Current Directions in Psychological Science

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Current Directions in Psychological Science Current Directions in Psychological Science http://cdp.sagepub.com/ What Are Emotion Expressions For? Azim F. Shariff and Jessica L. Tracy Current Directions in Psychological Science 2011 20: 395 DOI: 10.1177/0963721411424739 The online version of this article can be found at: http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/20/6/395 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Association for Psychological Science Additional services and information for Current Directions in Psychological Science can be found at: Email Alerts: http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav >> Version of Record - Dec 5, 2011 What is This? Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com by Jessica Tracy on December 8, 2011 Current Directions in Psychological Science What Are Emotion Expressions For? 20(6) 395 –399 © The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721411424739 Azim F. Shariff1 and Jessica L. Tracy2 http://cdps.sagepub.com 1University of Oregon and 2University of British Columbia Abstract Although research on the nonverbal expression of emotion has played a prominent role throughout psychology during the past two decades—including an instrumental role in the development of contemporary evolutionary psychology—little research has focused on the evolutionary origins and functions of the emotional expressions themselves. However, recent findings from psychophysical, comparative, social, and cross-cultural psychology are converging to produce a compelling functionalist account, suggesting that emotional expressions serve critical adaptive purposes. Most of these studies have narrowly focused on single emotions—an approach that has been very useful for providing new insights about specific expressions but not for developing a broader understanding of why humans universally display and recognize distinct emotions. Here we unify these disparate findings in order to illuminate this fundamental form of social communication. Keywords emotion expressions, nonverbal displays, evolutionary psychology, adaptation, signal Darwin’s (1872) The Expression of Emotions in Man and Ani- evolution-informed psychology addressing the ultimate ori- mals (EEMA) began as half of a single chapter devoted to gins and functions of psychological phenomena. Within emo- humankind in his massive manuscript on the evolution of tion research, these findings paved the way for new lines of plants and animals. Ultimately that manuscript expanded into research addressing questions about the functions these four books, beginning with On the Origin of Species in 1859 expressions may have evolved to serve. That is, having estab- and concluding with EEMA in 1872. Despite being the last lished that certain emotion expressions are universally recog- chapter of this groundbreaking series, EEMA marked the first nized, this “second chapter” prompted the question, “Why?” chapter in a long-standing naturalist investigation into nonver- bal expressions of emotions.1 In it, Darwin broke with estab- lished perspectives, controversially proposing innate, evolved, The Third Chapter: Evolved Functions of and survival-related functions for features of emotion expres- Emotion Expressions sions, which, he argued, are rooted in our shared evolutionary That question is now being addressed by several streams of heritage with other animals. The theoretical depth and testable research that are coalescing into what could be considered the hypotheses laid out in EEMA cemented Darwin’s role not just third chapter in this long history. Darwin (1872) proposed that as a progenitor of contemporary emotion-expression research emotion expressions evolved to serve two classes of functions: but also as the first evolutionary psychologist. (a) preparing the organism to respond adaptively to environ- If EEMA is the “first chapter” of research on the evolution mentally recurrent stimuli and (b) communicating critical social of emotion expressions, one could consider the second chapter information. Subsequent researchers (e.g., Chapman, Kim, to be the vast 1960s–1970s cross-cultural exploration of emo- Susskind, & Anderson, 2009; Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Ekman, tion recognition led by Ekman, Izard, and colleagues. These 1992) further developed this account, arguing that internal phys- researchers conducted the first major empirical test of Dar- iological regulation was likely the original adaptive function of win’s hypotheses by examining whether individuals from dis- emotion expressions, which later evolved to serve communica- parate cultures could reliably identify the emotions conveyed tive functions. Here we review emerging evidence for this “two- by certain expressions (see Ekman, 1992). Their discovery, stage model” of emotion-expression evolution. that a handful of emotions are cross-culturally recognized, was a major breakthrough in research on psychological universals. Corresponding Author: Indeed, cited as some of the strongest evidence supporting Azim Shariff, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, 1227 an underlying “human nature,” Ekman’s and Izard’s findings University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403 laid critical groundwork for the eventual development of an E-mail: [email protected] Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com by Jessica Tracy on December 8, 2011 396 Shariff, Tracy Adaptation: Emotion expressions for Exaptation: Emotion expressions for social physiological regulation communication From a functionalist perspective, emotions are generalized Evolutionary biologists make an important distinction between and (theoretically) coordinated suites of behavioral, physio- cues and signals. A cue provides information gleaned as a by- logical, cognitive, and affective processes selected to promote product of something that serves an alternate adaptive pur- automatic, adaptive responses to recurrent environmental pose; for example, chewing is a reliable cue that someone is events that pose fitness challenges. eating, but it did not evolve to communicate that information. Fear provides a useful illustration. Detection of potentially On the other hand, signals evolved specifically for the purpose threatening stimuli elicits a cascade of responses including of communication; for example, peacock plumage evolved as heavier breathing, the redistribution of blood in preparation a hard-to-fake signal of mate quality (Hasson, 1997). In the for rapid movement, and a marshaling of attentive resources to two-stage model, it is hypothesized that emotion expressions promote hypervigilance. These responses facilitate the ani- began as cues—providing information about internal states mal’s ability to escape a predator or other threat. From the but not existing for that reason—but eventually transformed, Darwinian perspective, the facial muscle movements that in both form and function, to become signals. In other words, together constitute a fear expression originally emerged as part in the course of evolutionary history, the function of expres- of this adaptive behavioral “macro.” sions itself evolved. Over time, as recognizing the internal Indeed, recent studies by Anderson and colleagues support states of other animals yielded fitness-positive consequences, this suggestion. The widened eyes of individuals instructed to the facial and bodily behavioral components of certain emo- pose a fearful facial expression were found to increase the tions came to cue those emotional states to observers. As social scope of their visual field and the speed of their eye move- interaction became more possible and even vital for many spe- ments, allowing expressers to better identify (potentially cies, the adaptive value of these expressions may have shifted threatening) objects in their periphery (Susskind et al., 2008). toward communication. As a result, the nonverbal behaviors Components of the fear expression thus may be as much a part associated with distinct emotions likely underwent ritualiza- of the adaptive emotional response as the frightened affect and tion: a process of change well researched in evolutionary zool- quickened heart rate. ogy whereby an animal’s nonverbal displays become Other expressions function similarly. The prototypic dis- exaggerated, more visible, distinctive, and/or prototypic in gust expression, characterized by a scrunched nose and order to function as reliable and effective signals (Eibl- mouth, results in constriction of these orifices, thereby reduc- Eibesfeldt, 1989).2 For emotion expressions, this shift from cue ing air intake (Chapman et al., 2009). Since the primary to signal can be thought of as their second stage of evolution—a adaptive functions of disgust are to alert an organism to, and paradigmatic example of exaptation, the common evolutionary protect an organism from, potentially noxious stimuli, the process whereby a feature that evolved for one reason gradually disgust expression’s feature of reducing the inhalation of air- morphs to serve a secondary adaptive function. borne particles can be seen as a part of the larger adaptive As a result of ritualization, emotion expressions have disgust response. become the highly recognizable displays that characterize These novel findings and ongoing follow-up work are daily life. Indeed, the ability to quickly and accurately recog- revealing the original functional legacy of emotion expres- nize these expressions appears to be a human universal and
Recommended publications
  • Sander-Alphah 203..208
    sander & Scherer Sander-AlphaH Page Proof page 203 28.2.2009 10:08am H happiness The emotion of happiness is a subjective, for optimism. Recent research has found that an individ- valenced reaction to a positive experience or event (Ort- ual’s experiences of happiness can be significantly bol- ony et al. 1988). Happiness can be conceptualized as an stered by the regular, committed practice of activities umbrella term that encompasses a variety of positive such as counting blessings, expressing optimism, and per- feelings, ranging from the low-intensity states of *con- forming acts of kindness (Lyubomirsky et al. 2005b). In tentment, *enjoyment, serenity, and *amusement to the sum, frequent happiness is a highly valued goal that is high-intensity states of elation, *joy, and euphoria. partially a function of *temperament, but can also be These positive states are typically experienced when a attained through effortful intentional activity. person is making progress towards the realization of SONJA LYUBOMIRSKY AND JAIME L. KURTZ *goals (Carver and Scheier 1998), and, notably, signal that the environment is benign and safe for both relax- hatred Hatred, the noun, and to hate, the verb, do not ation and exploration (Schwarz and Clore 1983). As such, completely coincide in their semantic ranges. Hatred researchers had originally conceptualized a happy emo- carries with it more intensity and greater seriousness tion as producing a form of ‘free activation’ that is than many of our most common uses of the verb. conducive to creativity and divergent thought (Frijda Hatred is unlikely to apply aptly to one’s feelings about 1986, Isen et al.
    [Show full text]
  • The Brain Basis of Emotion: a Meta-Analytic Review
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2012) 35, 121–202 doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000446 The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review Kristen A. Lindquist Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School/Massachusetts General Hospital/Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129, and Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138 [email protected] http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/lindqukr/ Tor D. Wager Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309 [email protected] http://www.psych.colorado.edu/tor/ Hedy Kober Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519 [email protected] http://medicine.yale.edu/psychiatry/people/hedy_kober.profile Eliza Bliss-Moreau California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] http://www.elizablissmoreau.com/EBM/home.html Lisa Feldman Barrett Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, and Departments of Radiology and Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School/ Massachusetts General Hospital/Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129 [email protected] http://www.affective-science.org/ Abstract: Researchers have wondered how the brain creates emotions since the early days of psychological science. With a surge of studies in affective neuroscience in recent decades, scientists are poised to answer this question. In this target article, we present a meta-analytic summary of the neuroimaging literature on human emotion. We compare the locationist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories consistently and specifically correspond to distinct brain regions) with the psychological constructionist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories are constructed of more general brain networks not specific to those categories) to better understand the brain basis of emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • The-Embodiment-Of-Emotion-Aghu.Pdf
    1 CN Chapter 10 CT The Embodiment of Emotion Lisa Feldman Barrett and Kristen A. Lindquist Boston College To appear in G.R. Semin and E.R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press. 2 In current psychological discourse, it is fashionable to talk about emotions as “embodied” phenomena. At first glance, this idea is not novel. Historically, almost all psychological theories of emotion have proposed that emotional reactions are constituted by the body in some fashion. Some suggest that changes in the body cause changes in the mind; others suggest the opposite, or that the body and mind interact to produce an emotional response. Amid theoretical differences, these theories use the common metaphor that the body and mind are separate and independent forces that can act upon one another in an emotional episode. Current embodiment theories of the mind challenge this assumption by suggesting that the body helps to constitute the mind in shaping an emotional response. This view has novel implications for understanding the structure and content of the conceptual system for emotion, as well as for defining what emotions are and how they are caused. In the present chapter, we explore a more modern embodiment view of emotion. First, we discuss how the Cartesian “machine metaphor” underlies much theorizing about emotion, as we situate an embodied view of emotion in its historical context. Our historical review is not intended to be comprehensive but rather to illustrate how emotion theories to date have conceptualized the role of the body and mind in emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • Unconscious Emotion
    (9)") (t-,r^r) 1,,R .9-L^*. 1\ L'x)J-"1 t UnconsciousEmotio7n9 7 AJed--qJ"-lq-t'L tf L*'nh"-' |3 C-7,e"'< Emotions are meansd esignedt o regulateb ehaviori n relation to agendass et 0U''<-u'> f r-aa ^fr> Yt-lL " D<A'-J by biological evolution.T hus, emotion pervadedt he critical ecologicalp roblems L that our distanta ncestorsh ad to solve if their genesw ere to be representeidn the UnconsciousE motion: nextg enerationT. hesep roblemsin cludedf indinga nd consumingfo od andd rink, finding shelterss, eekingp rotectiona nd supportf rom conspecificsa, ssertingo neself Evoul tionaryP erspectives, socially, satisfyingc uriosity,g etringa ccesst o and engagingw ith sexualp artners, caring for offspring,a nd avoiding and escapingli fe-threateninge vents.T hesea re PsychophysiologicDaal ta,a nd all activitiess tructuredb y emotions( seeT ooby & Cosmides,1 990).I n a biological perspectivet,h erefore,e motionsc an be understooda s clever meanss hapedb y evo- NeuropsychologicMale chanisms lution to make us want to do what our ancestorhsa d to do successfulltyo pass geneso n to comingg eneration(se .g.,O hman,1 993a,1 996). ConceptuaIlm plications A R N E O H M A N , A N D E R SF L Y K T , A N D D A N I E L I - U N D Q V I S T The evolutionary-functionapl erspectiveo n the psychologyo f emotion shifts the emphasisf rom the unique phenomenologyo f human feeling to action tendencies and responsep attemst hat we sharew ith fellow inhabitantso f the animalk ingdom, Rathert han conceptualizinge motiona s a centralf eelings tatem ore or lessi mper- fectly
    [Show full text]
  • A Framework for Digital Emotions Meghan Rosatelli Virginia Commonwealth University
    Virginia Commonwealth University VCU Scholars Compass Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2011 A Framework for Digital Emotions Meghan Rosatelli Virginia Commonwealth University Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd Part of the Interdisciplinary Arts and Media Commons © The Author Downloaded from http://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/239 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at VCU Scholars Compass. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of VCU Scholars Compass. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Copyright © 2011 by Meghan Rosatelli All rights reserved A Framework for Digital Emotions A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at Virginia Commonwealth University by Meghan Elizabeth Rosatelli Bachelor of Arts, University of Colorado at Boulder, 2004 Master of Arts, Virginia Commonwealth University, 2007 Director: Dr. Richard Fine Professor, Department of English Virginia Commonwealth University Richmond, Virginia August 2011 TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES……………………………………………………………..……………..…iii LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS…………………………………………………………………….iv ABSTRACT……………………………………………………………………………………….v INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………………………...……1 PART 1. A FRAMEWORK FOR DIGITAL EMOTIONS……….…………………….……….22 Chapter 1. Emotions are Fickle Things………………………………………………………..…23 Chapter 2. Emotions Put the “New” in “New Media”……………………………………….…..61
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Emotion Semantics
    To appear in Cognition. This preprint may contain errors not present in the published version. Evolution of emotion semantics Aotao Xua,∗, Jennifer E. Stellarb, Yang Xua,c aDepartment of Computer Science, University of Toronto bDepartment of Psychology, University of Toronto cCognitive Science Program, University of Toronto Abstract Humans possess the unique ability to communicate emotions through language. Although concepts like anger or awe are abstract, there is a shared consensus about what these English emotion words mean. This consensus may give the impression that their meaning is static, but we propose this is not the case. We cannot travel back to earlier periods to study emotion concepts directly, but we can examine text corpora, which have partially preserved the mean- ing of emotion words. Using natural language processing of historical text, we found evidence for semantic change in emotion words over the past century and that varying rates of change were predicted in part by an emotion concept's prototypicality|how representative it is of the broader category of \emotion". Prototypicality negatively correlated with historical rates of emotion semantic change obtained from text-based word embeddings, beyond more established variables including usage frequency in English and a second comparison lan- guage, French. This effect for prototypicality did not consistently extend to the semantic category of birds, suggesting its relevance for predicting seman- tic change may be category-dependent. Our results suggest emotion semantics arXiv:2108.02887v1 [cs.CL] 5 Aug 2021 are evolving over time, with prototypical emotion words remaining semantically stable, while other emotion words evolve more freely. Keywords: emotion; semantic change; semantic stability; prototype theory; word embedding ∗Corresponding author Email address: [email protected] (Aotao Xu) Preprint submitted to Cognition August 9, 2021 1 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Emotion a Natural Kind?*
    Is Emotion a Natural Kind?* Paul E. Griffiths, Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15232, USA [email protected] 7280wds plus references 1. Introduction In What Emotions Really Are: The problem of psychological categories (Griffiths, 1997) I argued that it is unlikely that all the psychological states and processes that fall under the vernacular category of emotion are sufficiently similar to one another to allow a unified scientific psychology of the emotions. The psychological, neuroscientific and biological theories that best explain any particular subset of human emotions will not adequately explain all human emotions. In a slogan, emotions are not a natural kind (pp. 14-17; 241-247)i. I argued that the same is probably true of many specific emotion categories, such as anger and love (p. 17). On some occasions when a person is properly said to be angry, certain psychological, neuroscientific and biological theories will adequately explain what is happening to that person. On other occasions of anger, however, different theories will be needed. I described my position as eliminitivism about emotion, because it implies that the term ‘emotion’ and some specific emotion terms like ‘anger’ are examples of what philosophers of language have called ‘partial reference’ (p. 242). The term ‘jade’ is the classic example of partial reference. The term ‘jade’ is used as if it referred to a particular kind of mineral, in the same manner as ‘malachite’ or ‘diamond’. In reality, however, the term covers two different stones, jadeite or nephrite. The term ‘jade’ partially refers to each of these two minerals.
    [Show full text]
  • The Problem of Privacy in the Evolution of Emotion and Emotional
    The Problem of Privacy and the Evolution of Emotion and Emotional Behavior T. V. Joe Layng, Ph.D. I will begin by retelling a story I that first heard from Israel Goldiamond. I have always wanted a bear skin. You can imagine how happy I was when I finally had the opportunity to hunt for one. For hours I tracked a bear through the woods and finally, there it was, my dream was about to come true. I carefully raised my rifle taking aim at the bear. At the same time, the bear saw me. It raised up on two legs, and growled a very menacing growl. I didn’t care, I was elated. My dream, my quest was to be realized. My heart pounded with excitement as I took aim, and then in a culminating act, pulled the trigger. Click. Click was all I heard. No gunshot. I tried again. Click. The rifle had apparently jammed. Fear quickly spread through my body. The bear was raised as before and my rifle was still aimed at the bear. But something had changed. Instead of a nice new bear rug in the living room, the bear was about to get a nice new human skin for its den. That is, the consequences had changed, or, more precisely, there had been a change in consequential contingencies. Same stimulus, same behavior, massively different consequence. The swift change from elation to fear tracked the swift change in contingencies. I began to run. Was I running because I was afraid? Was I afraid because I was running? No.
    [Show full text]
  • The Brain Basis of Emotion: a Meta-Analytic Review
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2012) 35, 121–202 doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000446 The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review Kristen A. Lindquist Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School/Massachusetts General Hospital/Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129, and Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138 [email protected] http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/lindqukr/ Tor D. Wager Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309 [email protected] http://www.psych.colorado.edu/tor/ Hedy Kober Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519 [email protected] http://medicine.yale.edu/psychiatry/people/hedy_kober.profile Eliza Bliss-Moreau California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] http://www.elizablissmoreau.com/EBM/home.html Lisa Feldman Barrett Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, and Departments of Radiology and Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School/ Massachusetts General Hospital/Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129 [email protected] http://www.affective-science.org/ Abstract: Researchers have wondered how the brain creates emotions since the early days of psychological science. With a surge of studies in affective neuroscience in recent decades, scientists are poised to answer this question. In this target article, we present a meta-analytic summary of the neuroimaging literature on human emotion. We compare the locationist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories consistently and specifically correspond to distinct brain regions) with the psychological constructionist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories are constructed of more general brain networks not specific to those categories) to better understand the brain basis of emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion and the Affective Sciences
    The Oxford Companion - TO- Emotion and the Affective Sciences EDITED BY DAVID SANDER AND KLAUS R. SCHERER OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS eustress continues to evolve, we may learn we have new powers entails a cognitive judgement as to desirability, value, or or limitations we did not previously know of, which is significance; an affective reaction or disposition along sure to give rise to new ethical issues. the dimensions of reward/aversion, pleasure/pain, and LOUIS C. CHARLAND goodness/badness; and a behavioural component along the dimension of approach/ avoidance (see APPROACH/ eustress The concept of eustress was introduced by WITHDRAWAL). Evaluative processes are multiple, extend­ Selye (1964) to refer to the ensemble of positive adaptive ing from genetically endowed approach/avoidance re­ reactions of the organism to beneficial stressors, as op­ actions to positive and negative stimuli, respectively. posed to *distress, which he viewed as a specific syn­ Examples include positive approach I ingestion responses drome that was triggered by unspecific harmful stimuli to palatable flavours and the negative avoidance I with­ or activities (general adaptation syndrome). For Selye, drawal reactions to pain stimuli. Other evaluative eustress represents the pleasant *stress of fulfilment, judgements may be acquired and based on mid-level without the harmful consequences of damaging distress. · associative processes and higher-level cognitive pro­ This concept includes properties of the stressor (benefi­ cesses. Examples include the positive rewarding effects cial stressor), the effort (positive valence), and the effects of money (which may have no intrinsic value, in and of (without damaging outcomes). Stressors are seen as itself ) or fear, anxiety, or avoidance reactions to things beneficial when they do not exceed the capacity for like snakes or the sound of a dentist's drill.
    [Show full text]
  • Of Emotion Tancies and Be Externalized As Social Sensu Strictu Is Restricted
    7~~~ ___________________________________ BOOKREVIEVVS __________________ N_A_TU__ R_E_V_O_L_.3_2_3~ __ O_CT __ OB_E_R_l_~_6 vides a new feeling system that coincides whose evolutionary origin and brain struc­ Great expectancies with the evolution of mammals; and (3) ture are quite different from those of these feelings can in turn modify expec­ mammals, to whom, apparently, emotion of emotion tancies and be externalized as social sensu strictu is restricted. Eric A. Sa/zen signals of emotional states. Students might find Parts I-III of this Livesey thus effectively matches his book valuable in appreciating the theore­ earlier definition of emotion, no doubt tical propositions of Part IV. The special­ Learning and Emotion: A Biological to his satisfaction and pleasure. His thesis ist reader, however, might have preferred Synthesis. Volume 1, Evolutionary Pro­ implies that the evolution of emotion de­ the relevant facts to have been referenced cesses. By Peter J. Livesey. Lawrence pended on the evolution of learned expec­ and integrated into the presentation of the Erlbaum:i986. Pp.312. $32.50, £22. tancies. He also proposes that the function theory. In Vol. 2 of the work (which, I'm of emotion is to feed back and influence told, will not be published until 1988 or EMOTION has been variously seen as expectancies and so act as a "teaching so), Livesey proposes to identify the providing emergency support responses, mechanism". If emotion guides learning neural systems for directing attention to energizing and response amplification, as Livesey implies, one wonders what relevant stimuli and for organizing appro­ motivation through incentive and re­ guided Oscar Wilde when, in The Picture priate behaviour on subsequent encoun­ inforcement, and adaptive instinctive of Dorian Gray, he wrote: "The advan­ ters, with special reference to the role of responses, or as being non-functional tage of the emotions is that they lead us the limbic system in the mediation of this response disorganization.
    [Show full text]
  • On Statistical Analysis of the Pattern of Evolution of Perceived Emotions Induced by Hindustani Music – Study Based on Listener Responses
    On Statistical Analysis of the Pattern of Evolution of Perceived Emotions Induced by Hindustani Music – Study Based on Listener Responses Vishal Midya1, Sneha Chakraborty1, Srijita Manna1, Ranjan Sengupta2 1 Indian Statistical Institute, Delhi, India 2C.V.Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, India The objective of this study is to find the underlying pattern of how perception of emotions has evolved in India. Here Hindustani Music has been used as a reference frame for tracking the changing perception of emotions. It has been found that different emotions perceived from Hindustani Music form a particular sequential pattern when their corresponding pitch periods are analyzed using the standard deviations (SD) and mean successive squared differences (MSSD). This sequential pattern of emotions coincides with their corresponding sequential pattern of tempos or average number of steady states. On the basis of this result we further found that the range of perception of emotions has diminished significantly these days compared to what it was before. The proportion of responses for the perceived emotions like Anger, Serenity, Romantic and Sorrow has also decreased to a great extent than what it was previously. The proportion of responses for the perceived emotion ‘Anxiety’ has increased phenomenally. Both standard deviation (SD) and mean successive squared difference (MSSD) are two very good measures in tracking the changing perception of emotions. The overall pattern of the change of perceived emotions has corresponded to the psychological and sociological change of human life. INTRODUCTION The evolution of emotion has been copiously addressed in recent times. Unlike most other stimuli such as smell, taste or facial expression, that evoke emotion, and can be linked with biological or survival value, Music has an intriguing ability to evoke powerful emotions without having such an intrinsic link.
    [Show full text]