Primate Cognition: from 'What Now?' to 'What If ?'

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Primate Cognition: from 'What Now?' to 'What If ?' 494 Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.11 November 2003 Primate cognition: from ‘what now?’ to ‘what if ?’ Louise Barrett1, Peter Henzi2 and Robin Dunbar1 1School of Biological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Biosciences Building, Crown St, Liverpool, UK L69 7ZB 2Behavioural Ecology Research Group, University of Natal, 4041 Durban, South Africa The ‘social brain’ hypothesis has had a major impact on unique features of human cognition, such as language the study of comparative cognition. However, despite a and metacognition [11]. strong sense, gained from both experimental and Here, we present a new hypothesis to explain the observational work, that monkeys and apes differ from differences between monkeys and apes, based on an each other, we are still no closer to understanding understanding of the ecological constraints under which exactly how they differ. We hypothesize that the dis- these animals operate. Specifically, we argue that the persed social systems characteristic of ape societies dispersed social systems (‘fission–fusion’ societies) in explains why monkeys and apes should differ cogni- which most (if not all) of the hominoids (apes and humans) tively. The increased cognitive control and analogical live can be viewed as cognitively more demanding social reasoning ability needed to cope with life in dispersed ‘market places’ than monkey groups. The demands of societies also suggests a possible route for human cog- navigating a more complex social landscape thus con- nitive evolution. This hypothesis is supported by beha- stituted a unique selection pressure among the ancestral vioural and neurobiological data, but we need more of apes for increased brain size and cognitive abilities. This both if we are to fully understand how our primate cou- ecologically-valid approach will, we hope, enable research- sins see the world. ers to uncover the cognitive differences that have, so far, been concealed. Primates have unusually large brains for body size. It is now widely recognized that this is mainly associated with The social market place their distinctive social skills (the ‘Machiavellian intelli- To date, the social brain hypothesis has focused on a general notion of social complexity (often indexed by group gence’ [1] or ‘social brain’ hypothesis’ [2,3]). Within the size) as a driving force in brain evolution [3] and has not primate array, however, there are significant differences in attempted to explain how the particular characteristics of competence – most strikingly between monkeys and apes species’ social structure are related to cognitive capacity. – both in general cognitive abilities and, more importantly, However, recent empirical work on non-human primates in social behaviour and sociocognitive skills. These [12–14], along with theoretical speculations on humans contrasts seem to parallel differences between species in [15], suggests that applying a ‘biological markets’ [16] key aspects of brain evolution [4–6]. framework to studies of primate sociality might help point However, despite extensive psychological experimen- the way to more informative experimental studies of tation, the exact nature of the cognitive differences monkey and ape cognition. The theory of biological between monkeys and apes are unclear [7], and the degree markets views animals as traders engaged in a mutually to which apes are capable of any form of metarepresenta- beneficial exchange of commodities. Individuals within a tional thought (whether socially-based or not) remains biological market attempt to maximize ‘profit’ (in terms of contentious [8,9]. We suggest that this is because the tests fitness) by selecting social partners that offer the best have not been exhaustive and/or the right kinds of tests value, with an exchange rate set by the supply and demand have not been performed. The latter could be because of the commodity in question. much of the work on social cognition has been anthropo- Unlike standard optimality models of decision making, centric, seeking to discover which human cognitive market models are inherently dynamic, recognizing that characteristics are shared by our simian cousins [10]. individuals’ decisions vary across time according to local Thesameistruefromaneurobiologicalperspective, circumstance. They are perfect for the analysis of social where the use of monkeys as a model species has decision-making because the social world is also inher- meant, again, that similarities between non-human ently dynamic: each individual has its own particular set of primates and humans have been highlighted at the desires and goals that might not coincide with those of expense of distinctive differences. It is only recently others and decisions therefore reflect an ongoing process of that researchers have begun to probe for neurobiolo- negotiation. gical differences that might be linked to the apparently Studies of baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus) [12–13] and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) [14] have shown that Corresponding author: Louise Barrett ([email protected]). animals trade grooming as a ‘service’, either for its own http://tics.trends.com 1364-6613/$ - see front matter q 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.005 Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.11 November 2003 495 utilitarian value or for its value equivalent; moreover, true market effects can be found, with the duration of grooming 1000 (the ‘price paid’) varying in relation to the supply of particular commodities [13]. Viewing primate groups as market places allows us to refine the social brain 100 hypothesis because we can identify the key differences between ape and monkey market places and thus predict consequent differences in cognitive skills. 10 Mean group size Mean group A market for brain power In primate market places, individuals must track the price 1 0.1 1 10 of commodities and respond flexibly to changes in supply Neocortex ratio and demand. This agrees with the ‘Machiavellian intelli- gence’ hypothesis, in that sociality is assumed to drive TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences brain evolution. It differs, however, by assuming that Figure 1. Evidence for grades within the primate brain suggests that there may be animals have not been selected to cope with increasingly quantitative, or even qualitative, differences between monkey and ape social cog- elaborate strategies and counter-strategies, the goals of nition. When a measure of social complexity (in this case, group size) is plotted which are to ‘outwit’ the competition. Instead, it assumes against a measure of relative brain size (in this case, the ratio of neocortex volume to the volume of the rest of the brain), it is apparent that hominoids (apes and that the evolution of brain size and structure have been humans, orange squares) require more neural capacity to support a given group driven by a need to track fluctuations in commodity value. size than monkeys (pink circles) do, who in turn require more than prosimians Monitoring the market place is intrinsically complex (blue triangles). Each datapoint is a single genus. Humans are represented by the top right-hand square. because the value of a particular partner is contingent on the value of others. Each of these values can shift with whole. By contrast, the nature of spider monkey fission– changes in reproductive state, health, dominance and fusion is not well studied and could differ from apes in ongoing social behaviour. Those who are good value today important ways. If, however, their behaviour is truly ape- might not be so tomorrow. This constant state of flux like, then we have an ideal test case: we would predict that means that keeping tabs on the social market is very they manifest ape-like cognitive abilities – something that different from the other kinds of contingent monitoring is, perhaps, hinted at by the fact that they are one of the that primates must do, such as tracking fluctuations in most encephalized of the South American monkeys. fruit availability [17]. Fruits, unlike conspecifics, do not In fission–fusion societies, individuals see each other make decisions of their own in response to primate only at infrequent intervals, often weeks apart, yet each behaviour. This inherent contingency in primate market recognizes the members of its community and is capable of places thus selects for a ‘knowledge-based’ [18], rather maintaining long-standing relationships. In such systems, than strictly cue-based, understanding of others, as individuals must be able to represent mentally individuals evidence from monkeys and, to an even greater degree, that are not present and to retain and manipulate apes has shown [18]. Nevertheless, there remains a information about them for substantial periods of time, cognitive difference between monkeys and apes that, whereas there is no such pressure for these abilities to although poorly characterized, is apparent when compar- evolve in spatially and temporally stable monkey groups ing their performance on psychological tests [7,19–20].It where animals are only out of view for at most a few hours. is also apparent that there are distinct grades within the The computational load required to monitor and track brain data that point specifically to increased cognitive changes in a dispersed ape market place is therefore powers among the apes [3,6,21] (Figure 1). We suggest that significantly greater than when members of a group see these emerged because of a key difference in the market each other every day [6]. A salient point to note in this places of these two taxonomic groups. context is that humans also live in dispersed societies Whereas monkeys are all highly gregarious and live in based on fission–fusion dynamics [25]. cohesive groups in which individuals encounter every member of their group every day, the apes – in particular, Monitoring space and time the chimpanzee and orang-utan (Pongo pygmaeus) – live More importantly, in terms of monitoring the state of their in more fragmented societies as a result of food compe- social market place, animals need to recognize that the tition, which forces females to forage in small parties or absence of a particular individual can affect the relative alone [22].
Recommended publications
  • Mirror Neurons and Colamus Humanitatem Avances En Psicología Latinoamericana, Vol
    Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana ISSN: 1794-4724 [email protected] Universidad del Rosario Colombia Skoyles, John R. Why our brains cherish humanity: Mirror neurons and colamus humanitatem Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, vol. 26, núm. 1, junio, 2008, pp. 99-111 Universidad del Rosario Bogotá, Colombia Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=79926108 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Why our brains cherish humanity: Mirror neurons and colamus humanitatem JOHN R. SKOYLES* Centre for Mathematics and Physics in the Life Sciences and Experimental Biology (CoMPLEX) University College London, London, UK Centre for Philosophy of Natural and Social Science (CNPSS) London School of Economics, London, UK Abstract Resumen Commonsense says we are isolated. After all, our El sentido común dice que estamos aislados. Des- bodies are physically separate. But Seneca’s cola- pués de todo, nuestros cuerpos están separados mus humanitatem, and John Donne’s observation físicamente. Pero la obra Colamus humanitatem that “no man is an island” suggests we are neither de Séneca y la observación de que “ningún hombre entirely isolated nor separate. A recent discovery in es una isla”, que hizo John Donne, sugieren que no neuroscience—that of mirror neurons—argues that estamos ni completamente aislados ni separados. the brain and the mind is neither built nor functions Un descubrimiento reciente de la neurociencia, el remote from what happens in other individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Ethology Bulletin
    Human Ethology Bulletin http://evolution.anthro.univie.ac.at/ishe.html VOLUME 17, ISSUE 3 ISSN 0739-2036 SEPTEMBER 2002 © 2001 The lntenzational Society for Human Ethology ]oie de Vivre in Montreal! The ISHE conference programincluded plenary speakers Filippo Aurelli, Barry Bogin, Sarah Hrdy, and Carol van Schaik, as well as symposia, papers, and posters addressing numerous topics within the general domain of Human Ethology. Despite sessions that extended into the evening hours, the talks were all well attended by about 80-100 members. See pages inside for more details and conference photos. BALLOT FOR OFFICER ELECTIONS VICE-PRESIDENT/ PRESIDENT ELECT Glenn Weisfeld Write In: _ MEMBERSHIP CHAIR ___ Astrid Juette Write In: _ Old Montreal was the site of the 16th Biennial Conference of the International Society for Human Ethology from August 7 through Send ballot by mail or e-mail to: Saturday, August 10. The city lived up to its reputation as one of North Americas most beautiful and entertaining cities, renowned for its Frank Salter, ISHE Secretary gourmet dining and bustling nightlife. Max Planck Society Members were treated to four excellent plenary Von-der-Tann-Str.3 addresses that provided much food for thought, 82346 Andechs as well as foUl surnptuous luncheons served at the Germany Hotel de Gouverneur, topped off with a Saturday E-mail: salter@bumanethologiede evening banquet at the historic Pierre du Calvet in the heart of the old port, pictured above. HumanEthology Bulletin, 17 (3), 2002 2 Society News Editorial Staff I. Minutes of the ISHE Board Meeting Editor Peter LaFreniere Submitted by ISHE Secretary Frank Salter 362 Little Hall 1h Department of Psychology Gouveneur Hotel, Montreal, 6 August 2002 University of Maine Orono, ME 04469 USA tel.
    [Show full text]
  • Molecular Lamarckism: on the Evolution of Human Intelligence
    World Futures The Journal of New Paradigm Research ISSN: 0260-4027 (Print) 1556-1844 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gwof20 Molecular Lamarckism: On the Evolution of Human Intelligence Fredric M. Menger To cite this article: Fredric M. Menger (2017) Molecular Lamarckism: On the Evolution of Human Intelligence, World Futures, 73:2, 89-103, DOI: 10.1080/02604027.2017.1319669 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02604027.2017.1319669 © 2017 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC© Fredric M. Menger Published online: 26 May 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 3145 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 1 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=gwof20 World Futures, 73: 89–103, 2017 Copyright © Fredric M. Menger ISSN: 0260-4027 print / 1556-1844 online DOI: 10.1080/02604027.2017.1319669 MOLECULAR LAMARCKISM: ON THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN INTELLIGENCE Fredric M. Menger Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA In modern times, Lamarck’s view of evolution, based on inheritance of acquired traits has been superseded by neo-Darwinism, based on random DNA mutations. This article begins with a series of observations suggesting that Lamarckian inheritance is in fact operative throughout Nature. I then launch into a discussion of human intelligence that is the most important feature of human evolution that cannot be easily explained by mutational
    [Show full text]
  • Harrub, Brad Adn Et Al. "The Origin of Language and Communication"
    Athena and Eve — Johnson Papers lution, editors Jones, Martin and Pilbeam conceded that The origin of ‘there are no non-human languages’, and then went on to observe that ‘language is an adaptation unique to humans, and yet the nature of its uniqueness and its biological basis language and are notoriously difcult to dene’ [emphasis added].3 In his book, The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language communication and the Brain, Terrance Deacon noted: ‘In this context, then, consider the case of Brad Harrub, Bert Thompson and human language. It is one of the most distinctive Dave Miller behavioral adaptations on the planet. Languages evolved in only one species, in only one way, with- By age four, most humans have developed an ability out precedent, except in the most general sense. to communicate through oral language. By age six And the differences between languages and all other 4 or seven, most humans can comprehend, as well as natural modes of communicating are vast.’ express, written thoughts. These unique abilities of What events transpired that have allowed humans to communicating through a native language clearly speak, while animals remain silent? If we are to believe the separate humans from all animals. The obvious evolutionary teaching currently taking place in colleges and question then arises, where did we obtain this dis- universities around the world, speech evolved as a natural tinctive trait? Organic evolution has proven unable to process over time. Yet no-one is quite sure how, and there are no known animals that are in a transition phase from elucidate the origin of language and communication.
    [Show full text]
  • JOHN R. SKOYLES Neuroscience Researcher; Coauthor, up from Dragons
    JOHN R. SKOYLES Neuroscience researcher; Coauthor, Up From Dragons This essay appeared in “The Edge” Here's what I believe but cannot prove: human beings, like all animals, have evolved a range of capacities for fighting disease and recovering from injury, including a variety of 'sickness behaviors'; humans beings alone however have discovered the advantages of off-loading much of the responsibility for managing their sickness behaviors to other people; the result is that for human beings the very nature of illness has changed—human illness is now largely a social phenomenon. This is possible because "illness" is a response. A rise in body temperature, for example, kills many bacteria and changes the membrane properties of cells so viruses cannot replicate. The pain of a broken bone or weak heart makes sure we let it heal or rest. Nature supplied our bodies in this way with a first-aid kit but unfortunately like many medicines their "treatments" are unpleasant. That unpleasantness, not the dysfunction which they seek to remedy is what we call "illness". These remedies, however, have costs as well as benefits making it often difficult for the body to know whether to deploy them. A fever might fight an infection but if the body lacks sufficient energy stores, the fever might kill. The body therefore must make a decision whether the gain of clearing the infection merits the risk. Complicating that decision is that the body is blind, for example, to whether it faces a mild or a life-threatening virus. The body thus deploys its treatments in a precautionary manner.
    [Show full text]
  • The Course of Knowledge: a 21St Century Theory
    The Course of Knowledge A 21st Century Theory by Dr. Alex Bennet and Dr. David Bennet with Dr. Joyce Avedisian Mountain Quest Institute i The Course of Knowledge A 21st Century Theory by Dr. Alex Bennet Dr. David Bennet with Dr. Joyce Avedisian Mountain Quest Institute MQIPress (2015) Frost, West Virginia ISBN 978-0-9798459-6-3 ISBN 0-9798459-6-3 The Knowledge Series In this book we explore the course of knowledge. Just as a winding stream in the bowls of the mountains curves and dips through ravines and high valleys, so, too, with knowledge. In a continuous journey towards intelligent activity, context-sensitive and situation-dependent knowledge, imperfect and incomplete, experientially engages a changing landscape in a continuous cycle of learning and expanding. i Table of Contents Cover Title Page Table of Contents ..................................................... i Figures ...................................................... ii In Appreciation ...................................................... iv Foreward Section I: Laying the Foundation ..................................................... 1 Chapter 1: Introduction ..................................................... 3 Chapter 2: Levels of Knowledge ..................................................... 9 Chapter 3: Types of Knowledge ..................................................... 21 Section II: The Voiced and Unvoiced ...................................................... 27 Chapter 4: Explicit, Implicit and Tacit Dimensions .....................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Mirror Neurons and Colamus Humanitatem
    Why our brains cherish humanity: Mirror neurons and colamus humanitatem JOHN R. SKOYLES* Centre for Mathematics and Physics in the Life Sciences and Experimental Biology (CoMPLEX) University College London, London, UK Centre for Philosophy of Natural and Social Science (CNPSS) London School of Economics, London, UK Abstract Resumen Commonsense says we are isolated. After all, our El sentido común dice que estamos aislados. Des- bodies are physically separate. But Seneca’s cola- pués de todo, nuestros cuerpos están separados mus humanitatem, and John Donne’s observation físicamente. Pero la obra Colamus humanitatem that “no man is an island” suggests we are neither de Séneca y la observación de que “ningún hombre entirely isolated nor separate. A recent discovery in es una isla”, que hizo John Donne, sugieren que no neuroscience—that of mirror neurons—argues that estamos ni completamente aislados ni separados. the brain and the mind is neither built nor functions Un descubrimiento reciente de la neurociencia, el remote from what happens in other individuals. de las neuronas espejo, sostiene que el cerebro y What are mirror neurons? They are brain cells la mente no son construidos ni funcionan alejados that process both what happens to or is done by an de lo que pasa en otros individuos. ¿Qué son las individual, and, as it were, its perceived “refl ec- neuronas espejo? Son células cerebrales que pro- tion,” when that same thing happens or is done by cesan tanto lo que le pasa como lo que hace un in- another individual. Thus, mirror neurons are both dividuo, y, por así decirlo, su “refl exión” percibida activated when an individual does a particular ac- cuando esa misma cosa le pasa a, o es hecha por, tion, and when that individual perceives that same otro individuo.
    [Show full text]
  • Shifting Paradigms How the Popular Psychology Movement Is Challenging Rationality Sam Mcnerney 5/13/11
    1 Shifting Paradigms How the Popular Psychology Movement is Challenging Rationality Sam McNerney 5/13/11 2 What a piece of work is man! How noble in Reason! How infinite in faculties! -Hamlet, Act II, Scene II Ever since the ancient Greeks, these assumptions have revolved around a single theme: humans are rational… This simple idea underlies the philosophy of Plato and Descartes; it forms the foundation of modern economics; it drove decades of research in cognitive science. Over time, our rationality came to define us. It was, simply put, what made us human. There’s only one problem with this assumption of human rationality: it’s wrong. -Jonah Lehrer, How We Decide 3 Table of Contents 0) Introduction • What Can Be Called Into Doubt • The Rationality Paradigm • Explain consequences 1) Historical Context • Origins • Biases and Heuristics • The Unconscious, Emotion, and Intuition • Consequences to the Rationality Paradigm 2) The Five Themes of Popular Psychology • Cognitive Biases - Introduction - Framing - Anchoring - The Endowment Effect - Conclusion • Emotion and Reason - Introduction - The Necessity of Emotion - Emotion Constitutes Reason - Emotion & Morality - Conclusion • Intuitions - Introduction - Gilovich’s Hot Hand - “Random” ipod Shuffle & The Gambler’s Fallacy - Joshua Bell & The Invisible Gorilla: The Role of Context - Memory - Unconscious Thought - Professor Ratings & Thin-Slicing - Choking on Thought - Conclusion • Positive Psychology - Introduction - Buying Happiness - How to Spend Money - The Paradox of Choice - Environment
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature of Procrastination
    University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Haskayne School of Business Haskayne School of Business Research & Publications 2007 The nature of procrastination Steel, Piers American Psychological Association (APA) Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65-94. http://hdl.handle.net/1880/47914 journal article Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca The Nature of Procrastination 1 Running head: PROCRASTINATION The Nature of Procrastination: A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review of Quintessential Self- Regulatory Failure Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65-94. The Nature of Procrastination 2 Abstract Procrastination is a prevalent and pernicious form of self-regulatory failure that is not entirely understood. Hence, the relevant conceptual, theoretical, and empirical work is reviewed, drawing upon correlational, experimental, and qualitative findings. A meta-analysis of procrastination‟s possible causes and effects, based on 691 correlations, reveals that neuroticism, rebelliousness, and sensation seeking show only a weak connection. Strong and consistent predictors of procrastination were task aversiveness, task delay, self-efficacy, impulsiveness, as well as conscientiousness and its facets of self-control, distractibility, organization, and achievement motivation. These effects prove consistent with Temporal Motivation Theory, an integrative hybrid of expectancy theory and hyperbolic discounting. Continued research into procrastination should not be delayed, especially since its prevalence appears to be growing. Keywords: Procrastination, irrational delay, pathological decision-making, meta-analysis The Nature of Procrastination 3 The Nature of Procrastination: A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review of Quintessential Self- Regulatory Failure Procrastination is extremely prevalent. Although virtually all of us have at least dallied with dallying, some have made it a way of life.
    [Show full text]
  • Fission‐Fusion Dynamics: New Research Frameworks Author(S): Filippo Aureli, Colleen M
    Fission‐Fusion Dynamics: New Research Frameworks Author(s): Filippo Aureli, Colleen M. Schaffner, Christophe Boesch, Simon K. Bearder, Josep Call, Colin A. Chapman, Richard Connor, Anthony Di Fiore, Robin I. M. Dunbar, S. Peter Henzi, Kay Holekamp, Amanda H. Korstjens, Robert Layton, Phyllis Lee, Julia Lehmann, Joseph H. Manson, Gabriel Ramos‐Fernandez, Karen B. Strier, and Carel P. van Schaik Source: Current Anthropology, Vol. 49, No. 4 (August 2008), pp. 627-654 Published by: The University of Chicago Press on behalf of Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/586708 . Accessed: 12/11/2015 08:47 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press and Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Current Anthropology. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Thu, 12 Nov 2015 08:47:44 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Current Anthropology Volume 49, Number 4, 2008 627 Fission-Fusion Dynamics New Research Frameworks by Filippo Aureli, Colleen M. Schaffner, Christophe Boesch, Simon K.
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature of Procrastination 1
    The Nature of Procrastination 1 Running head: PROCRASTINATION The Nature of Procrastination Piers Steel University of Calgary Keywords: Procrastination, irrational delay, meta-analysis Piers Steel, Human Resources and Organizational Development, University of Calgary. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Piers Steel, 444 Skurfield Hall, 2500 University Drive N.W., University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada T2N 1N4, or [email protected], or Fax: 403-282-0095 I would like to sincerely thank Henri Schouwenburg for his enthusiasm in this endeavor as well as his willingness to share and translate his considerable research on procrastination. The Nature of Procrastination 2 Abstract Procrastination is prevalent and pernicious but not entirely understood, motivating this empirical and theoretical review. The review attempts to be exhaustive, drawing upon correlational, experimental, and qualitative findings. Summarizing 648 correlations, a meta-analysis of procrastination’s causes and effects reveals that neuroticism, rebelliousness, and sensation- seeking show only a weak connection. Strong and consistent predictors of procrastination were task aversiveness, task delay, self-efficacy, impulsiveness, as well as conscientiousness and its facets of self-control, distractibility, organization, and achievement motivation. These effects prove entirely consistent with a hybrid of two theories of choice behavior, expectancy and hyperbolic discounting. Continued research into the prediction and treatment
    [Show full text]
  • Metab and Human Evolution080505
    Skoyles 1 Human metabolic adaptations and prolonged expensive neurodevelopment: A review John R. Skoyles Centre for Mathematics and Physics in the Life Sciences and Experimental Biology (CoMPLEX), University College London, London, NW1 2HE, UK. and Centre for Philosophy of Natural and Social Science, (CPNSS) London School of Economics, London WC2A 2AE, UK. Nature Precedings : hdl:10101/npre.2008.1856.2 Posted 30 Oct 2008 METABOLIC ADAPTATIONS Skoyles 2 ABSTRACT 1. After weaning, human hunter-gatherer juveniles receive substantial ( ≈3.5-7 MJ day -1), extended ( ≈15 years) and reliable (kin and nonkin food pooling) energy provision. 2. The childhood (pediatric) and the adult human brain takes a very high share of both basal metabolic rate (BMR) (child: 50-70%; adult: ≈20%) and total energy expenditure (TEE) (child: 30-50%; adult: ≈10%). 3. The pediatric brain for an extended period ( ≈4-9 years-of-age) consumes roughly 50% more energy than the adult one, and after this, continues during adolescence, at a high but declining rate. Within the brain, childhood cerebral gray matter has an even higher 1.9 to 2.2-fold increased energy consumption. 4. This metabolic expensiveness is due to (i) the high cost of synapse activation (74% of brain energy expenditure in humans), combined with (ii), a prolonged period of exuberance in synapse numbers (up to double the number present in adults). Cognitive development during this period associates with volumetric changes in gray matter (expansion and contraction due to metabolic related size alterations in glial cells and capillary vascularization), and in white matter (expansion due to myelination).
    [Show full text]