhausen (1994), Bartels (1996), Caprara, Barbaranelli, and Zimbardo (1997), and Willis and Todorov

(2006).

7SeeRaunio(2005).

8 dices”.Ineachparty,thecandidatewiththehighestnumberofvotesreceivesashisorher competitiveindexthetotalnumberofvotesobtainedbyhisorherparty,thecandidatewith thesecondhighestnumberofvotesobtainsanindexcalculatedashalfofthepartyvotes,the thirdcandidategetsanindexequaltoathirdofthepartyvotes,etc.Thenallcandidatesare rankedonthebasisoftheirindices,andfromthislist,therewillbeelectedasmanycandi datesasthereareseatsintheelectoraldistrict.Inthemunicipalelections,competitiveindices arecalculatedinasimilarway, witheachmunicipality forming a district. The number of electedmunicipalcouncilorsdependsonmunicipalitysize,reachingamaximumof85inHel sinki.Inthemunicipalelectionseachpartyisallowedtopresentoneandahalfasmanycan didatesonitslistasthenumberofseatsinthemunicipalcouncil.Themaximumnumberof candidatesthateachpartycanpresentinHelsinkiisthus127.

In the 2003 parliamentary election, turnout was 69.7%. Female candidates received

42.6%ofallvotes,and75ofthe200electedmembersofparliamentwerewomen(Statistics

Finland,2006).

3.2. TheSurveys

Inorderforbeautytobeameaningfulvariableforsocialscientiststostudy,perceptionsofit needtobequantifiedaswellasreflectsomewhatofastableconsensus.Langloisetal.(2000) infactfindthatthereisconsiderableagreementaboutwhoisandwhoisnotattractive,both withinandacrosscultures.AsHamermeshandBiddle(1994,p.1175)putit:“withinaculture atapointintimethereistremendousagreementonstandardsofbeauty,andthesestandards changequiteslowly.” 8Onthisbasis,wehaveconductedfourwebsurveysbasedonthesame questionnaire, but with some modifications in each treatment. In addition to asking about beauty,wealsoincludedquestionsaboutfourothertraitsinordertofindoutmoreprecisely

8Thesamepointismadebye.g.Feingold(1992b),Cunninghametal.(1995),andAharonetal.(2001).

9 what determines electoral success and how the results are to be interpreted. 9 By collecting responsesfromseveralcountrieswearealsoabletocheckforcrossculturaldifferences.We find,inourmainsurveywithnonFinnishrespondents,thatrespondentsindifferentcountries makeverysimilarassessmentsofthesamephotos(withtheFrenchpossiblyfindingcandi datesalittlelessbeautifulthanAmericans,Swedes,Germans,Danesandothers).

ThefoursurveysaredescribedbrieflyinTable1.

TABLE1. Thefoursurveys.

Nameofsurvey Nationalityof Informationtore Selectionof Numberof Numberof Timewhen

respondents spondentsthatthe photos respondents responses carriedout

photosdepictpolitical shownto

candidates respondents

Survey1:The NonFinnish Yes Random 2,772 16,218 Springsummer mainsurvey (fourper 2006

round)

Survey2:The Finnish Yes Random 3,698 26,477 Fall2006 surveyofFinns (fourper

round)

Survey3:The Swedishand Yes All(504per 16 8,064 Winter2007 smallsurvey Finnish round)

Survey4:The NonFinnish No Random(ten 3,525 38,985 Autumnwinter noinformation perround) 2005/2006 survey

Notes:Inthecolumnswiththenumberofrespondentsandresponses,onlyrespondentswhoassessedatleastfourphotos(andtheirre sponses)arereported.

Ourmainsurvey,survey1,wasconductedinthespringandsummerof2006outsideof

Finland.ThemainreasonforusingnonFinnishrespondentsisthattheycanbeexpectednot torecognizeanyofthecandidates,whichisanadvantagewhenanalyzingwhethervisualim agesfunctionasthinslicesofinformation.Withthehelpofdozensofcolleagues,studentsin variousuniversitieswereinvitedtoparticipate,either in lectures or by email. The biggest

9Wedonotclaimthattheassessmentsrepresenttruecharacteristicsofthepoliticalcandidates.Thisstudyisaboutpercep tionsandnoneoftherelationshipsreportedshouldbeinterpretedasclaimsofarelationshipamonganyunderlyingtruecha racteristics.

10 participantnumbers,morethan100fromeach,camefromSciencesPoinFranceandUppsala

Universityin.Toattractalsononstudents,invitationstoparticipateinourstudywere senttoUppsalaUniversityalumniaswellastomembers of two professional associations

(InternationalInstituteofPublicFinanceandEuropeanPublicChoiceSociety).Wealsocoo peratedwithseveralblogsthatadvertisedourstudy.Ourdatacollectionmethodallowsusto studyseparatelytraditionalstudentrespondentsandrespondentsrecruitedinotherways.The respondentshadtheoptiontoparticipateinalotteryof100eurosandcouldalsoorderafuture summaryoftheresults.

Afterreplyingtosomepersonalbackgroundquestions,eachrespondentwasshownfour photos,oneatatime,randomlychosenfromthedatabase of photos, in total two of each gender.Inconnectionwitheachphoto,severalquestionswereasked(seeBoxA1intheAp pendixforfurtherdetails).Therewasanoption,afterhavingassessedfourphotos,toassess additionalroundsoffourphotos,thistimewithachoiceastowhethertoassessonlyfemales, onlymalesoracontinuedmixture.Therewasnotimelimitforlookingatthephotos. 10 The sizeofthephotoswasapproximately5x3.5centimeters(2x1.4inches),andtheydepicted facesonly.Nootherinformationthanthephotowasgivenaboutanycandidate.Thecandi datescomefromfourpartieswith63%oftheelectedmembersofparliamentinthe2003elec tion:theSocialDemocraticParty,theNationalCoalitionParty(acenterrightparty),theLeft

AllianceandtheGreenLeague.

Finnishpoliticalpartiesadvertisetheircandidatesonposterswithindividualphotosof allcandidatesinadistrict.Sincetheparticipatingpoliticalpartiesprovideduswiththesepho tos,ourrespondentsassessedthesamephotosasthevoterswereexposedto.Therearetwo potentialproblemsrelatedtotheuseofcandidatephotos−inthisandinotherstudiesonthe roleofcandidateappearanceinpolitics.Thefirstoneisreversecausality:successfulpoliti cianscouldhaveaccesstostylistsandbetterphotographers.Thesecondoneisomittedva

10 Presumably,respondentshaveuseddifferentperiodsoftimewhenlookingatthephotos,butthisneednotbeaproblem.

Ambady and Rosenthal (1992) document that studies using longer periods of observation do not yield greater predictive accuracy,somethingwhichseemstohold,notleast,withregardtofaces(cf.Todorovetal.2005,pp.1623–1624,andWillis andTodorov2006).

11 riables,ifsomepoliticiansboth“dressforsuccess”anddootherunobservedthings,likevisit largenumbersofvoters,whichhelpthemgettingelected.However,weexpectbothproblems tobesmallerwhenusingofficialcandidatephotos.Ourinvestigationdoesnotsufferfromthe problemthatmoresuccessfulorbetterfinancedcandidateshirebetterphotographers:official candidatephotostakenbythesamephotographeroffersamoreequalplayingground.Moreo ver, a“badhairday” wouldproducemeasurementerrorforacandidateifphotosfromthe presswereused,whereaswithofficialcandidatephotos,oneexpectsanunflatteringpicture exposedinnumerousposterstobedetrimentalforelectoralsuccess.Inanycase,Hamermesh,

Meng,andZhang(2002)findthatattemptstoimproveone’slooks,intherealmofclothing andcosmetics,onlyhaveasmallimpactonhowbeautifuloneisperceivedbyothers.

Survey2,thesurveyofFinns,wascarriedoutinthefallof2006inFinland.Thistime, weattractedmainlystudentparticipants.Thissurveyallowsustoinvestigatehowrecognition ofcandidatesaffectsassessmentsandtoverifythatassessmentsbyFinnishrespondentsare broadlyinlinewithpatternsofnonFinnishrespondents. The biggest participant numbers, morethan300fromeach,camefromtheUniversityofJyväskylä,theUniversityofHelsinki, andtheUniversityofOulu.Respondentscouldparticipateinalotteryof30movietickets.

Survey3,thesmallsurvey,tookplaceinearly2007inFinlandandSwedenwith16 respondentsofvaryingageandgender.Thistime,eachrespondentassessedall504photosof candidatesintheHelsinkimunicipalelection.Themainreasonwastoseewhetherthiswayof assessingcandidates–usedinlabormarketstudies–yieldssimilarresultsasourlargescale surveyswhereeachoneofalargenumberofrespondentsassessesasmallnumberofrandom lyselectedphotos.

Survey 4, the noinformation survey, was conducted in the autumnwinter of

2005/2006.RespondentsfromoutsideofFinlandwereshownphotoswithoutanyinformation onthepersonsappearing.Thisallowsustotestwhetherassessmentsofbeautyandothertraits wereaffectedbyustellingthatthepersonsinphotosarepoliticalcandidates.

WefocusourinvestigationonthemainsurveywithnonFinnishrespondentswhoknew thattheywereassessingpoliticalcandidates,anddiscussresultsfromthethreeothersurveys inSection7.

12 3.3.Data

Ourdatabasecontains1,929photosofFinnishpoliticalcandidates–1,009ofmenand920of women,fromthemunicipal(57%)andparliamentarylevel(43%).Weonlyincludeassess mentsbyrespondentswhoassessedatleastfourphotos.Weonlyincludephotoswithatleast threeassessments.Thisgivesus1,786photos.InSection5,wedividethephotosintotwo groups–thoseofnonincumbents(1,555photos)andthoseofincumbents(231photos).By

“incumbents”ismeantpoliticalcandidateswhoservedintheofficeinquestion,orasmem bersofthenationalortheEuropeanparliamentsatthetimeoftheelection.Onaverage,each photowasassessedbyninerespondentsinthemainsurvey.

AsindicatedinTable2,AmericansandSwedesmakeupamajorityofour2,772res pondents.LargegroupsofrespondentsalsocomefromFrance,GermanyandDenmark.

TABLE 2. Respondentsbycountry.

Country Number Percent

USA 859 31.0

Sweden 850 30.7

France 261 9.4

Germany 220 7.9

Denmark 156 5.6

Othercountry 426 15.4

Total 2,772 100

Notes:Respondentsdenotethosewhoassessedatleastfourphotos(onefullround).66%weremen,34%women.32%wereundergra duatestudents,and14%weregraduatestudents.Averageage:31(32formenand30forwomen).

Throughourfourwebsurveys,weusemorerespondentsthanotherstudiesofbeautyor competence: 6,303 from outside of Finland and 3,708 from Finland, compared to four

(Hamermesh2006),five(KingandLeigh2007),50(MobiusandRosenblat2006),264(Ben jamin and Shapiro 2006), 843 (Todorov et al. 2005), and 903 (Rosar, Klein, and Beckers forthcoming).11

11 Todorovetal.(2005)collectedassessmentsofbeautyfromonly34respondents.

13 4.PerceptionsofBeautyandOtherTraits

Eachphotowasassessedinthefivedimensionsbeauty,competence,trustworthiness,likabili ty, and intelligence using five reply options, which we have converted to a fivenumber scale. 12 Thelowestpossiblebeautyratingcorrespondsto1,andthehighestpossibleto5,etc.

Inassessingeachtrait,respondentshadanoptiontoabstain.Inourmainsurvey,theshareof thosewhoabstainedvariedbetween0.5%forbeautyand7.9%fortrustworthiness.Thereis substantialagreementamongrespondents;ifweconcentrateontwogroupsofbeautyassess ments─aboveaverage(4and5)andbelowaverage(1and2)─thekappacoefficientofin terrateragreementis0.48andhighlystatisticallysignificant.Thecorrespondingcoefficients fortheotherfourtraitsrangefrom0.18to0.23,allofthemstatisticallysignificantatthe1% level.

However,menandwomendidnotalwaysagreeontheirassessments(Table3).Thereis acleartendencyformen,onaverage,togivephotosoffemalecandidateslesspositiveas sessmentsthanwomendo.Therearesmallerdifferencesintheassessmentsofphotosofmale candidates;theonlystatisticallysignificantdifferenceisthatmenfindmalecandidatesmore handsomeorbeautifulcomparedtowhatwomenfind.

12 Usingacardinalscaleofthiskindisstandardfareintheliterature:seee.g.HamermeshandBiddle(1994).Asreported morefullyinsection6.1,wehavealsousedalternativevariablesbasedinordinalassessments:theshareofresponseswherea candidatewasevaluatedasthemostbeautiful,mostcompetentandmosttrustworthyamongfourphotos.

14

TABLE 3. Assessmentsoffivetraits.

Variable Menassessingmale Womenassessing Menassessingfemale Womenassessing

candidates malecandidates candidates femalecandidates

Averagebeauty 2.64 2.57 2.79 3.01

(0.90) (0.91) (1.06) (0.97) Averagecompetence 3.30 3.27 3.21 3.39

(0.88) (0.88) (0.84) (0.85)

Averagetrustworthiness 3.04 3.02 3.29 3.42

(0.86) (0.89) (0.82) (0.83)

Averagelikability 3.07 3.06 3.23 3.37

(0.92) (0.95) (0.93) (0.94)

Averageintelligence 3.38 3.35 3.23 3.37

(0.83) (0.82) (0.79) (0.79)

Notes:Standarddeviationsinparentheses.Thefiguresarefromourmainsurvey.

Onaverage,menperceivemalecandidatestobemoreintelligentandcompetentthanfemale candidates,andfemalecandidatestobemorebeautiful,likableandtrustworthy.Womengive femalecandidatesmorepositiveassessmentsofalltraits,eventhoughthedifferenceinthe assessmentofintelligenceissmallandnotstatisticallysignificant.Thereis,lastly,noindica tionofa“dumbblondesyndrome,”whichKingandLeigh(2007)suggestasaninterpretation oftheirresults.Thereisastrongpositiverelationship,bothforfemaleandformalecandi dates,betweenbeautyandperceivedcompetenceandbetweenbeautyandperceivedintelli gence.Thisholdsirrespectiveofthegenderoftherespondentsortheageofthecandidates.A general pattern is that assessments of any pair of traits are positively correlated with each other,butcorrelationsarefarfromperfect. 13

13 Forcorrelationcoefficients,seeTableA1intheAppendix.

15 5.BeautyandElectoralSuccess

5.1.TheEmpiricalSetting

Inthissectionweinvestigatetherelationshipbetween beauty and electoral success. Given thatassessmentsbyFinnishvoterscouldbeinfluencedbytheirknowledgeofthecandidates, thereisariskthatusingFinnishrespondentswouldcreatesystematicmeasurementerror.To avoid this, the results in this and the following section are based on assessments by non

Finnish respondents in our main survey. 14 We present results for other respondent groups, includingFinns,inSection7.

LikeHamermesh(2006),wefirstlookattheshareoftheelectedcandidateswhoreceive aboveaverageassessments.Inthecaseofbeauty,about62%oftheelectednonincumbent candidates were assessed as being above average on theirlist.Thisindicatesthatalthough beauty may be an asset in politics, it is by no means a necessary requirement for being elected.However,againwefindthatthereisa clear gender gap:whereasonly43%ofthe elected male candidates had a beauty rating above average, the corresponding number for femalecandidatesis74%.Comparedtoothernonincumbentcandidatesoftheirowngender,

57%ofelectedmalecandidatesand70%ofelectedfemalecandidateswerethoughttobeof aboveaveragebeautyontheirlist.Thisgendergapsuggeststhatitmaybefruitfultoanalyze theeffectsofbeautyforeachgenderseparately. 15

A more detailedpicture emerges if we look at averageassessmentsandalsotakethe genderoftherespondentsintoaccount.Bothmenandwomenassesselectedandnonelected malecandidatessimilarly.Onedifferenceisthatperceivedcompetenceisabithigheramong electedcomparedtononelectedmalecandidates.Forbeauty,theassessmentsofelectedand

14 NoneofthenonFinnishrespondentscorrectlyrecognizedanyoneofthecandidates.In17casestherespondentmistooka candidateforanotherpolitician.TarjaHalonenwastheonlyFinnishpoliticianthatanyone,incorrectly,claimedtorecognize.

Tenanswerswereofthekind“Irecognizeherbutdon’trememberhername.”

15 Wehavedonethisthroughoutthepaperbutingeneralonlyreportstatisticallysignificantgenderdifferences.

16 nonelectedmalecandidatesareveryclosetoeachother. 16 Forfemalecandidatesthepicture isquitedifferent.Bothmenandwomenfindelectedfemalecandidatesmoregoodlooking thannonelectedones.Otherdifferencesaresmaller,butnotassmallasformalecandidates.

Hereonecanmentionthatmenseemtogiveelectedfemalecandidates highercompetence assessmentsthantheygivetononelectedfemalecandidates. 17

Nextweinvestigatetowhatextentbeautyandothertraitscanberelatedtotherelative successofcandidatesinthe2003and2004elections.Unlikeotherstudieswefocusfirston thelargegroupofnonincumbentcandidates(definedaspoliticalcandidateswhowerenot electedtotheofficeinquestionandwhowerenotmembersofthenationalorEuropeanpar liamentsatthetimeoftheelection)andthenlookatthefullsetofcandidates,includingin cumbents.Onereasonformakingthisdivisionisthatincumbencyisaverystrongpredictor ofelectoralsuccess(seee.g.Lee,forthcoming),andifadummyvariablefailstocaptureallof itseffects,otherestimatesriskbeingbiased.Anotherreasonisthatappearanceandotherthin slicesofinformationmaybemoreimportantforlesswellknowncandidates. 18 Furthermore,

AndreoniandPetrie(forthcoming)reportthatindividualcontributionsinapublicgoodsgame arehigherinthepresenceofbeautifulplayersaslongastheindividualcontributionsareun known,butthatthisbeautypremiumturnsintoabeautypenaltyoncetheindividualcontribu tionsarerevealed.Thissuggeststhatbeautycouldbemoreimportantfornonincumbents.

Thetraitvariablesareconstructedintwosteps.Firstwecomputethemeanofallas sessmentsofaparticularphoto.Fromthismeasurewethensubtractthemeanassessmentfor eachtraitforthecandidatesonthesamelist.Thatis,weuse relative measuresofthedifferent traits,capturinghowbeautiful,competentandtrustworthyacandidateisperceivedtobein relationtohisorhercompetitorsonthelist.

16 SeeFigureA1intheAppendix.However,incumbentcandidatesareseenasslightlybetterlookingthannonincumbent candidates(anaverageof2.82vs.anaverageof2.73).

17 SeeFigureA2intheAppendix.

18 WeareabletostudynonincumbentsseparatelyasFinlandhasaproportionalelectoralsystemwithpersonalvotesdeter miningtheorderinwhichcandidatesareelected,resultinginwithinpartycompetition.Apluralityvotesystem,likethatof theUnitedStates,typicallyfeaturescompetitionbetweenanincumbentandachallengerfromdifferentparties.Benjaminand

Shapiro(2006),Rosar,Klein,andBeckers(forthcoming),andKingandLeigh(2007)useadummyforincumbency.

17 Thedependentvariable,relativesuccess,isdefinedinthefollowingwayforcandidate i onlist j:

relativesuccess i,j =( pi/v j)*100(1) where piiscandidate i’snumberofpersonalvotesand vjisallvotesforcandidatesonlist j dividedbythenumberofcandidatesonlist j.19 Whenstudyingnonincumbentsinsection5.2 wecalculateboththetraitmeasuresandrelativesuccessbasedonnonincumbentcandidates only.Insection5.3thesamemeasuresarecalculatedforincumbentandnonincumbentcan didatestogether.Eachcandidate’svotesharewouldbeasimplerandmoredirectchoiceof dependentvariable.Weusethatmeasureinthesensitivityanalysis,buttheadvantageofthe relativesuccessmeasureisthatitmakeselection outcomes comparable, as list sizes differ

(especiallybetweenparliamentaryandmunicipalelections).

Asregressors,weusethethreetraitvariablesbeauty,competenceandtrustworthiness.

Thesethreewereselectedtokeeptheanalysissimplebyfocusingondissimilartraits. 20 Inour preferredspecificationwealsoincludetheagedummiesyoung,whichdenotesanageunder

30, and old ,whichdenotesanageover60.Ourdatashowthatbothmenandwomenfind youngercandidatesmorebeautifulthanoldercandidates.

5.2.onIncumbentCandidates

Webeginbylookingattheeffectsintheparliamentaryelectionforfemaleandmalenon incumbentcandidates.Mostnotably,asreportedinTable4,wefindthatbeautyisclearlyour mostimportantexplanatoryvariableofrelativesuccessbothforfemaleandformalecandi dates,andtheonlyregressorthatconsistentlyattainsstatisticalsignificance.

19 Themeanofrelativesuccessis100,capturingthatonaverageeachcandidatemustreceiveashareofthevotesequalto1/ listsize.Theaverageofrelativesuccessforelectedcandidates(incumbentsandnonincumbents)is338.Thatis,theyreceive

3.38timesthevotesoftheaveragecandidate.

20 Beautyandlikabilityshowedahighcorrelationandintelligenceandcompetenceshowedahighcorrelation.Insection6.2 wedescriberesultsfromaspecificationthatincludesallfivetraits.

18

TABLE 4. Relativesuccessintheparliamentaryelection,nonincumbents.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Relativesuccess Relativesuccess Relativesuccess Relativesuccess Relativesuccess

allnonincumbents allnonincumbents allnonincumbents femalenon malenonincumbents

incumbents

Beauty 34.89*** 31.17*** 33.43*** 29.85***

(6.31) (6.55) (8.58) (11.25)

Competence 23.08*** 10.95 5.441 11.70

(8.34) (8.61) (15.6) (9.88)

Trustworthiness 9.94 6.07 15.27 1.61

(9.30) (8.89) (14.2) (12.3)

Malecandidate 3.77 0.05 4.72

(6.37) (6.77) (6.74)

Young(age<30) 18.93** 3.93 16.23* 18.47 17.15

(9.45) (9.54) (9.70) (12.4) (14.9)

Old(age>60) 11.59 0.74 8.19 28.21 48.26

(22.5) (21.8) (22.3) (20.3) (38.5)

Numberofcandidates 641 641 641 343 298

AdjustedRsquared 0.06 0.02 0.06 0.09 0.04

Notes:Robuststandarderrorsinparentheses.Theregressionsincludeaconstantterm.*significantat10%;**significantat5%;*** significantat1%.

Incolumn1,beautyistheonlyofthethreetraitsthatisincluded,anditisfoundtobe highlystatisticallysignificant.Thecoefficientof beauty becomes marginally smaller when competence and trustworthiness are included as well (in columns 3–5). When we exclude beautyincolumn2,thesizeoftheestimatedcoefficientforperceivedcompetenceissubstan tiallyhigherthanincolumns3–5andalsoattainsstatisticalsignificance.Thissuggeststhatas perceptionsofbeautyandcompetencearepositivelycorrelated,theclaiminTodorovetal.

(2005)thatvotingpreferencesareanchoredoninferencesofcompetencefromfacialappear ancemayneedtobereconsidered.

Thethreelastcolumnsincludeallthreetraits.Ahigherbeautyscoreofonestandardde viationimpliesanincreaseinthenumberofpersonalvotes,relativetotheaveragenumberof votesforthenonincumbentsonthelist,by20.3%forallcandidates,24.1%forfemalecandi

19 dates, and 16.4% for male candidates. 21 The gender difference is however not statistically significant(whichgenerallyholdstrueforregressionresultsbasedonthismaindataset).To facilitatetheinterpretationoftheestimatedimpactofbeauty,notethatanincreaseofoneunit inrelativesuccessmeansaonepercentagepointincreaseinthenumberofvotes,relativeto theaveragenumberofvotesofallcandidatesonthesamelist.Accordingly,anincreaseinthe beautyassessmentbyonestandarddeviationisassociatedwitha20percentincreaseinthe numberofvotesfortheaveragenonincumbent.Onecanalsonotethatbeingyoungmaybea disadvantage.

Table5revealsthatthepointestimateofbeautyisonlymarginallysmallerforthemu nicipalelections.Ahigherbeautyscoreofonestandarddeviationimpliesanincreaseinthe numberofpersonalvotes,relativetotheaveragenumberofvotesforthenonincumbentson thelist,by16.6%forallcandidates,21.4%forfemalecandidatesand19.4%formalecandi dates.Exceptamongmalecandidates,theestimatesforcompetencearestatisticallysignifi cantandlargerthanintheparliamentaryelection.

21 Thestandarddeviationis0.65forallcandidates,0.72forfemalecandidatesand0.55formalecandidates.

20 TABLE 5. Relativesuccessinthemunicipalelections,nonincumbents.

(1) (2) (3)

Relativesuccess Relativesuccess Relativesuccess

allnonincumbents femalenonincumbents malenonincumbents

Beauty 25.58*** 27.16** 19.44***

(6.74) (11.30) (6.03)

Competence 18.54** 33.27** 7.278

(8.15) (15.7) (7.99)

Trustworthiness 15.60* 14.20 15.01

(8.17) (12.4) (10.8)

Malecandidate 27.82***

(6.53) young(age<30) 22.82*** 26.58* 17.01**

(7.86) (13.5) (7.88) old(age>60) 3.50 20.76 11.69

(12.8) (14.3) (18.9)

Numberofcandidat es 914 460 454

AdjustedRsquared 0.05 0.04 0.02

Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses. The regressions include a constant term. * significant at 10%; ** significant at 5%; ***significantat1%.

5.3.AllCandidates(IncumbentsandonIncumbents)

Thepreviousliteraturehasfocusedonpluralityvotesystemsandhasnotstudiedcompetition betweennonincumbents.Wenowinvestigatewhattheeffectwouldbe,asshowninTable6, ofaddingincumbentsandanincumbencydummy. 22

22 Toeconomize,inthetablesreportingregressionresultsfromhereon,wegenerallyonlyreportresultscorrespondingto column3inTable4,i.e.forfemaleandmalecandidatestogetherinaspecificationthatincludesadummyformalecandi datesandagedummies.Thereasonforthischoiceisthatwhencomparingtheestimatedbeautycoefficientsforfemaleand malecandidates,thedifferenceisnotstatisticallysignificantinregressionsbasedondatafromourmainsurvey.

21 TABLE 6. Relativesuccessintheparliamentaryandmunicipalelections,incumbentsandnonincumbents.

(1) (2)

Relativesuccess Relativesuccess

parliamentaryelection municipalelections

Beauty 19.13*** 17.36**

(5.82) (7.74)

Competence 11.57 5.49

(8.09) (10.48)

Trustworthiness 6.41 0.25

(6.59) (12.16)

Incumbent 190.86*** 352.91***

(19.35) (35.40)

Malecandidate 2.57 18.33**

(6.79) (9.12)

Young(age<30) 19.27** 5.49

(7.61) (10.08)

Old(age>60) 14.72 9.51

(18.16) (17.63)

Numberofcand idates 743 1,043

AdjustedRsquared 0.36 0.39

Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses. The regressions include a constant term. * significant at 10%; ** significant at 5%; ***significantat1%.

Fortheparliamentaryelection,reportedincolumn1,thebeautycoefficientissmaller thanitscounterpartintheregressionwithnonincumbentsonlyandimpliesthataonestan darddeviationincreaseinbeautyisassociatedwith an increase of relative success of 12.4 units.Perceivedcompetencedoesnotattainstatistical significance. For the municipal elec tions,reportedincolumn2,beautyhasacoefficientofalmostthesamesizeasintheregres sionwithnonincumbentsonly,andthestatisticalsignificanceofperceivedcompetencethat appearedinthatregressionvanishes. 23

Finally,wehavecarriedoutsomehypotheticalandpurelymechanicalcalculationsin ordertoroughlyseehowmanynonelectedcandidatesthatcouldhavebeenelectedifthey hadhadbetterlooks.Oneachlist,thiswasdonebyanimaginaryreductionofthebeautyas

23 ResultswithoutagedummiesforTables4–6areverysimilarandareavailableuponrequest.

22 sessmentofallelectedcandidatesbyonestandarddeviationcombinedwithanequallylarge imaginaryincreaseinthebeautyassessmentofthesamenumberofnonelectedcandidates.

UsingtheestimatedbeautycoefficientsinTable6,thishypotheticalprocedureaddstothe relativesuccessofnonelectedcandidatesattheexpenseoftheelectedones.Thiscrudeexpe rimentshowsthat15%ofthecandidateselectedintheparliamentaryelectionwouldbere placed by competitors who were made more beautiful through this procedure. The corres pondingfigureinthemunicipalelectionsis11%.

5.4.OccupationandEducationasAlternativeThinSlicesofInformation

Finnishcandidatesareallowedtoreporttheireducationandoccupationontheofficialparty liststhatareplacedinvotingbooths.Almostallcandidates−98%inoursample−reportat leastoneofthesepiecesofinformationontheirpartylist.Thisinformationonthecandidates’ educationandoccupationisalsolistedinmostelectoralads.Therefore,votershaveaccessto atleasttwootherthinslicesofinformation,inadditiontophotos. 24

Regressionresultsindicatethatthebeautycoefficientisvirtuallyunaffected,both intermsofsizeandstatisticalsignificance,whenweincludeourbatteryofoccupationaland educationaldummyvariables.Listingoneselfasaworker,artistorstudentisassociatedwith lowerelectoralsuccesswhenbothoccupationalandeducationaldummiesareincluded.Like wise,reportinguppersecondaryeducationorcomprehensiveschoolorlessisnegativelyre latedtoelectoralsuccess.DetailsarereportedinTableA2intheAppendix;column3inTa ble4canbeconsultedforcomparison.

Tosummarizeourfindings,beautyemergesasanassetinpolitics.

24 Infact,37%ofthevotersintheFinnishelectionstudystatedthatacandidate’seducationhadaconsiderableimpacton theirchoice(BengtssonandGrönlund2005,p.245).

23 5.5.Interpretation

Having establishedalinkbetweenbeautyassessmentsandelectoralsuccess,wenextbring togetherourresultsto evaluate alternativeexplanations of why beauty matters. Recall that

MobiusandRosenblat(2006)foundthatbeautycaninfluencepayoffsevenifitisuncorre latedwithproductivityandthatTodorovetal.(2005)foundthatcompetencewasthemost importantpredictorofelectoralsuccess.Withourdataweareabletodiscriminatebetween, ontheonehand,theexplanationthatvotersfavorgoodlookingcandidatesbecausetheyenjoy watchingthemorbecausegoodlookingpoliticiansaremoresuccessfulinsocialinteraction, and,ontheotherhand,theexplanationthatbeautymattersasasignalrelatedtocompetence orsimilartraits.

Wehavealreadyreportedincolumns1to3ofTable4thatbeautyassessmentsarea morerobustexplanatoryvariableofelectoralsuccessthancompetenceortrustworthinessas sessments.Beautyassessmentsarelikewiseamorerobustexplanatoryvariablethanintelli genceandlikabilityassessments(asreportedinSection6.2).Thisisourfirstpieceofevi dencesuggestingthatbeautyplaysaroleofitsown,ratherthanjustservingasasignalof competence.Alsoourfindingthatbeautyretainsitsstatisticalsignificancewhenoccupation andeducationareincludedintheregressionssuggeststhatbeautydoesnotonlymatterasa signalofcompetence.

Ourinclusionofseveraltraitevaluationsandofinformationaboutoccupationandedu cationthussuggeststhatvotersfavorgoodlookingcandidatesbecausetheyenjoywatching goodlookingpoliticians,orbecause goodlookingpoliticiansaremoresuccessfulinsocial interaction.Anotherpossibility−which wecannot ruleoutwithourdata−isthatbeauty servesasasignalforsomeothercharacteristicsthancompetence,intelligence,likabilityor trustworthiness.

Asourthirdpieceofevidenceweconjecturethatifbeautyhelpsbecausevotersliketo watchgoodlookingpoliticiansorbecausegoodlookingpeoplearemoresuccessfulinsocial interaction,thenitshouldmattersimilarlyforincumbentsandchallengers.If,ontheother hand,beautyservesasasignal(ofcompetence,healthorothertraits)thenonewouldexpect

24 itseffecttobelargerforchallengers,asvotersdonothaveasmuchinformationaboutthemas theyhaveaboutincumbents.Wefindthataninteractiontermbetweenbeautyandincumben cyisstatisticallyinsignificant,bothintheparliamentaryandinthemunicipalelections.Simi larly, if we include interaction terms between incumbency and the three variables beauty, competenceandtrustworthiness,theyareallstatisticallyinsignificant.Sincebeautyappearsto beasimportantforincumbentsasforchallengers,alsoourthirdtestsupportstheinterpreta tionthatsocialskillsorthepleasureoflookingatbeautifulpeopleexplainwhybetterlooking candidatesaremorepopular.

6.SensitivityAnalysis

Wewillnowinvestigatetowhatextenttheresultsreportedsofararesensitivetovariousal ternativewaysofexaminingtherelationshipbetweenbeautyandelectoralsuccess.Wereport theresultsbriefly,butineachcase,alldetailsare available upon request. Our finding that beautyisstronglyassociatedwithelectoralsuccessismaintainedineachalternativespecifica tion.

6.1.MeasuresBasedonOrdinalAssessments

Beauty,competenceandtrustworthinesshavesofarbeenmeasuredcardinally.Wehavealso usedalternativemeasuresbasedonordinalassessments.Likeourprevioustraitvariables, thesevariables,beautyshare,competenceshareandtrustshare,areconstructedintwosteps.

Firstwecomputetheshareofassessmentswhereacandidatewasfoundtobethemostbeauti ful,mostcompetentandmosttrustworthy,whenpresentedwiththreeotherrandomlychosen candidates.Fromthismeasurewethensubtractitsmeanoverthenonincumbentsonthesame list.TheresultsrevealthatthepreviousqualitativeresultsofTables4and5hold,asbeauty dominatesandretainsstatisticalsignificance.Anincreaseinthebeautysharebyonestandard deviationisassociatedwithanincreaseinthenumberofvotesby39%fortheaveragenon

25 incumbentcandidate.Theseresultsindicatethatthepositiverelationbetweenbeautyandelec toralsuccessisnotjustaconsequenceofthequestionusedorthewayweconstructtheex planatoryvariables.

6.2.SensitivityinOtherDimensions

Wehavealsomadeanumberofminorchangesinourmainempiricalspecifications(in

Tables4and5).Herewereportresultsfromspecificationswhereweexchangethedependent variable,redefineincumbency,includeperceptionsofadditionaltraits,checkforoutliers,sep aratestudentsandnonstudents,anduseperceivedinsteadofrealage.

Webeginbyreplacingrelativesuccesswithvoteshareasthedependentvariableinthe regressionsreportedinSection5,toseewhethertheresultsarequalitativelyaffected.Vote shareisdefinedinthefollowingwayforcandidate ionlist j,

voteshare i,j =(p i/w j)*100(2) where piisnonincumbentcandidate i’snumberofpersonalvotesand wjisthenumberofall votesfornonincumbentcandidatesonlist j. Therelationshipbetweenthismeasureandrela tivesuccessisthatw j=v j*thenumberofnonincumbentcandidatesonlist j.Thisvariableis easiertointerpretintuitivelythanrelativesuccess,butsincethenumberofcandidatesdiffer betweenlists,theestimatedcoefficientsfordifferentlistsarenotreadilycomparable. 25 It turnsoutthattheresultsarequalitativelyverysimilartothoseofTable4.Againandmost notably,wefindthatbeautyisbyfarourmostimportantexplanatoryvariable.Competence doesnotattainstatisticalsignificance.Ahigherbeautyscoreofonestandarddeviationim pliesanincreaseof1.61percentagepointsinthevoteshareintheparliamentaryelection.In themunicipalelection,withmorecandidatesonthelists,thecorrespondingfigureis0.15.

Althoughthesenumbersmayappearsmall,notethattheaveragevoteshareamongallnon

25 SeeBerggren,Jordahl,andPoutvaara(2006)foraversionofthisstudywherevotesharewasusedasthemaindependent variable.

26 incumbentsis4.47%intheparliamentaryelectionand0.57%inthemunicipalelections.The correspondingaveragesforelectednonincumbentsare11.75%and2.21%.

Thesecondchangewemaketotestthesensitivityofourresultsistoredefineincum bency.Above,incumbentsweredefinedaspoliticalcandidateswhoservedintheofficein questionorasmembersofthenationalorEuropeanparliamentsatthetimeoftheelection.A morecommondefinitionofincumbencyistoincludeonlythecandidateswhoservedinthe office inquestion (hence,regardingcandidateswhohadbeenelectedtosomeotherofficeas nonincumbents).Usingthisdefinition,Tables4–6havebeenreproducedandnobigdiffer encesappear,neitherfortheparliamentarynorforthemunicipalelections.

Intheempiricalmodelsreportedsofar,wehaveincludedthreeofthefivetraitsthat wereassessedbyourrespondents:beauty,competenceandtrustworthiness.Weexcludedli kabilityandintelligenceinordertosimplifytheanalysisandkeepthefocusonthreedissimi lartraits(e.g.,intelligencecanbeexpectedtobeconceptuallyquitesimilartocompetence).

Wehaveconductedtheanalysiswithallfivetraitsincluded,anditshowsthattheexclusionis aninnocuousone.Beautyretainsitsstatisticalsignificanceandremainsaboutasimportantin termsofcoefficientsizecomparedtoTable4(thecoefficientis27.3fortheparliamentary electionforallnonincumbentcandidates,comparedto31.12inTable4);whereasthelikabil ityandintelligencecoefficientsdonotattainstatisticalsignificance. 26

Tofurtherpinpointtherelationshipbetweenbeautyand electoralsuccess,andtosee whethertherelationshipisdrivenbyoutliers,wehavecomputedSpearmanrankcorrelations forthe444nonincumbentcandidatesintheHelsinkimunicipalelections.TheHelsinkimu nicipalelectionsarebestsuitedforthis,sinceallfourpartieshavealargenumberofcandi datesandaboutthesamenumberofnonincumbentsontheirlists.Therankcorrelationbe tween beauty and relative success is especially strong for female candidates, for whom

Spearman’srhois0.285.Formale candidates,thecorrelation is 0.103 but not statistically significant.Combiningfemaleandmalecandidateswegetastatisticallysignificantcorrela tionof0.232.

26 Includingfivetraitsinsteadofthreedoesnotresultinmulticollinearityproblemsaccordingtovarianceinflationfactors.

27 TheanalysisofSpearmanrankcorrelationsalsoallowsustocomparetherelationship betweenelectoralsuccessandtheassessmentsofthefivedifferenttraitsoneatatimeandto implementahorseracebetweentheseasexplanatoryvariablesforelectoralsuccess.Forboth females and males, the Spearman rank correlation between electoral success and beauty is largerandhasahigherlevelofstatisticalsignificancethantherankcorrelationbetweenelec toralsuccessandperceivedcompetence,trustworthiness,likability,orintelligence.

Unlikeseveralotherstudies,wehavesubstantial numbersofbothstudentsandnon students among our respondents. It turns out that the assessments by (undergraduate and graduate)students andotherrespondentsare remarkablysimilar,withtheonlystatistically significantdifferencesbeingthatstudentsassessthecandidatessomewhatmorenegativelyin beauty(averageof2.69vs.2.79fornonstudents)andsomewhatmorepositivelyintrustwor thiness(averageof3.23vs.3.17fornonstudents).Intermsofregressionresults,lookingat relativesuccess,nonincumbentcandidatesandconfininganalysistophotoswithatleastthree studentassessments,beauty attainsstatisticalsignificance and the size of the coefficient is

23.3intheparliamentaryand21.0inthemunicipalelections.Beautyremainsimportant,even whenjustusingthisgroupofrespondents.

Finally,asweaskedrespondentstoestimatetheageofeachcandidate,wehavealsoex changedtherealageusedintheregressionsabovewiththeageperceivedbyrespondents.It turnsoutthattheestimatedcoefficientofbeautyis almost identical whenperceived age is used.

7.ThreeAdditionalSurveys

Inadditiontothesensitivityanalysisintheprecedingsection,withaninvestigationofthe resultsderivedfromourmainsurvey,wehavealsocarriedoutthree additionalsurveys,as wasreportedinSection3.2.Wehavedonethisinordertostudytheeffectsofusingrespon dentsfromFinland(whomayrecognizecandidates),inordertocompareourapproachofhav ingmanyrespondentseachofwhomassessesasmallnumberofphotoswiththatofmostpre

28 viousstudies(whichusefewrespondentseachofwhomassessesalargenumberofphotos) and,lastly,inordertoseewhetherknowingthatthephotosdepictpoliticalcandidatesaffects theassessments.

7.1.FinnishRespondents

WehaveundertakenasurveybasedonthesamesetofpoliticalcandidateswithonlyFinnish respondents(survey2).Theresultsindicateonlysmalldifferencescomparedtoourmainsur veywithnonFinnishrespondents.

AsweaskedtheFinnishrespondentstoindicateiftheyrecognizedcandidates,weare abletostudyhowresultsdifferinthedegreeofrecognition.InTable7,wereportestimated beauty and competence coefficients stemming from regressions using the same set of va riablesasinTable4,column3–i.e.beauty,competence, trustworthiness, male candidate, youngandold.Asbefore,werestrictourselvestononincumbents.

Column1containsregressionresultsforallcandidates.Column2containsresultsfrom regressionswhereweexcludeindividualassessmentsofcandidatesthattherespondentsrec ognized(bygivingafirstname,afamilynameorboth).Incolumn3weexcludephotosof candidatesrecognizedbyatleastonerespondent.Lastly,column4containsresultsfromnon

Finnishrespondentsbasedonthesamesampleofcandidates.27 Hence,asonemovestothe rightfromcolumn1to4,theprobabilityofcandidaterecognitionisgraduallydiminished.

27 Sincecoefficientsdonotchangemuchwhenweadjustthesampleofcandidates(comparecolumn4withcolumn3in

Table4andcolumn1inTable5),thedifferencesthatwedoobserveseemnottobedrivenbysamplecomposition,butin steadbyrecognition.

29 TABLE 7. Relativesuccess,nonincumbents.

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Finnishrespon Finnishrespondents, Finnishrespondents, NonFinnish

dents,including individualassessments photosofcandidates respondents

recognizedcan ofrecognizedcandi recognizedbyatleast

didates datesareexcluded onerespondentareex

cluded

Beauty,parliamentaryelection 30.37*** 32.54*** 21.48** 29.10***

Beauty,municipalelections 27.05*** 32.50*** 31.15*** 24.25***

Competence,parliamentaryelection 39.62*** 28.33** 52.49*** 11.74

Competence,municipalelections 31.90** 6.03 9.50 13.29*

Numberofcandidates,parliamentary 704 704 559 559 election

Numberofcandidates,municipal 965 965 799 799 elections

Notes:TheregressionmodelusedisthatofTable4,column3,andTable5,column1.Tofacilitatecomparability,thesampleincolumns 3and4isadjustedtocontainthesamesetofcandidates.Thistableonlyreportsthebeautyandcompetencecoefficients.*significantat 10%;**significantat5%;***significantat1%(basedonrobuststandarderrors).

The beauty coefficients are rather stable. The competence coefficients are in contrast

quiteunstable.Previousstudieshaveeitherjustexcluded individual assessments of recog

nizedcandidates(BenjaminandShapiro2006),excluded“wellknown”candidatesfromthe

setofphotos(KingandLeigh2007),orboth(Todorovetal.2005). 28 Sincerecognitioncanbe

partialandunconscious,wethinkthattheresults ofpreviousstudiesshouldbeinterpreted

withsomecaution,astheyarebasedonassessmentsbyrespondentsofthesamenationalityas

thepoliticalcandidatesanddonotsystematicallytestiftheuseofforeignrespondentspro

ducessimilarresults.Thisentailsariskfornonreportedrecognitionwhichweavoidinour

mainstudywithnonFinnishrespondents.Inparticular,theunstablecompetencecoefficients

pointatapossibleproblemwiththeresultsofTodorovetal.(2005),whofindthatperceived

competenceisagoodpredictorofelectoralsuccess.

28 BenjaminandShapiro(2006)didnotasktheirparticipantstoevaluatecandidatesfromMassachusetts,thestateinwhich

almostalloftheirparticipantsresided,ortoevaluatecandidatesfromthestatewheretheygrewup.KingandLeigh(2007)

alsouseonenonAustralianrespondenttoevaluatephotosinasensitivitytest.

30 7.2.RespondentsAssessingAllPhotos

Wehavealsoconductedsurvey3,withasmallnumberofrespondentswhoeachassessed all

504photosofHelsinkimunicipalcandidates. 29 Thereasonwastoseewhetherthiswayof assessingphotos–usedinlabormarketstudies–givesrisetodifferentoverallassessments andresultscomparedtotheapproachtakeninourothersurveys,whereamuchgreaternum berofrespondentseachassessedarandomlydrawnsmallnumberofphotos.Wehaveten

FinnishandsixSwedishrespondentsinthissurvey.Forbothnationalities,onehalfoftheres pondentsaremenandtheotherhalfwomen.Theyoungestrespondentis22andtheoldest70, with36asthemeanage. 30

WhenlookingatregressionresultsinTable8,threenewcomparisonscanbemade(all ofthemrestrictedtotheHelsinkimunicipalelection): between results based on this small survey’sSwedishrespondents(seecolumn1) andresultsfromourmainsurveywithnon

Finnishrespondents(column3);betweenresultsbasedonthissurvey’sFinnishrespondents

(column2)andresultsfromoursurveyofFinns(column4);andbetweenresultsbasedon

SwedishandresultsbasedonFinnishrespondentsinthesmallsurvey(columns1and2)

TABLE 8. RelativesuccessintheHelsinkimunicipalelections,allcandidates,nonincumbents.

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Swedishrespondents Finnishrespondents nonFinnishrespondents Finnishrespondents

smallsurvey smallsurvey mainsurvey surveyofFinns

Beauty 26.68** 24.77** 28.71** 28.69**

Competence 2.86 45.76** 21.36 43.61*

Notes:TheregressionmodelusedisthatofTable4,column3.Thistableonlyreportsthebeautyandcompetencecoefficients.*signifi cantat10%;**significantat5%;***significantat1%.

29 Thereasonforusingonlythissubsetofallphotosisthatitwouldbetootimeconsumingforarespondenttoevaluate

1,929photos.

30 ThepairwisecorrelationsofbeautyassessmentsamongourSwedishrespondentsrangefrom0.42to0.61,withanaverage of0.52,comparedtoarangefrom0.12to0.62withanaverageof0.42fortheFinnishrespondents.

31 Thedifferencesasfarasbeautyisconcernedareverysmallirrespectiveofwhichcomparison ismade–andnotably,beautyretainsstatisticalsignificancethroughout.Onceagain,thereisa differenceinthecompetencecoefficientsbetweenthenonFinnishrespondentsincolumns1 and3andtheFinnishrespondentsincolumns2and4.Thisdifference,asweargueinSection

7.1,plausiblydependsontheoccurrenceofrecognition.

Thus,thetwomethods–usingasmallnumberofrespondentswhoassessallphotosand usingalargenumberofrespondentswhoassessarandomselectionofphotos–seemtoyield quitesimilarresults.Wehaveinvestigatedthistentativeconclusionfurther,inordertoseeto whatextenttheresultsaresensitivetothecompositionofrespondents.Todothiswehave composeddifferentgroupsoffourrespondentsandestimatedregressionsbasedontheiras sessments,alongthelinesofHamermesh(2006)andKingandLeigh(2007).Indoingthiswe keptthegroupsbalancedintermsofageandgenderofincludedrespondents.Whenstudying

Swedishrespondents,thiswasdonebylettingeachgroupbecomposedoftwomenandtwo women,includingtheoldestpersonofeachgender.Thisrestrictiongivesrisetofourgroups ofSwedishrespondents.Wefindthatbeautycoefficientsarequitestable(rangingfrom25.8 to29.3)andalwaysstatisticallysignificant.However, when combining the ten Finnish re spondentsin16differentgroupsoffour,theresultsarenotasclearcut,againplausiblyre flectingtheimportanceofrecognition.Thebeautycoefficientsrangefrom10.4to20.3and areonlystatisticallysignificantinsixofthe16regressions.Thisfluctuationsuggeststhatsur veyswithasmallnumberofrespondents–fourinthiscase–mayproduceunstableresults whenrespondentscomefromthesamecountryasthepersonsthattheyassess.

7.3.RespondentswithoutInformationaboutthePhotos

Wehavefurthermoreconductedsurvey4,thenoinformationsurvey,inwhichitwas not re vealed that the photos depict political candidates or that we are studying politics. 31 Once

31 Inthissurvey,respondentshadtoevaluateatleasttenphotos.Anotherdifferencewasthattherewasnooptionofchoosing

“Donotknow/Donotwanttoanswer”whenevaluatingthephotos.

32 more,thesamephotosareusedasinthemainsurveyandinthesurveywithonlyFinnishre spondents.

Intermsofaverageassessmentsofthetraits,wefindsmalldifferences,typicallyinthe orderof0.1–0.2unitsonthefivepointscale,comparedtowhenrespondentsknewthatthe photosdepictedpoliticalcandidates.Wehavealsocarriedoutregressionsfortheparliamenta ryandmunicipalelectionsfornonincumbents.Intheparliamentaryelection,beautyretains statisticalsignificanceforallcandidates,butitissomewhatlessimportantintermsofesti matedcoefficientsize,comparedtotheresultsinTable4.Inthemunicipalelections,beauty is somewhat more important for female candidates and less important for male candidates

(compared with Table 5). Furthermore, tests of statistical significance indicate a difference betweenthebeautycoefficientsoffemaleandmalecandidates,thelatterbeingsmallerand notstatisticallysignificantlydifferentfromzero.

Inall,thesefindingsindicatethatassessmentsmaybemodestlyaffectedbytheknow ledgethatthephotosdepictpoliticalcandidates.Butbeautycomesthroughasastrongexpla natoryvariableheretoo,especiallyforfemalecandidates.

8.Conclusions

WeinvestigatehowbeautyisrelatedtoelectoralsuccessinFinlandandfindthatcandidates wholookbetterthantheirlistcompetitorsaremoresuccessful.Intheparliamentaryelection, anincreaseinbeautyofonestandarddeviationisassociatedwitha20%increaseinthenum berofvotesfortheaveragenonincumbentcandidate.Inthemunicipalelections,thefigureis

17%.ThefiguresarebasedonassessmentsbynonFinnsinordertomakesurethatcandidates werenotrecognized.

The Finnish electoral system provides an ideal testing ground. It is proportional and eachvoterhastovoteforonecandidateonapartylist,whichmakesitpossibletolookatthe effectofbeautyinwithinpartycompetition.Studyingwithinpartycompetitionholdsseveral advantages.First,studiesofbetweenpartycompetitionmayfaceareversecausalityproblem

33 ifapartyismoresuccessfulinrecruitinggoodlookingcandidatesindistrictswhereitenjoys strongsupport.Second,withinpartycompetitionallowsustocontrolforideologyveryeffec tively.Third,wecanstudynonincumbentcandidatesseparatelyinadditiontoasampleof bothincumbentsandnonincumbents.

Whydoesbeautymatter?Weareabletodiscriminatebetweentwoalternativeexplana tions.Ontheonehand,votersmayfavorgoodlookingcandidateseitherbecausetheyenjoy watchingthemorbecausegoodlookingpoliticiansaremoresuccessfulinsocialinteraction.

Ontheotherhand,beautymaymatterasathinsliceofinformationusedtoinfercompetence orsimilartraits.ThisexplanationaccordswellwiththeresultsofTodorovetal.(2005),indi catingthatcompetenceassessmentspredictelectoralsuccess.

Evidenceassembledbystudyingbeautyassessmentstogetherwithassessmentsofcom petenceandothertraits,byincludinginformationabouteducationandoccupationasalterna tivethinslicesofinformation,andbytestingwhether the effects of beauty differ between incumbentsandnonincumbentsallpointinthesame direction: voters favor goodlooking candidateseitherbecausetheyenjoywatchingthem,orbecausegoodlookingpoliticiansare moresuccessfulinsocialinteraction.ThereareatleasttwopotentialreasonsforwhyTodorov etal.obtainadifferentresult:unreportedcandidaterecognitionandreversecausality.

Extensivesensitivityanalysisconfirmsourmainresults.Usingmeasuresbasedonor dinalassessmentsofbeauty,competenceandtrustworthiness,aswellasmoreminorspecifi cationchanges–noneofthesemodificationsalterthequalitativefindings.Furthermore,our three additional surveys, using Finnish respondents, using respondents who assessed all as opposedtoarandomselectionofphotosandusingrespondentswhodidnotknowthatthe photosdepictpoliticalcandidates,confirmthemainresult.Beautymatters.

Lastly,inFinland,allpartieshaveseveralmaleandfemalecandidatesoneachlist.Al thoughtheestimatedeffectofbeautyisaboutthesameformaleandfemalecandidatesinour mainregressions,therearesomesignsofbeautybeingmoreimportantforfemalecandidates.

First,beautymattersonlyforfemalecandidatesinsomespecificationsinthemunicipalelec tions.Second,theSpearmanrank correlationbetween beauty and our measure of electoral successisstatisticallysignificantonlyforfemalecandidates(intheHelsinkimunicipalelec

34 tion).Third,thestandarddeviationofthecandidates’ beauty is higher for female than for malecandidates,meaningthatalargershareoffemalecandidatescanbefoundintheupper tailofthebeautydistribution.Inconsequence,beautyseemsmoreimportantforfemalecan didates.Thisstandsincontrasttolabormarketstudies,wherethebeautypremiumhasbeen foundtobenefitmalesmorethanfemales.

35 Appendix

BOX A1. Excerptfromthewebsurvey.

Whatisyourevaluationofthephysicalappearance orattractivenessofthispersoncomparedtotheaverage amongpeoplelivinginyourcountryofresidence? Veryunattractive Belowaverage Average Aboveaverage Veryhandsomeorbeautiful Cannotsay/Prefernottoanswer Whatisyourevaluationofthecompetenceofthispersoncomparedtotheaverageamongpeoplelivinginyour countryofresidence? Veryincompetent Belowaverage Average Aboveaverage Verycompetent Cannotsay/Prefernottoanswer Whatisyourevaluationofthelikabilityofthisp erson(i.e.hownice,pleasant,andagreeabledoyoufindthis person)comparedtotheaverageamongpeoplelivinginyourcountryofresidence? Veryunlikable Belowaverage Average Aboveaverage Verylikable Cannotsay/Prefernottoanswer Whatisyourevaluationofthetrustworthinessofthisperson(i.e.howethical,honest,andresponsibledoyou findthisperson)comparedtotheaverageamongpeo plelivinginyourcountryofresidence? Veryuntrustworthy Belowaverage Average Aboveaverage Verytrustworthy Cannotsay/Prefernottoanswer Whatisyourevaluationoftheintelligenceofthis personcomparedtotheaverageamongpeoplelivinginyour countryofresidence? Veryunintelligent Belowaverage Average Aboveaverage Veryintelligent Cannotsay/Prefernottoanswer Whatisyourevaluationoftheageofthisperson?Useyourkeyboardtofillintheageintheboxbelow.

TABLE A1. Correlationmatrix.

Beauty Competence Trustworthiness Likability Intelligence

Beauty 1.00

Competence 0.32 1.00

Trustworthiness 0.22 0.38 1.00

Likability 0.41 0.32 0.51 1.00

Intelligence 0.28 0.65 0.36 0.28 1.00

Notes:Allofthereportedtraitsexhibitstatisticallysignificantcorrelationswitheachother.

36 5

4,5

4 Men assessing elected male candidates 3,5 Men assessing non-elected male candidates 3 Women assessing elected male candidates 2,5 Women assessing non-elected 2 male candidates

1,5

1

FIGURE A1. Assessmentsofelectedandnonelectedmalecandidates.

5

4,5

4 Men assessing elected female candidates 3,5 Men assessing non-elected female candidates 3 Women assessing elected female candidates 2,5 Women assessing non-elected 2 female candidates

1,5

1

FIGURE A2. Assessmentsofelectedandnonelectedfemalecandidates.

37

TABLE A2. Relativesuccessintheparliamentaryelection,withoccupationalandeducationaldummies.

Relativesuccess Relativesuccess Relativesuccess

nonincumbents nonincumbents nonincumbents

Beauty 29.58***(6.22) 29.16***(6.42) 28.79***(6.22)

Competence 9.56(8.22) 10.51(8.58) 10.18(8.25)

Trustworthiness 2.56(8.52) 6.54(8.72) 3.53(8.54)

Partyworker 16.12(30.8) 14.77(30.0)

Management 6.82(18.5) 0.01(18.9)

Researcher 36.61(27.8) 26.79(28.1)

Teacher 15.87(14.9) 25.54(16.3)

Upperwhitecollar 8.38(15.6) 15.21(16.4)

Medicaldoctor 2.17(17.7) 14.43(19.4)

Nurse 19.58(16.1) 14.26(17.5)

Lowerwhitecollar 23.15(14.9) 23.31(15.3)

Worker 34.92**(14.1) 30.07**(13.9)

Entrepreneur 15.09(17.8) 16.32(17.7)

Artist 36.73**(16.2) 39.49**(16.0)

Student 53.25***(17.8) 34.23**(15.6)

Notemployed 38.37*(20.4) 29.67(19.6)

Universityeducation 17.14**(8.62) 13.15(10.5)

Vocationaleducation 9.98(8.86) 4.70(10.3)

Uppersecondaryeducation 35.18***(12.5) 25.33*(14.6)

Comprehensiveschoolorless 49.06***(10.4) 44.18***(10.7)

Maledummy 3.80(6.92) 6.14(6.76) 4.83(7.00)

Young(age<30) 5.77(12.2) 3.16(12.7) 0.54(13.1)

Old(age>60) 10.67(22.5) 9.62(21.5) 9.79(21.9)

Numberofcandidates 641 641 641

AdjustedRsquared 0.09 0.09 0.10

Notes:TheoccupationalclassificationfollowsStatisticsFinland(2001),thoughwehavemergedcertainoccupationalcategorieswitha smallnumberofcandidatesandlistedpartyworkersasagroupoftheirown.Thereferencegroupforoccupationiscandidateswhodidnot listtheiroccupation.Comprehensiveschoolorlesscorrespondstoatmost10yearsofschooling.Uppersecondaryeducationcorrespondsto 12yearsofschooling,andvocationaleducation10−12years.Uppersecondaryeducationservesusuallyaspreparingforuniversitylevel education,andmanyofthecandidateswithuppersecondaryeducationlistedashighesteducationhavestarted,butnotcompleted,university studies.Vocationaleducationincludes,e.g.,basicnurses,nurses,commercialschoolgraduates,clerks,andartisans.Thereferencegroupfor educationiscandidateswhodidnotlisttheireducation.Robuststandarderrorsinparentheses.Theregressionsincludeaconstantterm.* significantat10%;**significantat5%;***significantat1%.

38 References

Aharon, Itzhak, Nancy Etcoff, Dan Ariely, Christopher F. Chabris, Ethan O’Connor, and

HansC.Breiter(2001).“BeautifulFacesHaveVariableRewardValue:fMRIandBe

havioralEvidence.” euron, 32,537–551.

Ambady,Nalini,andRobertRosenthal(1992).“ThinSlicesofExpressiveBehaviorasPre

dictors of Interpersonal Consequences: A MetaAnalysis.” Psychological Bulletin,

111,256–274.

Andreoni, James, and Ragan Petrie (forthcoming). “Beauty, Gender and Stereotypes: Evi

dencefromLaboratoryExperiments.” JournalofEconomicPsychology .

Bartels,LarryM.(1996).“UninformedVotes:InformationEffectsinPresidentialElections.”

AmericanJournalofPoliticalScience, 40,194–231.

Bengtsson,Åsa,andKimmoGrönlund(2005).“Ehdokasvalinta.”[”CandidateSelection.”]In

VaalitjademokratiaSuomessa[ElectionsandDemocracyinFinland],editedby

HeikkiPaloheimo.WSOY.

Benjamin, Daniel J., and Jesse M. Shapiro (2006). “ThinSlice Forecasts of Gubernatorial

Elections.”NBERWorkingPaperNo.12660.November.

Berggren,Niclas,HenrikJordahl,andPanuPoutvaara(2006).“TheLooksofaWinner:

Beauty,GenderandElectoralSuccess.”IZADiscussionPaperNo.2311.September.

Besley,Timothy(2004).“PayingPoliticians:TheoryandEvidence.” JournaloftheEuropean

EconomicAssociation ,2,193–215.

Biddle,JeffE.,andDanielS.Hamermesh(1998).“Beauty,ProductivityandDiscrimination:

Lawyers’LooksandLucre.” JournalofLaborEconomics, 16,172–201.

Budesheim,ThomasLee,andStephenJ.DePaola(1994).“BeautyortheBeast?TheEffects

ofAppearance,Personality,andIssueInformationonEvaluationsofPoliticalCandi

dates.” PersonalityandSocialPsychologyBulletin, 20,339–348.

Caprara,GianVittorio,ClaudioBarbaranelli,andPhilipG.Zimbardo(1997).”Politicians’

UniquelySimplePersonalities.” ature, 385,493.

39 Caselli,Francesco,andMassimoMorelli(2004).“BadPoliticians.” JournalofPublicEco

nomics ,88,759–782.

Cunningham,MichaelR.,AlanR.Roberts,AnitaP. Barbee, Perry B. Druen, and Cheng

Huan Wu (1995). “Consistency and Variability in the CrossCultural Perception of

FemalePhysicalAttractiveness.” JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology, 68,

261–279.

Downs,Anthony(1957).AnEconomicTheoryofDemocracy.HarperCollins.

Eagly, Alice H., Richard D. Ashmore, Mona G. Makhijani, and Laura C. Longo (1991).

“What Is Beautiful Is Good, But…: A MetaAnalytic Review of Research on the

PhysicalAttractivenessStereotype.” PsychologicalBulletin, 110,109–128.

Feingold, Alan (1992a). “GoodLooking People Are Not What We Think.” Psychological

Bulletin, 111,304–341.

Feingold, Alan (1992b). “Gender Differences in MateSelectionPreferences:ATestofthe

ParentalInvestmentModel.” PsychologicalBulletin, 112,125–139.

French,MichaelT.(2002).“PhysicalAppearanceandEarnings:FurtherEvidence.”Applied

Economics, 34,569 −572.

Hamermesh,DanielS.(2006).“ChangingLooksandChanging'Discrimination':TheBeauty

ofEconomists.” EconomicsLetters, 93,405–412.

Hamermesh,DanielS.,andJeffE.Biddle(1994).“BeautyandtheLaborMarket.” American

EconomicReview, 84,1174 −1194.

Hamermesh, Daniel S., Xin Meng, and Junsen Zhang (2002). “Dress for Success – Does

PrimpingPay?” LabourEconomics, 9,361–373.

Harper,Barry(2000).“Beauty,StatureandtheLabourMarket:ABritishCohortStudy.” Ox

fordBulletinofEconomicsandStatistics, 62,771 −800.

Jackson,LindaA.,JohnE.Hunter,andCaroleN.Hodge(1995).“PhysicalAttractivenessand

IntellectualCompetence:AMetaAnalyticReview.” SocialPsychologyQuarterly, 63,

108–122.

Kahneman,Daniel,PaulSlovic,andAmosTversky(1986).JudgementunderUncertainty:

HeuristicsandBiases.CambridgeUniversityPress.

40 Kanazawa,Satoshi,andJodyL.Kovar(2004).“WhyBeautifulPeopleAreMoreIntelligent.”

Intelligence, 32,227–243.

King,Amy,andAndrewLeigh(2007).“BeautifulPoliticians.”AvailableatSSRN:

http://ssrn.com/abstract=961138 .

Langlois,JudithH.,LisaKalakanis,AdamJ.Rubenstein,AndreaLarson,MonicaHallamm,

andMonicaSmoot(2000).“MaximsorMythsofBeauty?AMetaAnalyticandTheo

reticalReview.” PsychologicalBulletin, 126,390–423.

Lau,RichardR.,andDavidP.Redlawsk(2001).“AdvantagesandDisadvantagesofCogni

tiveHeuristicsinPoliticalDecisionMaking.” AmericanJournalofPoliticalScience,

45,951–971.

Lee,DavidS.(forthcoming).“RandomizedExperimentsfromNonRandomSelectioninU.S.

HouseElections.” JournalofEconometrics .

Lupia,Arthur(1994).“ShortcutsVersusEncyclopedias:InformationandVotingBehaviorin

CaliforniaInsuranceReformElections.” AmericanPoliticalScienceReview, 88,63–

76.

Macrae,C.Neil,AlanB.Milne,andGalenV.Bodenhausen(1994).“StereotypesasEnergy

SavingDevices:APeekInsidetheCognitiveToolbox.”JournalofPersonalityand

SocialPsychology, 66,37–47.

Messner,Matthias,andMattiasK.Polborn(2004).“Paying Politicians,” Journal of Public

Economics, 88,2423–2445.

Mobius,MarkusM.,andTanyaS.Rosenblat(2006).“WhyBeautyMatters.” AmericanEco

nomicReview, 96,222–235.

Mocan,Naci,andErdalTekin(2006).“UglyCriminals.”NBERWorkingPaperNo.12019.

Pfann,GerardA.,JeffE.Biddle,DanielS.Hamermesh,andCiskaM.Bosman(2000).“Busi

nessSuccessandBusinesses’BeautyCapital.” EconomicsLetters, 67,201–207.

Poutvaara, Panu, and Tuomas Takalo (2007). “Candidate Quality.” International Tax and

PublicFinance, 14,7–27.

Raunio,Tapio(2005).“Finland:OneHundredYearsofQuietude.”In ThePoliticsofElector

alSystems, editedbyMichaelGallagherandPaulMitchell.OxfordUniversityPress.

41 Redlawsk,DavidP.,andRichardR.Lau(2003).“DoVotersWantCandidatesTheyLikeor

CandidatesTheyAgreeWith?Affectvs.CognitioninVoterDecisionMaking.”

Mimeograph,UniversityofIowa.

Reynolds,Andrew,BenReilly,andAndrewEllis(2005). ElectoralSystemDesign:Thenew

International IDEA Handbook. International Institute for Democracy and Electoral

Assistance.

Rosar,Ulrich,MarkusKlein,andTiloBeckers(forthcoming).“TheFrogPondBeautyCon

test:PhysicalAttractivenessandElectoralSuccessoftheConstituencyCandidatesat

theNorthRhineWestphaliaStateElection2005.” EuropeanJournalofPoliticalRe

search .

Simon,HerbertA.(1985).“HumanNatureinPolitics:TheDialogueofPsychologywithPo

liticalScience.” AmericanPoliticalScienceReview, 79,293–304.

Statistics Finland (2001). “Classification of Occupations 2001.”

http://tilastokeskus.fi/tk/tt/luokitukset/lk_en/ammatti_index.html.

Statistics Finland (2003). “Parliamentary Elections, 2003.”

http://www.tilastokeskus.fi/tup/suoluk/suoluk_vaalit_en.html.

Todorov,Alexander,AnesuN.Mandisodza,AmirGoren,andCrystalC.Hall(2005).“Infe

rencesofCompetencefromFacesPredictElectionOutcomes.” Science, 308,1623–

1626.

Wattenberg,MartinP(1991).TheRiseofCandidateCenteredPolitics:PresidentialElections

ofthe1980s.HarvardUniversityPress.

Willis,Janine,andAlexanderTodorov(2006).“FirstImpressions:MakingUpYourMind

Aftera100MsExposuretoaFace.” PsychologicalScience, 17,592–598.

42