Ergativity in Kurdish Language (PP

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ergativity in Kurdish Language (PP طؤظارى زانكؤ بؤ زانستة مرؤظايةتييةكان بةرطى. 91 ، ذمارة.6، سالَى 5192 Ergativity in Kurdish Language (PP. 143-160) Assistant Prof. Dr. Ali M. Jukil English Dept. College of Basic Education-Salahaddin University- Hawler (SUH) Received: 16 /02/2015 Accepted: 05 /05/2015 Abstract This study deals with the ergativity in Kurdish Language, since the ergativity is a case system and within a case system, languages can be classified into "accusative" "ergative" and "active" according to how they mark the core arguments in a clause. The objective of this study to find out the phenomenon of ergativity status as a case system in the Kurdish language. First of all, the study presents transitivity in Kurdish language, and then it explains case systems in which it identifies ergative, accusative and active languages. After this the study illustrates case marking which is divided into three types; case endings, markedness of pronouns and markedness of clitics which are the core topics to examine Kurdish 'language, particularly the Hawler subdialect comparing with sulemani subdialect and Northern Kurmanji (NK) dialect to indicate split-ergativity and clitic movement in Hawler subdialect. Then the study presents the criteria to test ergativity such as Equi-deletion, raising rules and causative. At the end it presents the conclusion and bibliography. 1-Introduction his study deals with the ergativity in Kurdish Language, since the ergativity is a case system and within a case system, languages can be classified into "accusative" T "ergative" and "active" according to how they mark the core arguments in a clause. The term "ergativity" as a case system used to identify the grammatical pattern in which the subject of a transitive clause is different from the intransitive subject which is treated in the same way as the object of a transitive clause. Ergative refers to case marking of a noun phrase which is the subject of transitive. It is the case which is contrasting with another case called "nominative" but at present "absolutive" in which the subject of the intransitive verb and the "object" of the transitive verbs are in the same case, the nominative or the absolutive, while the subject of the transitive verb is in ergative case. The whole points about the ergative languages can only be interpreted by reference to the transitive verbs. First of all, the study presents transitivity in Kurdish language, and then it explains case systems in which it identifies ergative, accusative and active languages. After this the study illustrates case marking which is divided into three types; case endings, markedness of pronouns and markedness of clitics which are the core topics to examine Kurdish 'language, particularly the Hawler subdialect comparing with Sulemani subdialect and Northern Kurmanji (NK) dialect to indicate split-ergativity and clitic movement in Hawler subdialect. Then the study presents the criteria to test ergativity such as Equi-deletion or Big PRO, raising rules and causative. At the end it presents the conclusion and bibliography. 11. Transitivity This section presents the relationship between NPs and verb within the clause. All languages have intransitive and transitive clauses. In transitive, a verb is one place verb which requires only one NP (argument) i.e., intransitive clause involves with a verb and just on NP participant. e.g., darmé, damri, daçi, darwa, .... as in 143 Vol.19, No.6, 2015 طؤظارى زانكؤ بؤ زانستة مرؤظايةتييةكان بةرطى. 91 ، ذمارة.6، سالَى 5192 1) diwaraka darmé. (intransitive) wall the T collapse. S V The wall is collapsing. 2) Piyâwaka damri. (intransitive) man the T die. S V The man is dying. 3) ?aw daçit. ( intransitive) He T go he. S V He is going. 4) Qutabyaka darwât. ( intransitive) Student the T walk he. S V The student walks (is walking). While a transitive verb is a two-place verb, here the verb requires two NPs (arguments) i.e., transitive clause involves with a verb and two NP participants, one of the NPs fills the place of the subject and the other the place of the object. e.g., 5) damréni, kuşt, danwéni, darzéni darmând, damrând, danwând, darzând. 6) Kabra diwarak darméni. (transitive) Man wall the is collapsing / falling down. The man is falling down the wall. 7) Kabra diwarakay darmand. (transitive) Man wall the was collapsing/ falling down. The man was falling down the wall. 8) Polisaka piyawkujekay kuşt . ( transitive) Police the murdered the killed. The police killed the murdered. 9) Źnaka mindalakay danwěni. ( transitive) Woaman the child the her is sleeping. The woman made her child to be slept. 10) Źnaka mindalakay danwand. (transitive) Woman the child the her was sleeping. The woman makes her child to be slept. 11) Kiçaka çayaka darženi . ( transitive) Girl the tea the is spilling. The girl is spilling the tea. 144 Vol.19, No.6, 2015 طؤظارى زانكؤ بؤ زانستة مرؤظايةتييةكان بةرطى. 91 ، ذمارة.6، سالَى 5192 12) Kiçaka çayaka daržand. (transitive) Girl the tea the was spilling. The girl was spilling the tea. In Kurdish most verbs are inherently transitive or intransitive for example' rôyiştin "to go" ; kawtin "to fall" hâtin "to come" ?âxâftin "to speak", çun "to go", ... etc are inherently intransitive, while verbs like girtin "to seize" birdin "to take" dîtin "to see" dân "to give" are inherently transitive. 13) ?aw rôyişt. (intransitive) He went. 14) ?aw kawt. (intransitive) S/he fell S/he fell. 15) ?aw hât. (intransitive) S/he came. S/he came. 16) ?aw di?axeft / dadwet. (intransitive) S/he T speak/ T speak. S/he is speaking. 17) ?aw çu:wa bâzâr. (intransitive) S/he went to bazaar. S/he went to bazar. 18) Policaka dizakay girt. (transitive) Police the thief the s/he seized. The police arrested the thief. 19) Qutabiyaka kitébi romanakay bird. (transitive) Student the book novel the took. The student took the novel book. 20) ?âmin mindâlakanim dît. (transitive) I children the saw. I saw the children. 21) ?amin kitébakânim ba ?ali da. (transitive) I book the pl. to Ali gave. I gave the books to Ali. In Kurdish verbs may be used transitively and these verbs take verbal affix en / ând which is a very productive suffix for deriving transitive verbs which can be considered ditranstive or complex transitive verbs because each of these verbs can select the number of internal arguments and also they can be turned into causative verbs. e.g., 22) Diwâraka darûxé. present tense( intransitive) 23)Piyawaka diwarakay ruxand. (transitive) (causative) 24) Diwâraka rûxa. past tense( intransitive) 25) Piyâwaka diwarakay ruxand. (transitive) ( causative) (Jukil,2000;67-87). In Kurdish it can be distinguished between intransitive clause such as (24) and passive clause below since the verbs in the passivized clauses are inflected by the suffix ra/ ré; e.g., 145 Vol.19, No.6, 2015 طؤظارى زانكؤ بؤ زانستة مرؤظايةتييةكان بةرطى. 91 ، ذمارة.6، سالَى 5192 26)Diwaraka daruxéndré. (present tense) The previous presentation indicates that Kurdish distinguishes between clauses with a verb and just one core argument and those clauses with a verb and two core arguments in addition to ditransitive clauses a subtype of transitive clauses; i.e., Kurdish works in terms of three primitive relations (Dixon 2002 ;6) 1- NP core argument as an intransitive subject (S). e.g.; 27) ?âzâd hat, past tense intransitive Azad came. 28) ?âzâd dét, present tense intransitive Azad is coming. 29) Xânwaka darmét. present tense intransitive The house if falling down. 30) Xânwaka rima. past tense intransitive The house was falling down. 2- NP or core argument as transitive subject (A) agent. e.g.; 31) ?âzâd Xânwaki kirî. Past tense transitive Azad bought a house. 32) ?âzad Xânwak dakrét. present tense transitive Azad will buy a house. 33)? âzâd Xânwakay ruxând. Past tense transitive Azad collapsed the house 34)? Âzâd Xânwakay daruxénét. Present tense transitive Azad is collapsing the house 3- NP as transitive object (O) or (P). 35) ?âzâd Xânwakay bajéheşt / bajehéla. Azad left the house. 36) ?âzâd Xânwaka bajédelét. Azad will leave the house. The above examples indicate that (S) "subject" is a mere argument of an intransitive verb; (O) is clearly object and (A) is for agent. The prototypical semantic role is taken by the subject of a transitive verb, and the transitive subject "A" can be distinguished from object (0) or (P); "?âzâd" and xânwaka (house) respectively. (Tallerman 1998; 152). 1II.0. Case System This section presents the major case systems because within the case system, languages are classified as "accusative", "ergative" and "active" according to how they mark the core arguments in a clause. Subject which is a universal category links functions from intransitive and transitive clause type; and the relationship between S, A and O are the basic universal syntactic relations. This relation is entirely natural in accusative language to case-mark 146 Vol.19, No.6, 2015 طؤظارى زانكؤ بؤ زانستة مرؤظايةتييةكان بةرطى. 91 ، ذمارة.6، سالَى 5192 subject (S) and (A) with one single case which is nominative, whereas the (O) , the argument of a transitive verb is marked with accusative, and this can be presented as; (Dixon 2002;6). Nominative; A S Accusative; O While ergative languages show an ergative -obsolutive case system in which (S) and (O) are assigned the same case, called absolutive, whereas (A) is an ergative case as in; Absolutive : S O Ergative: A While in active languages (A) and (O) in the transitive clause might be marked as nominative vs accusative, whereas in the transitive clause the verb determines whether the S argument patterns with (A) or with (O). The languages are also called split languages in which intransitive verbs are divided into two groups; active and state verbs. The subject of the active verb is grouped with the agent (A) of a transitive verb which has an ergative case, while the subject of the state verb is grouped with (O) in the transitive construction, and an accusative language.(Barwari,2004;84).
Recommended publications
  • The Strategy of Case-Marking
    Case marking strategies Helen de Hoop & Andrej Malchukov1 Radboud University Nijmegen DRAFT January 2006 Abstract Two strategies of case marking in natural languages are discussed. These are defined as two violable constraints whose effects are shown to converge in the case of differential object marking but diverge in the case of differential subject marking. The strength of the case bearing arguments will be shown to be of utmost importance for case marking as well as voice alternations. The strength of arguments can be viewed as a function of their discourse prominence. The analysis of the case marking patterns we find cross-linguistically is couched in a bidirectional OT analysis. 1. Assumptions In this section we wish to put forward our three basic assumptions: (1) In ergative-absolutive systems ergative case is assigned to the first argument x of a two-place relation R(x,y). (2) In nominative-accusative systems accusative case is assigned to the second argument y of a two-place relation R(x,y). (3) Morphologically unmarked case can be the absence of case. The first two assumptions deal with the linking between the first (highest) and second (lowest) argument in a transitive sentence and the type of case marking. For reasons of convenience, we will refer to these arguments quite sloppily as the subject and the object respectively, although we are aware of the fact that the labels subject and object may not be appropriate in all contexts, dependent on how they are actually defined. In many languages, ergative and accusative case are assigned only or mainly in transitive sentences, while in intransitive sentences ergative and accusative case are usually not assigned.
    [Show full text]
  • Ergative As Accusative Case: Evidence from Adiyaman
    ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI ÜMİT ATLAMAZ ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! BOĞAZİÇİ UNIVERSITY 2012 ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI Thesis submitted to the Institute for Graduate Studies in the Social Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Masters of Arts in Linguistics by Ümit Atlamaz ! ! Boğaziçi University 2012 ! ! Thesis Abstract Ümit Atlamaz, “Ergative as Accusative Case: Evidence from Adıyaman Kurmanji” This study aims to investigate the nature of ergativity in Adıyaman Kurmanji within the premises of the Minimalist Program. Adıyaman Kurmanji displays two alignment patterns depending on the tense. In non-past structures nominative alignment is observed whereas the past tense requires an ergative alignment. Based on these two types of alignments many linguists like Haig (2004), Thackston (2006), and Gündoğdu (2011) argue that Kurmanji is a split ergative language. Accordingly, the major aim of this study is to investigate the structure of the ergative pattern in Adıyaman Kurmanji. In this study, the initial step was to compare the ergative and nominative subjects in terms of certain tests like binding, scope and EPP to determine the phrase structure and where the subjects reside on the structure. Additionally, voice properties of the language were inspected as a background to the major claim. Based on the results of the tests applied and the motivation obtained from the data, it was argued that what has been called ergative in Adıyaman Kurmanji is, indeed, a passive structure diachronically reanalyzed as the past tense. According to Trask’s (1979) typology of ergative languages, there are two types of ergative languages, which labels as Type A and Type B.
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Agreement and Case Marking in Burushaski
    Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session Volume 40 Article 5 1996 Verb agreement and case marking in Burushaski Stephen R. Willson SIL-UND Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers Part of the Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Willson, Stephen R. (1996) "Verb agreement and case marking in Burushaski," Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session: Vol. 40 , Article 5. DOI: 10.31356/silwp.vol40.05 Available at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers/vol40/iss1/5 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by UND Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session by an authorized editor of UND Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Verb Agreement and Case Marking in Burushaski Stephen R. Willson 1 Burushaski verb agreement and case marking phenomena are complex and have not been described adequately by any current theory ofsyntax. In particular, no explanation has yet been given as to why a variety of nominals can trigger agreement in the verbal prefix. In some cases the apparent subject triggers this agreement, in others the direct object appears to do so, in others the indirect object, in others the possessor of the direct object, in others a benefactive or source nominal. Also, the constraints on the usage of ergative, absolutive and oblique case, and other indicators ofgrammatical relations on nominals, have been insufficiently characterized in the literature on Burushaski.
    [Show full text]
  • Ergativity Can Be Defined As Either a Morphological of Syntactic Process
    Laura J. Mahalingappa - University of Texas at Austin Acquisition of Split-Ergativity in Kurmanji Kurdish: Variability and Language Change Questions of learnability surrounding ergativity have prompted studies suggesting that children acquire the morphological systems of ergative and accusative languages equally easily (Pye 1990) and that sociolinguistic distribution can cause the late appearance of ergative case- marking (Ochs 1982). However, what do children acquiring ergativity do when presented with the problem of variability in caretaker input? Research suggests that children can acquire “variable forms of a language at an early stage, reflecting the proportion in which the variants occur” in caregiver input (Henry 2002, 278). This study examines the acquisition of split-ergativity in Kurmanji Kurdish, where, in present-tense transitive sentences, the oblique case (OBL) marks patients and the direct case (DIR) marks agents, the verb agreeing with the agent. In past-tense transitive sentences, however, agents are marked with OBL and patients are marked with DIR, the verb agreeing with patient instead of agent (Example 1). Subjects of intransitive sentences are always marked with DIR. However, recent research suggests that split-ergativity in Kurmanji is weakening due to either internally-induced change or contact with Turkish (Dixon 1994, Dorleijn 1996), perhaps moving towards a full nominative-accusative system. This change is demonstrated by the disappearance of OBL on masculine nouns and the increasing use of OBL to mark objects in past-tense transitive structures (Example 2). Therefore, children acquiring Kurmanji are faced with learning split-ergative grammatical relations with highly variable non-systematic input from caretakers, likely due to language change in progress.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of Chain Foot and Island in Relative Clauses in the RT Analysis
    FROM INTRUSIVE TO RESUMPTIVE THE ACQUISITION OF WH-DEPENDENCIES BY BEHDINI LEARNERS OF ENGLISH By Shivan Shlaymoon Toma A thesis submitted in accordance with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Second Language Acquisition in the School of Languages, Cultures, and Societies Department of Linguistics and Phonetics The University of Leeds May 2016 Thesis supervisor: Dr Cécile De Cat I confirm that the work submitted is my own and that appropriate credit has been given where reference has been made to the work of others. This copy has been supplied on the understanding that it is copyright material and that no quotation from the thesis may be published without proper acknowledgement. 2016 The University of Leeds Shivan Shlaymoon Toma ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, praises and thanks to God, the Almighty, for having made everything possible and for His showers of blessings throughout my research work to complete this thesis. I have been waiting for this section for so long to express my deepest gratitude to Dr Cecile De Cat, my supervisor, who advised me through all the steps necessary for the completion of this thesis, from designing the structure of the study, collecting data, setting research questions, analyzing data, and discussing the results. Her competence in the field of empirical research and her expertise in statistical analysis were utterly helpful and resolved major doubts in this research. She has also been an encouraging, patient and nice person. I am extremely grateful to the Iraqi Kurdistan Regional Government for sponsoring and funding my studies through the Human Capacity Development Program (HCDP) in Higher Education, which is an ambitious program that aims to develop human capacities in Kurdistan Region in the field of higher education.
    [Show full text]
  • Types of Crosslinguistic Variation in Case Assignment
    Types of Crosslinguistic Variation in Case Assignment Mark Baker Rutgers University In this work, I do not address directly issues about what range of hypotheses concerning crosslinguistic variation are or aren’t Minimalist, or questions about micro- vs. macro- parameters, or clarifying the role of the lexicon versus syntactic principles in crosslinguistic variation, or so on. It is not because I’m above talking about such things or don’t have opinions about them. That is simply not what I want to focus on here. Rather I want to offer something more along the lines of addressing what Cedric Boeckx calls Greenburg’s problem. I agree with him that Greenburg’s problem is quite distinct from Plato’s problem, but I think that it’s also interesting, and we should be trying to come up with solutions for it—although not perhaps in exactly the way Greenburg might have foreseen. In particular, I’ll focus here on a specific issue in syntactic variation, namely the theory of morphological case, especially overt structural case. I imagine that if there is such a thing as abstract Case (i.e. NP licensing) that that is systematically related, but that topic is not my focus here. In presenting this research, I also want to illustrate a style of relating to the material that I want to recommend, what I’ve been calling Formal Generative Typology (Baker, 2010). This involves committing to two kinds of ideas which I think are both attractive. The first is the generativist vision of accepting a degree of abstractness in our descriptions of languages, and looking at how insight into empirical details can emerge from proper formalization of the patterns.
    [Show full text]
  • Variation in the Ergative Pattern of Kurmanji Songül Gündoğdu, Muş
    Variation in the Ergative Pattern of Kurmanji Songül Gündoğdu, Muş Alparslan University, Muş, Turkey LANGUAGE KEYWORDS: Northern Kurdish (Kurmanji) AREA KEYWORDS: Middle East (Turkey) ABSTRACT (ENGLISH) Kurdish-Kurmanji (or Northern Kurdish) belongs to the Iranian branch of the Indo-European language family. This study is dedicated to a deeper understanding of a specific grammatical feature typical of Kurmanji: the ergative structure. Based on the example of this core structure, and with empirical evidence from the Kurmanji dialect of Muş in Turkey, I will discuss the issues of variation and change in Kurmanji, more precisely the ongoing shift from ergative to nominative-accusative structures. The causes for such a fundamental shift, however, are not easy to define. The close historical vicinity to Turkish and Armenian might be a trigger for the shift; another trigger is language-internal (diachronic) change. In sum, the investigated variation sheds light on a fascinating grammatical change in a language that is also sociopolitically in a situation of constant change, movement, and upheaval. ABSTRACT (DEUTSCH) Kurdisch-Kurmanci (oder Nordkurdisch) gehört zum iranischen Zweig der indoeuropäischen Sprachfamilie. Eine zentrale und charakteristische grammatische Struktur dieser Sprache ist der Ergativ; ihm ist der vorliegende Beitrag gewidmet. Am Beispiel des Ergativ werde ich Variation und Wandel im Kurmanci diskutieren und insbesondere auf den Dialekt von Muş (Türkei) eingehen. Dabei zeigt sich, dass sich derzeit im Kurmanci der Wandel von einer Ergativ- zu einer Nominativ-Akkusativ-Sprache vollzieht. Die Ursachen für einen derart grundlegenden Wandel sind nicht leicht zu benennen. Ein Grund mag in der schon historisch engen Nachbarschaft zum Türkischen und Armenischen liegen; ein anderer Grund mag im sprach-internen (diachronen) Wandel des Kurmanci selbst zu finden sein.
    [Show full text]
  • Overview of Ergativity
    Chapter 2 Ergativity Chapter 2 Overview of Ergativity Ergativity is a term that refers to a certain pattern that some languages show in treating arguments of a verb. Generally, arguments are classified into three types: the subject of a transitive verb, the object of a transitive verb and the subject of an intransitive verb. Adopting Dixon’s (1979) terminology, we will refer to them as A, O, and S, respectively. If a language treats A and S in the same way, distinguishing O, the pattern is called accusative. In contrast, in an ergative pattern, S and O are treated as equivalent, while A is treated differently. The notion of ergativity is often used with regard to case marking. In an ergative case system, S and O appear in the same case, absolutive and A appears in a special case, ergative. An ergative pattern of case marking is referred to as morphological ergativity. Ergativity is not merely a morphological phenomenon. Some languages treat S and O as equivalents in some syntactic operations such as coordination and relativisation. An ergative pattern found at the level of syntax is referred to as syntactic ergativity. It should be noted that morphological ergativity does not entail syntactic ergativity. A language may have an ergative case marking without showing syntactic ergativity. However, a language that shows syntactic ergativity necessarily has an ergative case system. Syntactic ergativity is incompatible with accusative case marking. In other 13 Chapter 2 Ergativity words, there seems to be some correlation between morphological ergativity and syntactic ergativity. This leads to a hypothesis that ergative case marking is associated with syntactic configuration.
    [Show full text]
  • Structural Case in Finnish
    Structural Case in Finnish Paul Kiparsky Stanford University 1 Introduction 1.1 Morphological case and abstract case The fundamental fact that any theory of case must address is that morphological form and syntactic function do not stand in a one-to-one correspondence, yet are systematically related.1 Theories of case differ in whether they define case categories at a single structural level of representation, or at two or more levels of representation. For theories of the first type, the mismatches raise a dilemma when morphological form and syntactic function diverge. Which one should the classification be based on? Generally, such single-level approaches determine the case inventory on the basis of morphology using paradigmatic contrast as the basic criterion, and propose rules or constraints that map the resulting cases to grammatical relations. Multi-level case theories deal with the mismatch between morphological form and syntactic function by distinguishing morphological case on the basis of form and abstract case on the basis of function. Approaches that distinguish between abstract case and morphological case in this way typically envisage an interface called “spellout” that determines the relationship between them. In practice, this outlook has served to legitimize a neglect of inflectional morphol- ogy. The neglect is understandable, for syntacticians’ interest in morphological case is naturally less as a system in its own right than as a diagnostic for ab- stract case and grammatical relations. But it is not entirely benign: compare the abundance of explicit proposals about how abstract cases are assigned with the minimal attention paid to how they are morphologically realized.
    [Show full text]
  • The Clitic Status of Person Markers in Sorani Kurdish
    The clitic status of person markers in Sorani Kurdish Hiba Gharib and Clifton Pye* Department of Linguistics, University of Kansas The ambiguous nature of clitics challenges simple distinctions between morphology and syntax. Clitic properties also feature in theoretical accounts of ergativity. In this paper we analyze the nature of the person markers in Sorani Kurdish. The direct person markers are conventionally treated as verb affixes, while the oblique person markers are assumed to be clitics. A variety of clitic tests show that both the direct and oblique person markers have some affix properties as well as some clitic properties. The results illustrate the difficulty that language-specific features pose for a general theory of clitics. Keywords: Sorani Kurdish, clitic, affix, ergativity 1. Introduction The ambiguous nature of clitics challenges simple distinctions between morphology and syntax (Spencer & Luís, 2012). In this paper we analyze the nature of person markers in Sorani Kurdish. Sorani (Central Kurdish) is a dialect of the Kurdish language spoken in northern Iraq and northwest Iran. Kurdish, with Balochi and Taleshi, constitute the West Iranian branch of the Iranian languages (Haig, 2017). The Iranian language branch separated from the Indo-Aryan languages some 4,000 years ago. The Iranian languages share many typological features including an OV word order, the frequent use of complex predicates based on light verbs and a tense-based alignment split that derives from transitive verb participles (Haig, 2017, p. 465). The Iranian languages inherited an aspect-based contrast on transitive verbs from Old Iranian. Old Iranian had an accusative alignment system in which all subjects received direct case marking.
    [Show full text]
  • Syntactic Ergativity: Analysis • Explore Related Articles • Search Keywords and Identification
    LI02CH09-Deal ARI 5 December 2015 12:3 ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: • Download figures as PPT slides • Navigate linked references • Download citations Syntactic Ergativity: Analysis • Explore related articles • Search keywords and Identification Amy Rose Deal Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2016. 2:165–85 Keywords The Annual Review of Linguistics is online at ergativity, A¯ movement, inherent case, dependent case, case discrimination, linguist.annualreviews.org agent focus, wh-agreement, antiagreement, typology, morphology, syntax This article’s doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040642 Abstract Copyright c 2016 by Annual Reviews. Some languages showing morphological ergativity in case and/or agreement All rights reserved also show ergative patterns in core syntactic domains—syntactic ergativity. The most-studied type of syntactic ergativity is a ban on the A¯ movement of ergative subjects; an additional type concerns the distribution of abso- lutives in nonfinite clauses. This article first presents the standard view of syntactic ergativity, which is closely connected to the treatment of ergative as an inherent case. Evidence from Shipibo suggests that a ban on erga- ¯ Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2016.2:165-185. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org tive A extraction does not require inherent ergative. This points to a view of syntactic ergativity centered around morphological case discrimination. Access provided by University of California - Berkeley on 01/25/16. For personal use only. One consequence is that pure head-marking languages cannot feature a true ban on ergative extraction, because ergative morphological case is not in use.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammatical Case in the Languages of the Middle East and Europe
    GRAMMATICAL CASE IN THE LANGUAGES OF THE MIDDLE EAST AND EUROPE ACTS OF THE INTERNATIONAL COLLOQUIUM VARIATIONS, CONCURRENCE ET EVOLUTION DES CAS DANS DIVERS DOMAINES LINGUISTIQUES Paris 2–4 April 2007 edited by MICHÈLE FRUYT MICHEL MAZOYER and DENNIS PARDEE studies in ancient ORientaL civiLizatiOn • vOLume 64 THE ORIENTAL INSTITUTE OF THE UNivERSITY OF CHICAGO chicagO • IllinOis Library of Congress Control Number: 2011933159 ISBN-10: 1-885923-84-8 ISBN-13: 978-1-885923-84-4 ISSN: 0081-7554 The Oriental Institute, Chicago © 2011 by the university of chicago. all rights reserved. Published 2011. Printed in the united states of america. studies in ancient ORientaL civiLizatiOn • numbeR 64 the Oriental institute of the university of chicago Series Editors Leslie schramer and thomas g. urban with the assistance of Rebecca cain Series Editors’ Acknowledgments Jessen O’brien and Felicia Whitcomb assisted in the production of this volume. Cover Image The University of Chicago Center in Paris. Photo by Dan Dry Printed by McNaughton & Gunn, Saline, Michigan the paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of ameri- can national standard for information services — Permanence of Paper for Printed Library materials, ansi z39.48-1984. TABLE OF CONTENTS Preface. Michèle Fruyt, Michel Mazoyer, and Dennis Pardee..................................................... vii 1. cas et analyse en morphèmes? Christian Touratier . 1 2. the conjugation Prefixes, the dative case, and the empathy hierarchy in sumerian. Christopher Woods . 11 3. agent, subject, Patient, and beneficiary: grammatical Roles in hurrian. Dennis R. M. Campbell................ 21 4. des cas en élamite? Florence Malbran-Labat . 47 5. Évolution des cas dans le sémitique archaïque: la contribution de l’éblaïte.
    [Show full text]