ST~~YI~~ ?F ~F~F Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

ST~~YI~~ ?F ~F~F Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research ?f ~P~f ST~~YI~~ ?f ~f~f Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research Ralph L. Rosnow Temple University Robert Rosenthal Harvard University II w. H. FREEMAN AND COMPANY NEW YORK ACQUISITIONS EDITOR: Susan Finnemore Brennan We are as sailors who are forced to rebuild their ship on the open sea, PROJECT EDITOR: Christine Hastings without ever being able to start fresh from the bottom up. Wherever COVER AND TEXT DESIGNER: Victoria Tomaselli a beam is taken away, immediately a new one must take its place, ILLUSTRATION COORDINATOR: Bill Page PRODUCTION COORDINATOR: Maura Studley and while this is done, the rest of the ship is used as support. In this COMPOSITION: W. H. Freeman Electronic Publishing Center/Susan Cory way, the ship may be completely rebuilt like new with the help of the MANUFACTURING: RR Donnelley & Sons Company old beams and driftwood-but only through gradual rebuilding. 1 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Rosnow, Ralph L. People studying people: artifacts and ethics in behavioral research/ Ralph L. Rosnow, Robert Rosenthal. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 0- 7167-3070-7. -ISBN 0-7167- 3071-5 (pbk.) 1. Psychology-Research. 2. Social sciences-Research. I. Rosenthal, Robert, 1933- II. Title BF76.5.R645 1997 150'.7'2-dc21 96-48978 CIP © 1997 by W. H. Freeman and Company No part of this book may be reproduced by any mechanical, photographic, or electronic process, or in the form of a phonographic recording, nor may it be stored in a retrieval system, transmitted, or otherwise copied for public or private use, without written permission from the publisher. Printed in the United States of America First printing, 1997 Contents Preface ix Rrtif acts in Behavioral Research l The Nature of Artifacts 1 Clever Hans 3 The Hawthorne Studies S Rosenzweig's Critique 7 Resistance to the Artifact Idea 9 Random Versus Systematic Error 11 Biasing Effects of Investigators 13 Interactional and Noninteractional Bias 13 The Investigator as Observer 16 Interpretation of Data 18 Intentional Error 21 Biosocial Attributes of Investigators 24 Psychosocial Attributes of Investigators 2 7 Situational Factors 28 Modeling Effects 32 Coping with Investigator Biases 36 Expectations as Self-Fulnlling Prophecies 41 Expectancy Bias 41 A Sample of Studies 43 The First 34S Studies 46 Moderation of Interpersonal Expectancies S1 Mediation of Interpersonal Expectancies S2 Teacher Expectancy Effects S4 Controls for Expectancy Effects SS Expectancy Control Design S9 The Person Behind the Look 63 Demand Characteristics 63 The "Good Subject" Effect 65 Evaluation Apprehension 68 The Obedient Subject 69 The Dayyan's Decree 72 Mediation of Participation-Related Artifacts 77 Minimizing Experimental Artifacts 82 Detecting the Occurrence of Artifacts 85 The Volunteer Subject 89 io Why Study the Volunteer Subject? 89 SU Quantifying Volunteer Bias 91 sp Reliability of Volunteering 94 ke Comparing Volunteers and Nonvolunteers 96 iz Characteristics of the Volunteer Subject 97 Situational Determinants of Volunteering 102 al~ Volunteer Status and Research Outcomes 105 ing Power and the Pseudovolunteer 107 mu Predicting the Direction of Bias 108 ab cor. Ethical and Methodological Challenges 113 ing Tides of Change 113 thy Ethics and Artifacts in Collision 115 alo The "Ten Commandments" of the APA 118 IS Opportunity Knocks More Than Once 121 Not a Cannon, but a Popgun 124 Beneficence, Respect, and Justice 125 Where Are We Heading? 126 Risks and Benefits 127 artij Waste Not, Want Not 130 sear Pfu1 Notes 133 nee1 Name Index 163 Edv Subject Index 169 Cro1 Kau Viii t~~pter l nrtif acts in Behavioral Research All scientific inquiry is subject to error, and it is far better to be aware of this, to study the sources in an attempt to reduce it, and to estimate the magnitude of such errors in our findings, than to be ignorant of the errors concealed in the data. One must not equate ignorance of error with the lack of error. The lack of demonstration of error in certain fields of inquiry often derives from the nonexistence of methodological research into the problem and merely denotes a less advanced stage of that profession. The Nature of Artifacts f he effort to understand human behavior must of the oldest of human behaviors. ~ itself be one But for all the centuries of effort, there is much we do not yet know. The unsolved behavioral prob­ lems of mental illness, racism, sexism, and vio­ lence, both the idiosyncratic and institutionalized, bear witness to how much we do not yet know. Because of the urgency of the questions waiting to I . I Chapter Rrtifacts in Behavioral Research J , be answered, it should not be surprising that behavioral scien- We can conceptualize this portion of the complexity of be­ 21 tists, and the publics that support them, suffer from a certain im- havior, which can be attributed to the intrinsic human aspects of n patience. That impatience is understandable, but perhaps, from behavioral and social research, as a set of artifacts (or systematic time to time, we need to remind ourselves that Neurath's "ship of errors) to be isolated, measured, considered, and, sometimes, science" has not been afloat very long in the behavioral sciences. eliminated. That is, artifacts are not simply inconsequential effects The application of that reasoning and of those procedures in a research design; they may actually jeopardize the validity that we call the scientific method to the understanding of human of the researcher's inferences from his or her results. Another behavior is of relatively recent origin. Yet what we have learned way of saying this is that artifacts are unintended or uncon­ about human behavior in the short period, say, from Wilhelm trolled human aspects of the research situation that confound Wundt's founding of experimental psychology in Leipzig in 1879 the investigator's conclusions about what went on in the study. until now is out of all proportion to what was learned in the pre­ As we shall see later, artifacts can also teach us about topics ceding centuries. Although most of what we want to know is still of substantive interest. Indeed, as William McGuire observed, unknown, this success of the application of "scientific method" today's artifact may be tomorrow's independent variable;2 that to the study of human behavior has given us great hope of an ac­ is, the same conditions discounted as "nuisance variables" at celerating return of Jpiowledge on our investment of time and one time may later be exploited as variables of interest in their effort. To be sure, this application of what we think of as scien­ own right. tific method has not simplified human behavior; instead, it has shown us more clearly just how complex human behavior really is. In modern behavioral science, the research participant (or Clever Hans "research s~bject") serves as our model of people in general, or The systematic investigation of artifacts began in the late 1950s, at least of a certain kind of person. We know that behavior is the bulk of the research following in the 1960s and 1970s. Well be­ complex because we sometimes change this person's world ever fore this work began in earnest, there had been early indications so slightly and detect substantial changes in his or her behavior, that artifacts might be lurking in the investigative procedures used while at other times we change that world greatly and detect by behavioral scientists. A classic case showed that artifacts could hardly any changes in behavior. It is as if the research partici­ result from the observer's expectations or hypotheses. In this 1904 pants' minds were somehow in a different world from ours. We case, a German psychologist, Oscar Pfungst, carried out a six-week also know that behavior is complex because a careful experiment investigation of a remarkable horse owned by a mathematics conducted in one place at one time often yields results very dif­ teacher named Wilhelm von Osten.3 Since the beginning of ferent from the "same" experiment conducted in another place recorded civilization, there had been reports of "learned animals;' at another time. We have learned that much of the complexity of but no animal had so captured the imagination of the general human behavior is inherent, but we have also learned that some public and of European psychology as von Osten's horse, "Clever of this complexity may result from uncontrolled aspects of the Hans." Pfungst's elegant series of studies solved the riddle of research situation, especially from the interaction between the Hans's amazing performance and implied how easily observers researcher and the participant. can be deceived by the self-fulfilling nature of their expectations. Artifacts in Behavioral Research I Chapter I I Before Pfungst had entered the scene, rumors had circulated unwitting movements and mannerisms. Someone would ask 4J widely of the astounding "intellectual" feats performed by Hans. Hans a question requiring a long tapping response, and the per- JS By tapping his hoof according to a code taught to him by von son might then lean forward as though settling down for a long Osten, Hans was said to answer questions put to him in German. wait. Hans responded to the questioner's forward movement, Asked to spell a word, Hans would tap out the letters, aided not to the actual question, and kept tapping away with his hoof ostensibly by a code table set in front of him. To respond yes to until the questioner communicated his expectancy that Hans a question, he would nod his head up and down; to respond would stop tapping. This the questioner might do by suddenly no, he would execute a deliberate sideways motion.
Recommended publications
  • Beyond Clever Hans: Learning from People Without Their Really Trying
    Beyond Clever Hans: Learning From People Without Their Really Trying by Vivek Veeriah Jeya Veeraiah A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Department of Computing Science University of Alberta c Vivek Veeriah Jeya Veeraiah, 2017 Abstract Facial expressions and other body language are important for human commu- nication. They complement speech and make the process of communication simple and sustainable. However, the process of communication using existing approaches to human-machine interaction is not intuitive as that of human communication. Specifically, the existing approaches to human machine inter- action do not learn from whatever subtle non-verbal cues produced by a user. Many of the existing approaches map body language cues to instructions or re- wards and use them to train a machine. These mappings are not learned from ongoing interactions and are assumed to be defined by some external source. As a consequence, the communicative process through these approaches can cause significant cognitive load on the user. This is an important problem that needs to be addressed if we are to amplify our existing cognitive and physical capabilities through intelligent machines. Towards addressing this, we intro- duce our idea that allows machines to learn from whatever subtle cues people produce during the process of interaction. Particularly, the agent learns a value function that maps the user’s non-verbal cues to later rewards. By using this value function, the user can teach their agent to complete a task according to their preferences, and consequently maximize their satisfaction. We demon- strate this by training an agent using facial expressions.
    [Show full text]
  • Clever Hans, by Oskar Pfungst 1
    Clever Hans, by Oskar Pfungst 1 Clever Hans, by Oskar Pfungst The Project Gutenberg EBook of Clever Hans, by Oskar Pfungst This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere at no cost and with almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg License included with this eBook or online at www.gutenberg.org Title: Clever Hans (The horse of Mr. Von Osten): A contribution to experimental animal and human psychology Author: Oskar Pfungst Translator: Carl L. Rahn Release Date: October 11, 2010 [EBook #33936] Language: English Character set encoding: ISO-8859-1 *** START OF THIS PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK CLEVER HANS *** Clever Hans, by Oskar Pfungst 2 Produced by Audrey Longhurst, Linda Hamilton and the Online Distributed Proofreading Team at http://www.pgdp.net [Illustration: CLEVER HANS] CLEVER HANS (THE HORSE OF MR. VON OSTEN) A CONTRIBUTION TO EXPERIMENTAL ANIMAL AND HUMAN PSYCHOLOGY BY OSKAR PFUNGST WITH AN INTRODUCTION BY PROF. C. STUMPF, AND ONE ILLUSTRATION AND FIFTEEN FIGURES TRANSLATED FROM THE GERMAN BY CARL L. RAHN Fellow in Psychology in the University of Chicago WITH A PREFATORY NOTE BY JAMES R. ANGELL Professor of Psychology in the University of Chicago [Illustration] NEW YORK HENRY HOLT AND COMPANY 1911 COPYRIGHT, 1911, BY HENRY HOLT AND COMPANY PREFATORY NOTE [BY JAMES R. ANGELL] The University of Chicago It gives me great pleasure to accept the invitation of the publishers to write a word of introduction for Mr. Rahn's excellent translation of "Der Kluge Hans", a book which in the original has been but little known to American readers.
    [Show full text]
  • Doing Away with Morgan 'S Canon
    Doing Away with Morgan’ s Canon SIMON FITZPATRICK Abstract : Morgan ’ s Canon is a very widely endorsed methodological principle in animal psychology, believed to be vital for a rigorous, scientifi c approach to the study of animal cognition. In contrast I argue that Morgan ’ s Canon is unjustifi ed, pernicious and unnecessary. I identify two main versions of the Canon and show that they both suffer from very serious problems. I then suggest an alternative methodological principle that captures all of the genuine methodological benefi ts that Morgan ’ s Canon can bring but suffers from none of its problems. 1. Introduction Morgan ’ s Canon is a very widely endorsed methodological principle in animal psychology. Proposed at the end of the 19th century by the British comparative psychologist Conwy Lloyd Morgan, it states that: In no case may we interpret an action as the outcome of the exercise of a higher psychical faculty, if it can be interpreted as the outcome of the exercise of one which stands lower in the psychological scale (Morgan, 1894 , p. 53). This principle has played a central role in subsequent scientifi c and philosophical work into the nature of non-human animal minds. So central in fact that it has been described as ‘ possibly the most important single sentence in the history of the study of animal behaviour ’ ( Galef, 1996, p. 9; cited in Radick, 2000 , p. 3). Morgan ’ s Canon has attained this eminent status because it has been seen as a necessary component of a suitably rigorous approach to explaining animal behaviour. According to a recent textbook, ‘ Morgan ’ s Canon … has enabled us to approach the analysis of behaviour sensibly and to avoid the superfi cial anthropomorphism which led to many absurdities in the past ’ ( Manning and Dawkins, 1998 , p.
    [Show full text]
  • BROWN, BARNUM (1873–1963) Dinosaur Fossil Collector Amed After the Great Showman P
    BRoWn, BaRnUM (1873–1963) Dinosaur Fossil collector amedafterthegreatshowmanP.T.Barnum,indefatigabledinosaurdiggerBarnum Brownassembledhisownversionof“The Greatest Showon Earth”:aparadeofgiant n dinosaurfossilswrenchedfromthecliffsandarroyosoftheAmericanWest.Brown’s -lastingcontribution—hundredsoftonsofdinosaurfossils—formedthenucleusoftheAmeri .canMuseumofnaturalHistory’sworld-famouscollection during the 1960s, Brown, nearly 90, could still be seen leading visitors around the crammeddinosaurhalls,announcing,“Here’sanotheroneofmychildren,”ashepointed outthebonesofasauriangiant.Butwhenhebeganhiscareerin1897,themuseumhad .notasingledinosaur AsachildinCarbondale, Kansas,Browncollectedfossilsfromfreshlyplowedfields.He -attendedtheUniversityof Kansas,thenmovedto New YorkCity,wherehestudiedpaleontol ogyatColumbiaUniversityandbeganworkingatthemuseumwhilehewasstillagraduate student. Forhisfirstfieldassignment,themuseum’sdirector,HenryFairfield Osborn,sentBrown BONE hUNTER barnum brown, when almost ninety, toComoBluff,Wyoming,toprospectitsrichJurassicdeposits.Brownandhiscolleagues supervised construction of discoverednewbedscontainingenormousquantitiesoffossils,includingtheApatosaurus .sinclair oil’s lifesize dino- (thencalled Brontosaurus)thatstilldominatesoneofthemuseum’shugedinosaurhalls saur models, shown below However, yale paleontologist othniel C. Marsh was furious about his former sites being being barged down the hud- .workedandbeganabitterfeudwithosbornthatlastedtotheendofhislife son river to the 1964 new york World’s fair.
    [Show full text]
  • THE TRUE FAIRYTALE of CLEVER HANS of Course We Don’T Want to Tell Any Fairy Tales Here …
    THE TRUE FAIRYTALE OF CLEVE R HANS THE TRUE FAIRYTALE OF CLEVER HANS Of course we don’t want to tell any fairy tales here … … Even though the story of how a three-man business founded by Hans Grohe in 1901 in the little Black Forest town of Schiltach became a world-wide enterprise with over 3,200 employees might seem to some to be more like a fairy tale… What follows is the story of the firm of Hansgrohe, spanning more than a century of growth and development. And the story of Hans Grohe himself, the handyman, the craftsman, the visionary pioneer in the field of sanitation, without whom the company of Hansgrohe could never have come into being. And the story of his descendants who right up to the present day lead and guide the firm of Hansgrohe SE. It’s not a dry-as-dust story, and doesn’t claim to be complete in itself or to be a scientific account; it is told here sometimes tongue in cheek, with great joy in the telling, giving deep insights into the world of Hansgrohe and into the family firm, founded by Clever Hans. And if the story of “Lucky Hans” (“Hans im Glück”) as once told by the Grimm Brothers in their collection of fairy tales is unfamiliar to you, just read on. Truth is stranger than fiction, as they say … 2 Hans had served his master for seven years and now he had an urgent desire to go to Schiltach. So he asked his master to give him his wages.
    [Show full text]
  • “Clever Hans Effect” You Are Going to Listen to a Scientific Radio Show
    Kammerhofer/Zangerl Going for Finals B2, Audio-CD – Transcripts GOING FOR FINALS B2,-AUDIO-CD – TRANSCRIPTS 1 The “Clever Hans Effect” You are going to listen to a scientific radio show about an experiment with an animal. First you will have 45 seconds to study the task below, then you will hear the recording twice. While listening, complete the sentences (1–9) using a maximum of 4 words. Write your answers in the spaces provided. The first one (0) has been done for you. After the second listening, you will have 45 seconds to check your answers. Hello and good evening, dear listeners. Today I’m talking about the “Clever-Hans-Effect”. This term refers originally to a trained horse, named “Clued Hans”, referred to in the literature as “Clever Hans” … who was taught by his master, William Von Osten, to do math and to report on this math … So William would ask the horse, ask Hans, to add 3 + 2 for example and Hans would tap his hoof five times. This may have seemed as if the horse understood not only the spoken word, but understood math, to mean that it was able to do mathematical calculations on its own, which was unheard of in the ani- mal kingdom. Several scientists reviewed this demonstration and declared it genuine. However, when proper con- trols were put into place it turned out that, if the people who knew the correct answer to the question Hans was being asked were not within sight of Hans, Hans could not come up with the correct answer.
    [Show full text]
  • A History of Modern Psychology, 10Th
    This page intentionally left blank This page intentionally left blank A History of Modern Psychology TENTH EDITION DUANE P. SCHULTZ University of South Florida SYDNEY ELLEN SCHULTZ Australia • Brazil • Japan • Korea • Mexico • Singapore • Spain • United Kingdom • United States This is an electronic version of the print textbook. Due to electronic rights restrictions, some third party content may be suppressed. Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. The publisher reserves the right to remove content from this title at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. For valuable information on pricing, previous editions, changes to current editions, and alternate formats, please visit www.cengage.com/highered to search by ISBN#, author, title, or keyword for materials in your areas of interest. A History of Modern Psychology, © 2011 Wadsworth, Cengage Learning Tenth Edition ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. No part of this work covered by the copyright Duane P. Schultz and Sydney Ellen herein may be reproduced, transmitted, stored or used in any form or by Schultz any means graphic, electronic, or mechanical, including but not limited to photocopying, recording, scanning, digitizing, taping, Web distribu- Senior Publisher: Linda Schreiber-Ganster tion, information networks, or information storage and retrieval sys- Executive Editor: Jon-David Hague tems, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United Editorial Assistant: Sheli DeNola States Copyright
    [Show full text]
  • People Studying People: Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research
    People studying people: artifacts and ethics in behavioral research The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Rosnow, Ralph L. and Robert Rosenthal. 1997. People studying people: artifacts and ethics in behavioral research. New York: W.H. Freeman Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:32300329 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA ?f ~P~f ST~~YI~~ ?f ~f~f Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research Ralph L. Rosnow Temple University Robert Rosenthal Harvard University II w. H. FREEMAN AND COMPANY NEW YORK ACQUISITIONS EDITOR: Susan Finnemore Brennan We are as sailors who are forced to rebuild their ship on the open sea, PROJECT EDITOR: Christine Hastings without ever being able to start fresh from the bottom up. Wherever COVER AND TEXT DESIGNER: Victoria Tomaselli a beam is taken away, immediately a new one must take its place, ILLUSTRATION COORDINATOR: Bill Page PRODUCTION COORDINATOR: Maura Studley and while this is done, the rest of the ship is used as support. In this COMPOSITION: W. H. Freeman Electronic Publishing Center/Susan Cory way, the ship may be completely rebuilt like new with the help of the MANUFACTURING: RR Donnelley & Sons Company old beams and driftwood-but only through gradual rebuilding. 1 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Rosnow, Ralph L.
    [Show full text]
  • Unmasking Clever Hans Predictors and Assessing What Machines Really Learn
    Unmasking Clever Hans Predictors and Assessing What Machines Really Learn Sebastian Lapuschkin1, Stephan W¨aldchen2, Alexander Binder3, Gr´egoireMontavon2, Wojciech Samek1y and Klaus-Robert M¨uller2;4;5y∗ 1Department of Video Coding & Analytics, Fraunhofer Heinrich Hertz Institute Einsteinufer 37, 10587 Berlin, Germany 2Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Technische Universit¨atBerlin Marchstr. 23, 10587 Berlin, Germany 3ISTD Pillar, Singapore University of Technology and Design 8 Somapah Rd, Singapore 487372, Singapore 4Department of Brain and Cognitive Engineering, Korea University Anam-dong, Seongbuk-ku, Seoul 136-713, Republic of Korea 5Max Planck Institut f¨urInformatik Campus E1 4, Stuhlsatzenhausweg, 66123 Saarbr¨ucken, Germany yCorresponding authors. E-mail: [email protected], [email protected]. Abstract Current learning machines have successfully solved hard application problems, reaching high accuracy and displaying seemingly \intelligent" behavior. Here we apply recent techniques for explaining decisions of state-of-the-art learning machines and analyze various tasks from computer vision and arcade games. This showcases a spectrum of problem-solving behaviors ranging from naive and short-sighted, to well- informed and strategic. We observe that standard performance evaluation metrics can be oblivious to distinguishing these diverse problem solving behaviors. Furthermore, we propose our semi-automated Spectral Relevance Analysis that provides a practically effective way of characterizing and validating the behavior of nonlinear learning machines. This helps to assess whether a learned model indeed delivers reliably for the problem that it was conceived for. Furthermore, our work intends to add a voice of caution to the ongoing excitement about machine intelligence and pledges to evaluate and judge some of these recent successes in a more nuanced manner.
    [Show full text]
  • Beyond Clever Hans: Learning from People Without Their Really Trying
    Beyond Clever Hans: Learning From People Without Their Really Trying by Vivek Veeriah A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Department of Computing Science University of Alberta c Vivek Veeriah, 2017 Abstract Facial expressions and other body language are important for human commu- nication. They complement speech and make the process of communication simple and sustainable. However, the process of communication using existing approaches to human-machine interaction is not intuitive as that of human communication. Specifically, the existing approaches to human machine inter- action do not learn from whatever subtle non-verbal cues produced by a user. Many of the existing approaches map body language cues to instructions or re- wards and use them to train a machine. These mappings are not learned from ongoing interactions and are assumed to be defined by some external source. As a consequence, the communicative process through these approaches can cause significant cognitive load on the user. This is an important problem that needs to be addressed if we are to amplify our existing cognitive and physical capabilities through intelligent machines. Towards addressing this, we intro- duce our idea that allows machines to learn from whatever subtle cues people produce during the process of interaction. Particularly, the agent learns a value function that maps the user's non-verbal cues to later rewards. By using this value function, the user can teach their agent to complete a task according to their preferences, and consequently maximize their satisfaction. We demon- strate this by training an agent using facial expressions.
    [Show full text]
  • From Our Readers
    From Our Readers The letters column is a forum for views multiplied beyond necessity." But when on matters raised in previous issues. Let­ we translate this statement into "the ters are more likely to be published if simplest explanation for an observation they are brief and typed double-spaced. is most likely to be the correct one," then They may be edited for space and clarity. I pause. In the first place, if the simplest explanation is most likely to be correct, then it is not always correct. Then how Occam's Razor do we know when it is correct? A law with exceptions is of no use unless we I agree with Elie Shneour's results in his have a rule defining the exceptions. article on Occam's Razor (SI, Summer As a matter of fact, there is no reason 1986), but disagree with the method he to believe that the simplest explanation uses to arrive at them. If we take the for anything is the correct one. There is meaning of Occam's Razor to be "the no scientific or logical basis for this belief. simplest explanation for an observation It is simply a convenient prejudice. is most likely to be the correct one," what Counterexamples abound. Consider John is the observation that he is trying to Dalton's belief that all elemental gases explain? Is he trying to explain the bib­ are composed of molecules, each with a lical account of the Flood? Where is there single atom. This was the simplest theory an observation of a flood or an ark? The that explained the observations of the only thing we observe is a story about a day.
    [Show full text]
  • The Horse That Won't Go Away. Clever Hans, Facilitated
    The Horse That Won’t Go Away Clever Hans, Facilitated Communication, and the Need for Clear Thinking Thomas E. Heinzen William Paterson University Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University Susan A. Nolan Seton Hall University heinzen_fm.indd 1 11/24/14 1:20 PM Publisher: Rachel Losh Senior Acquisitions Editor: Daniel DeBonis Development Editor: Elsa Peterson Editorial Assistant: Katie Pachnos Senior Marketing Manager: Lindsay Johnson Marketing Assistant: Allison Greco Director of Editing, Design, and Media Production: Tracey Kuehn Managing Editor: Lisa Kinne Production Editor: Janice Stangel Production Manager: Sarah Segal Photo Editor: Richard Fox Art Director: Diana Blume Interior and Cover Designer: Kevin Kall Art Manager: Matthew McAdams Illustrations: Lachina Composition: Linda Harms Printing and Binding: RR Donnelley Library of Congress Preassigned Control Number: 2014956522 ISBN-10: 1-4641-4574-1 ISBN-13: 978-1-4641-4574-2 © 2015 by Worth Publishers All rights reserved. Printed in the United States of America First printing Worth Publishers 41 Madison Avenue New York, NY 10010 www.worthpublishers.com heinzen_fm.indd 2 11/24/14 1:20 PM For Michael Brown –Tom Heinzen To James Randi, tireless and fearless slayer of myths – Scott Lilienfeld For Ranko Bojanic – Susan Nolan heinzen_fm.indd 3 11/24/14 1:20 PM a b o u t t h e a u t h o r s Thomas E. Heinzen is Professor of Psychology at William Paterson University of New Jersey. A graduate of Rockford College, he earned his Ph.D. in social psychology at the State University of New York at Albany. After publishing his first book on frustration and creativity in government, Heinzen worked as a public policy research associate, consulted for the Johns Hopkins Center for Talented Youth, and then began his teaching career.
    [Show full text]