<<

Notes

MARINE SCIENCE, 31(1): 386–397 (January 2015) Published 2014. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA. DOI: 10.1111/mms.12154

Acoustic evidence that harbor (Phocoena phocoena)avoid bottlenose (Tursiops truncatus)

EIREN K. JACOBSON,1 Scripps Institution of Oceanography, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, California 92093-0208, U.S.A.; KARIN A. FORNEY, Marine Mammal and Turtle Divison, Southwest Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Fisheries Service, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, 110 Shaffer Road, Santa Cruz, California 95060, U.S.A.; JAMES T. HARVEY, Moss Landing Marine Laboratories, 8272 Moss Landing Road, Moss Landing, California 95039, U.S.A.

Along the West Coast of the United States, harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) and coastal common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) overlap in distribution from Point Conception to San Francisco Bay in a narrow coastal band (Wells et al. 1990; Hansen and Defran 1993; Carretta et al. 1998, 2001). This overlap is relatively recent. Increased water temperatures in California during the 1982–1983 El Nino~ event prompted a northward range expansion of coastal bottlenose dolphins past Point Conception into central California (Hansen 1990, Wells et al. 1990). Harbor porpoises occur in distinct populations along the California coast (Calambokidis and Barlow 1991, Chivers et al. 2002, Carretta et al. 2009), whereas coastal bottlenose dolphins move throughout their range and comprise a single population (Defran et al. 1999, Hwang et al. 2014). In California and in other regions where these species overlap, interspecies aggres- sion has been observed (Ross and Wilson 1996, Dunn et al. 2002, Cotter et al. 2012). Between 1991 and 1993 in Moray Firth, Scotland, blunt force trauma associ- ated with bottlenose attacks was the largest contributor to stranded harbor mortality (Ross and Wilson 1996). attacks on harbor porpoises have also been documented in Cardigan Bay, Wales (Jepson and Baker 1998) and in Monterey Bay, California (Cotter et al. 2012, Wilkin et al. 2012). The first documented stranding of a harbor porpoise killed by bottlenose dolphins in Cali- fornia occurred in July 2005. Between 2007 and 2009 researchers witnessed three bottlenose dolphin attacks on harbor porpoises in Monterey Bay (Cotter et al. 2012). These attacks involved 23 individual bottlenose dolphins, 21 of which were known

1Corresponding author (e-mail: [email protected]).

386 NOTES 387 to be males. Observed attacks occurred in the late summer and early fall, during the possible breeding season for bottlenose dolphins. In Monterey and Santa Cruz counties between 2005 and 2011, 179 harbor por- poise strandings were documented. Of these, 43 of 68 stranded porpoises in which cause of death could be determined presented signs of blunt force trauma, at least 28 of which were consistent with attacks by bottlenose dolphins.2 In California between 1998 and 2010, 54 of 216 stranded porpoises in which cause of death was determined presented signs of blunt force trauma consistent with attacks by bottlenose dolphins (Wilkin et al. 2012). In 2007 an apparent increase in harbor porpoise strandings in central California triggered the declaration of an Unusual Mortality Event (UME) and the stranding rate was more than twice the mean annual rate of the previous dec- ade during 2008 and 2009. During this time, stranding rates were highest from June to November and blunt force trauma was the most common diagnosed cause of death (Wilkin et al. 2012). Taken together, the findings from California suggest that in this region a relatively sudden onset of aggression towards harbor porpoise was exe- cuted by a few male bottlenose dolphins. Hypothesized drivers for bottlenose dolphin attacks on harbor porpoise can be broadly divided into ecological and behavioral explanations (Ross and Wilson 1996, Cotter et al. 2012). Possible ecological drivers include prey competition and feeding interference, whereas possible behavioral drivers include object-oriented play to prac- tice fighting or infanticidal behaviors, with high levels of testosterone and a skewed sex ratio perhaps contributing to heightened aggression in male bottlenose dolphins (Ross and Wilson 1996, Cotter et al. 2012). All of the proposed ecological explanations hinge on some form of competition for resources, for which evidence in California is weak. Spitz et al. (2006) reported quali- tative similarities in diet composition of bottlenose dolphins and harbor porpoise in the Bay of Biscay, but quantitative examination found strong evidence that diet pro- files were distinct between these two species. There has been no comprehensive com- parative study of the diets of bottlenose dolphins and harbor porpoise on the U.S. West Coast (Cotter et al. 2012). The most relevant available data indicate that bottle- nose dolphins in California feed primarily on surfperches (family Embiotocidae)and croakers (family Sciaenidae;Defranet al. 1999 and references therein), whereas harbor porpoises in this region feed mainly on market squid (Doryteuthis opalescens) and small forage fish (Cotter et al. 2012 and references therein). Harbor porpoise stomachs col- lected from stranded in Monterey Bay contained primarily northern anchovy (Engraulis mordax), spotted cusk-eel (Chilara taylori), rockfish (family Sebastes), and market squid (Dorfman 1990, Sekiguchi 1995, Byrd 2001). More recent analyses of stranded animals in central California confirmed northern anchovy, market squid, and sardine (Sardinops sagax) as harbor porpoise prey items.3 Although not conclusive, these data indicate that harbor porpoise are generalists and may have seasonal or inter- annual variation in preferred prey types. The limited evidence for dietary overlap in combination with known differences in depth distributions for these two species indi- cate that competition for resources is an improbable explanation for the observed bot- tlenose dolphin aggression in California.

2NOAA Fisheries West Coast Region, 501 West Ocean Boulevard, Suite 4200, Long Beach, CA 90802. Unpublished data obtained in July 2013. 3Michelle Berman, Department of Vertebrate Zoology, Santa Barbara Museum of Natural History, 2559 Puesta del Sol, Santa Barbara, CA 93105. Unpublished data obtained in February 2013. 388 MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, VOL. 31, NO. 1, 2015

The prevailing hypothesis for bottlenose dolphin aggression towards harbor porpoises is that it is either a misdirection of infanticidal tendencies or a way of devel- oping infanticidal behaviors (Patterson et al. 1998, Kaplan et al. 2009, Cotter et al. 2012). In Moray Firth, Scotland, stranded bottlenose dolphin calves presented inter- nal and external injuries consistent with infanticide (Patterson et al. 1998) at the same time that harbor porpoise strandings were dominated by dolphin-inflicted blunt force trauma (Ross and Wilson 1996). Infanticide also has been observed in bottle- nose dolphins in Virginia (Dunn et al. 2002) and Florida (Kaplan et al. 2009). It is likely that infanticidal behavior was previously observed in bottlenose dolphins but was documented by researchers as play rather than aggression (Dunn et al. 2002). In some of these instances, attacked individuals of both species have been similarly sized (100–150 cm; Ross and Wilson 1996, Patterson et al. 1998, Dunn et al.2002).This link with infanticide is supported by some observations that aggressors are males (Kaplan et al. 2009, Cotter et al. 2012) who may use infanticide as a strategy to gain reproductive access to females (Kaplan et al. 2009). In California 92% of bottlenose dolphin aggressors in observed attacks were known or putative males, and there was 40% overlap in bottlenose dolphin identity between attacks (Cotter et al. 2012). However, there may be other explanations for the observed interspecies aggression since not all stranded harbor porpoises with dolphin-inflicted injuries fall within this size range (Wilkin et al. 2012), bottlenose dolphins have been observed attacking other delphinids (e.g., Barnett et al. 2009), and infanticide has never been docu- mented in bottlenose dolphins in California (Cotter et al. 2012). In the present study, we investigated whether this documented conflict between bottlenose dolphins and harbor porpoises in California affected harbor porpoise behavior within the nearshore environment where habitat overlap occurs with bottle- nose dolphins. We expected to find lower densities of harbor porpoises when bottle- nose dolphins were present than when they were absent. We used data from moored click detectors (C-PODs; Chelonia Ltd., http:// www.chelonia.co.uk) which were deployed at a study site in Monterey Bay (Fig. 1) to quantify the relative presence of harbor porpoises as well as bottlenose dolphins. These data were collected as part of a separate study but provided an opportunity to examine the acoustic behavior of both species within a few kilometers of the sites where bottlenose dolphin attacks on harbor porpoises were observed by Cotter et al. (2012) in previous years. Our instruments were moored in Monterey Bay at 36°52053″N, 121°50031″W approximately 15 m deep and 6 m above the sea floor. We used a mooring design that required diver installation and retrieval but allowed C-PODs to be serviced by hand from a small boat. Our C-POD installation was active from 7 October to 30 December 2011, for a total of 2,016 one-hour sample periods. On 1 November we serviced the mooring and exchanged C-PODs. C-PODs have been used in a variety of acoustic monitoring applications (e.g.,Gal- lus et al. 2012, Teilmann and Carstensen 2012, Benke et al. 2014). C-PODs record narrow-band high-frequency (120–140 kHz) porpoise echolocation clicks and mid- frequency (30–60 kHz) dolphin echolocation clicks. C-PODs detect and record only echolocation signals; therefore we have no information about other odontocete vocal- izations, including whistles. Summary information for each echolocation signal detected is stored on a flash memory card. In postprocessing, the data were extracted using the C-POD software v. 2.024 and signal detections were classified into echolo- cation click trains using a manufacturer-supplied KERNO classifier (Tregenza 2012). The KERNO classifier identifies trains based on similarity of successive interclick intervals and the coherence of click parameters including peak frequency and NOTES 389

Figure 1. Study site in Monterey Bay, California. The yellow star indicates the position of the C-POD mooring. The approximate distribution of bottlenose dolphins is indicated in red (dark red: high density, 0–500 m from shore; light red: low density, 500–1,000 m from shore). The approximate distribution of harbor porpoises is indicated in blue (dark blue: high density, 0–40 m depth; medium blue: low density, 40–100 m depth). duration. The inclusion of inter-click interval as a detection criterion reduces the false positive error rate since high-frequency noise from sediment or surf conditions is not patterned like cetacean click trains. For our analyses, we filtered the data to include only the highest quality echolocation click trains. We used acoustic data exclusively to quantify harbor porpoise and bottlenose dol- phin presence in the study area. At our study location, bottlenose dolphins and harbor porpoises are the only commonly encountered odontocetes. To confirm species identi- fications, we used data collected during six days of cliff-top observations and six days of aerial line-transect surveys that were conducted as part of a separate study. During both of these visual surveys we observed harbor porpoises and bottlenose dolphins in the vicinity of the C-POD and did not observe any other odontocetes. Therefore, we have attributed all narrow-band high-frequency click trains to harbor porpoise and 390 MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, VOL. 31, NO. 1, 2015 all mid-frequency click trains to bottlenose dolphins. The maximum detection range of a C-POD for harbor porpoise is approximately 400 m, whereas bottlenose dolphins may be detected from more than 1,000 m (Chelonia Ltd. 2012). Since the number of echolocation click trains increases nonlinearly with harbor porpoise density, we used a relative rather than absolute metric of echolocation activ- ity. Before analysis, echolocation click trains were binned into one-hour periods. We chose hour-long sampling periods to reduce autocorrelation in the data set while also preserving the signal of bottlenose dolphin encounters, which occur on timescales of one to a few hours. Echolocation click trains were reported as either a Proportion of Positive Minutes (PPM) or as a count of positive minutes in each hour. A positive minute was defined as containing at least one echolocation click train. Additionally, in some analyses bottlenose dolphins were counted simply as absent or present for each hour. The C-PODs also recorded in situ temperature measurements every min- ute, which were averaged for each hour period. Because our instruments were moored successively rather than in tandem, we are unable to quantify possible instrumental differences in sensitivity. However, our chosen metric of PPM is robust to variation in instrumental sensitivity (Benke et al. 2014). To validate our data set, we randomly selected a subsample of 1,000 min for man- ual confirmation. We calculated a false positive error rate for harbor porpoise positive minutes of 4% and a false positive error rate for bottlenose dolphin positive minutes of 0%. The false negative error rate is likely high for both species, however, for our application it is preferable to avoid false detections in the data set at the potential expense of missed detections. Preliminary examination of the data indicated a negative relationship between har- bor porpoise presence and bottlenose dolphin presence. To explore this relationship, we examined changes in harbor porpoise PPM during discrete encounters with bottlenose dolphins using a boxplot (Fig. 2). Encounters were defined as single or consecutive hours with bottlenose dolphins present. We collated one- (n = 65), two- (n = 28), and three-hour (n = 7) bottlenose dolphin encounters and calculated harbor porpoise vocal activity before (T0), during (T1–T3), and after (T2–T4) these encoun- ters. Qualitatively, we found that harbor porpoise acoustic presence at our study site decreased during bottlenose dolphin encounters. We performed a randomization test (Efron and Tibshirani 1993) to examine whether this observed pattern of lower mean harbor porpoise PPM when bottlenose dolphins were present (mean PPM = 0.06) compared with when they were absent (mean PPM = 0.18) could have occurred by chance (Fig. 3). This was accomplished by shifting the record of bottlenose dolphin presence to a new, randomly generated starting time and recalculating the mean har- bor porpoise PPM when bottlenose dolphins were artificially present. We repeated this process 10,000 times to produce the distribution shown in Figure 3. Our ran- domization procedure was designed to preserve autocorrelation in the original time series. The randomization test results indicated that the observed harbor porpoise mean PPM was significantly less when bottlenose dolphins were present compared with the distribution of values expected by chance alone (P = 0.0002) thus the observed pattern was extremely unlikely to have occurred by chance. Possible explanations for the observed decrease at our study site include niche par- titioning (i.e., habitat characteristics drive changes in species presence) and avoidance behavior (i.e., harbor porpoises avoid potential aggression by bottlenose dolphins by either changing their distribution or their acoustic behavior). Harbor porpoise and bottlenose dolphins have little diet overlap and different depth distributions, supporting a lack of competition for resources, and have NOTES 391

One−hour encounters, n=65 Two−hour encounters, n=28 Three−hour encounters, n=7 1.0

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2 Harbor Porpoise PPM Harbor Porpoise

0.0 0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1 Bottlenose Dolphin PPM

0.0 T0 T1 T2 T0 T1 T2 T3 T0 T1 T2 T3 T4

Figure 2. Box-and-whisker plots illustrating changes in the Positive Minutes (PPM) per hour for harbor porpoise (top panels) and bottlenose dolphins (bottom panels) before, during, and after all one-, two-, and three-hour bottlenose dolphin encounters in Monterey Bay. The black horizontal line indicates the median PPM value in each time period, while the blue shaded region represents the range between the first and third quartiles. The dashed lines show the range of the minimum and maximum values and the dots are outlier points that are more than 1.5 times the interquartile range beyond the first and third quartiles. T1 represents the onset of bottlenose dolphin presence and the red shaded regions indicate hours when bottle- nose dolphins were present. At T0 and at the final period in each plot (T2, T3, or T4, depend- ing on length of encounter), bottlenose dolphins were absent. Qualitatively, harbor porpoise PPM decreases when bottlenose dolphins are present and increases after they depart. fundamentally different ecological niches. Best available data indicate that suitable habitat for both species is defined primarily in terms of depth, proximity to shore, and temperature (Fig. 1; Barlow 1988, Forney 1999, Carretta et al. 2001). Coastal bottlenose dolphins are found primarily within 1 km of shore (Hansen 1990, Defran et al. 1999), whereas harbor porpoise are less constrained by distance from shore and are more broadly distributed throughout waters 0–100 m deep, with greatest densi- ties in depths from 0 to 40 m (Barlow 1988, Carretta et al. 2001, Forney et al. 2001). Because depth and proximity to shore are static, and temperature does not change rapidly enough to explain the observed changes in relative dolphin and por- poise presence, we do not believe that changes in habitat suitability could drive the observed periods of simultaneous increase in bottlenose dolphin presence and decrease in harbor porpoise presence in Monterey Bay. Monterey Bay is at the northern end of the range of bottlenose dolphins (Hansen 1990, Defran et al. 1999). As water temperatures decreased during our study period the frequency of bottlenose dolphin encounters decreased. Bottlenose dolphin pres- 392 MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, VOL. 31, NO. 1, 2015 Frequency 0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000

0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30 Randomized Mean Harbor Porpoise PPM

Figure 3. Blue bars indicate the randomization distribution of mean harbor porpoise Positive Minutes (PPM) during bottlenose dolphin encounters (mean = 0.18). The black arrow is the true mean harbor porpoise PPM during bottlenose dolphin encounters (mean = 0.06). There are no possible randomizations that result in a lower-than-observed mean PPM value when bottlenose dolphins are present. ence at our study site could be considered as a stochastic occurrence moderated by temperature. We were interested in modeling the effect of bottlenose dolphin pres- ence on counts of harbor porpoise positive minutes, but were concerned that changes in habitat might affect dolphin and porpoise presence. To explicitly include possible environmental influences, we included both dolphin presence and temperature as pre- dictor variables of harbor porpoise positive minutes. Because of the large proportion (29%) of zeros and overdispersion of counts in the harbor porpoise acoustic record, the data do not fit standard negative binomial or Poisson distributions. We tested for the goodness-of-fit of Generalized Linear Models specified with negative binomial and Poisson distributions using a chi-square test on the residual deviance and degrees of freedom. These tests indicated that models fitted using these distributions did not fit the data (chi-square tests, P < 0.05). Therefore, we chose a zero-inflated negative binomial (ZINB) model (Zeileis et al. 2008). This is a two-part model, with zeros generated by a binomial model with a logit link and counts generated by a negative binomial model with a log link. This model formula- tion allows zeros to be generated by either part of the model. Results of the ZINB model indicated that the data contained significantly more zeros than expected under standard distributions and that the data are overdispersed (Theta = 0.49). We use Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) to choose between possible combinations of predictor variables in the ZINB model formulation (Table 1). NOTES 393

Table 1. Partial list of zero-inflated negative binomial (ZINB) model parameters for the binomial and negative binomial portions of the model with their degrees of freedom (DF) and AIC scores. A “+” indicates that both variables were included, whereas a “9” indicates that both variables and their interaction term were included in the model. The best model is in bold.

Binomial model (zeros) Negative binomial model (counts) DF AIC Temperature Temperature 5 12,768.71 Dolphin Presence Dolphin Presence 5 12,731.62 Temp 9 Dolphin Temp 9 Dolphin 9 12,692.38 Temp 9 Dolphin Temp + Dolphin 8 12,692.48 Temp + Dolphin Temp 9 Dolphin 8 12,708.23

The best model includes temperature, dolphin presence, and their interaction to model the binomial process (probability of occurrence), and temperature and dolphin presence without their interaction to model the count (number of acoustic detections given occurrence). The model with interaction terms in both parts of the model had a slightly better AIC score (DAIC = 0.10) but the interaction term was not significant in the count portion of the model (Z-test, P = 0.14). The best-fit model, with 95% confi- dence intervals, is shown in Figure 4. Both the presence of dolphins and temperature were significant in both parts of this best model (Z-test, P < 0.05 for all coefficients). The models including either only temperature or only dolphin presence performed poorly, indicating that neither is adequate to explain harbor porpoise acoustic activity.

20 Dolphins Absent

15

10 Dolphins Present 5 Harbor Porpoise Positive Minutes per Hour Minutes Positive Harbor Porpoise

0 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Temperature (°C)

Figure 4. Fitted zero-inflated negative binomial (ZINB) model (red: dolphins present; black: dolphins absent) with 95% confidence intervals (shaded areas) indicating that harbor porpoise positive minutes per hour increased with temperature when dolphins were absent. 394 MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, VOL. 31, NO. 1, 2015

Our model predicts that when bottlenose dolphins are present, harbor porpoise acoustic activity is reduced by 52% at relatively low temperatures and by 82% at higher temperatures compared with when bottlenose dolphins are absent. Tempera- ture decreased during our study, so this result may reflect seasonal changes in the rela- tive abundances of harbor porpoises and bottlenose dolphins at our study site. Our data indicated that both species were relatively more abundant in October than in December. The magnitude of the harbor porpoise response to bottlenose dolphin presence may be heightened when both species are relatively more abundant. We hypothesized that harbor porpoises passively detect bottlenose dolphin vocal- izations in Monterey Bay and respond by reducing vocalization rates or leaving the area. The narrow-band high-frequency echolocation clicks of harbor porpoises have evolved independently in three odontocete lineages (Madsen et al. 2005). It has been suggested that use of these frequencies by harbor porpoises prevents passive acoustic detection by killer (Madsen et al. 2005, Rankin et al. 2013) whose hearing sen- sitivity declines above 100 kHz (Szymanski et al. 1999). Rankin et al. (2013) found that the vocal activity of dolphins decreased with proximity to killer whales, indicat- ing that they used stealth to avoid passive detection and predation by killer whales. Harbor porpoises have sensitive hearing in the range from 16 to 140 kHz (Kaste- lein et al. 2002), so they are able to hear echolocation clicks of bottlenose dolphins, which range in frequency from 30 to 110 kHz (Wahlberg et al. 2011). Bottlenose dolphin hearing is most sensitive in the range of 15–110 kHz (Johnson 1967, Brill et al. 2001, Popov et al. 2007) although they have limited hearing up to 150 kHz (Popov et al. 2007) and there is evidence that high-frequency hearing diminishes with age (Houser and Finneran 2006). Free-ranging harbor porpoise echolocation clicks are produced at a peak frequency of 129–145 kHz with a source level of 191 dB (Villadsgaard et al. 2007). Clicks with these parameters would attenuate in Mon- terey Bay seawater at a rate of approximately 40 dB per km (Fisher and Simmons 1977). Bottlenose dolphin hearing cutoff frequencies are 80 dB at 130 kHz (Popov et al. 2007). From these parameters, we might estimate that, bottlenose dolphins would be able to hear harbor porpoises echolocating within about 2 km. However, experimental work on the propagation of artificial harbor porpoise clicks in harbor porpoise habitats has found transmission loss of 61 dB at ranges of 200 m (DeRuiter et al. 2010) indicating that the true bottlenose dolphin detection range of harbor por- poise echolocation clicks may be <500 m. Since bottlenose dolphins could be pas- sively or actively detecting harbor porpoises, we are unable to resolve whether harbor porpoises are responding to bottlenose dolphin presence by leaving the nearshore area or by reducing vocal activity to avoid detection. In summary, our data indicated that harbor porpoise avoided bottlenose dolphins in Monterey Bay, possibly to reduce their risk of being attacked. There are two potential effects of bottlenose dolphin aggression on harbor porpoises in Monterey Bay. First, mortality that results from bottlenose dolphin attacks may directly impact harbor por- poise populations. The Monterey Bay population is approximately 3,715 individuals and appears to be increasing following past impacts from gill net bycatch (KAF, unpublished data). During the UME of harbor porpoises off central California, blunt force trauma was the most common identified cause of death (Wilkin et al. 2012), however, if this additional source of mortality was causing population declines, we would be unlikely to detect it given the frequency of past survey efforts and the precision of abundance and trends (Forney et al. 1991). The second potential impact of bottlenose dolphin aggression on harbor porpoises is a reduction in fitness as a result of exclusion from suitable habitat in the nearshore environment. This impact would NOTES 395 be difficult to quantify. The population of coastal bottlenose dolphins in California is estimated to be 323 individuals (Dudzik et al. 2006). Cotter et al. (2012) reported the identities of 23 individual bottlenose dolphins that have participated in attacks, sug- gesting a minimum involvement of 7% of the bottlenose dolphin population. If this behavior continues to spread through the coastal bottlenose dolphin population in California, direct mortality and habitat exclusion could negatively impact harbor por- poise populations.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Moss Landing Marine Laboratories Small Boat Center, especially John Doug- las and the scientific divers for assistance with field operations. Erica Burton assisted us with Monterey Bay National Marine Sanctuary Permit MBNMNS-2011-026, under which this research was conducted. Sarah Wilkin, Michelle Berman, and the stranding network for Mon- terey and Santa Cruz counties contributed valuable data on harbor porpoise strandings and diets of harbor porpoise and bottlenose dolphins. Jay Barlow, Jeff Moore, R. H. Defran, John Hildebrand, and two anonymous reviewers provided helpful reviews of an earlier version of this manuscript.

Literature Cited

Barlow, J. 1988. Harbor porpoise, Phocoena phocoena, abundance estimation for California, Oregon, and Washington: 1. Ship surveys. Fishery Bulletin 86:417–432. Barnett, J., N. Davison, R. Deaville, R. Monies, J. Loveridge, N. Tregenza and P. D. Jepson. 2009. Postmortem evidence of interactions of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) with other dolphin species in south-west England. Veterinary Record 165:441–444. Benke, H., S. Br€ager, M. D€ahne, et al. 2014. Baltic Sea populations: Status and conservation needs derived from recent survey results. Marine Ecology Progress Series 495:275–290. Brill, R. L., P. W. B. Moore and L. A. Dankiewicz. 2001. Assessment of dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) auditory sensitivity and hearing loss using jawphones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 109:1717–1722. Byrd, B. L. 2001. Abundance, distribution, food habits, and prey availability of the harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in northern Monterey Bay, California. M.S. thesis, California State University, Stanislaus, CA. 97 pp. Calambokidis, J., and J. Barlow. 1991. Chlorinated hydrocarbon concentrations and their use for describing population discreteness in harbor porpoises from Washington, Oregon, and California. U.S. Department of Commerce, NOAA Technical Report NMFS 98:101–110. Carretta, J. V., K. A. Forney and J. L. Laake. 1998. Abundance of southern California coastal bottlenose dolphins estimated from tandem aerial surveys. Marine Mammal Science 14:655–675. Carretta, J. V., B. L. Taylor and S. J. Chivers. 2001. Abundance and depth distribution of harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in northern California determined from a 1995 ship survey. Fishery Bulletin 99:29–39. Carretta, J. V., K. A. Forney and S. R. Benson. 2009. Preliminary estimates of harbor porpoise abundance in California waters from 2002 to 2007. U.S. Department of Commerce, NOAA Technical Memorandum NMFS SWFSC-435. 14 pp. Chelonia Ltd. 2012. C-POD specification. Available at http://www.chelonia.co.uk/ cpod_specification.htm. 396 MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, VOL. 31, NO. 1, 2015

Chivers, S. J., A. E. Dizon, P. J. Gearin and K. M. Robertson. 2002. Small-scale population structure of eastern North Pacific harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) indicated by molecular genetic analyses. Journal of Cetacean Research and Management 4:111–122. Cotter, M. P., D. Maldini and T. A. Jefferson. 2012. “Porpicide” in California: Killing of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) by coastal bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Marine Mammal Science 28:E1–E15. Defran, R. H., D. W. Weller, D. L. Kelly and M. A. Espinosa. 1999. Range characteristics of pacific coast bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the southern California bight. Marine Mammal Science 15:381–393. DeRuiter, S. L., M. Hansen, H. N. Koopman, A. J. Westgate, P. L. Tyack and P. T. Madsen. 2010. Propagation of narrow-band-high-frequency clicks: Measured and modeled transmission loss of porpoise-like clicks in porpoise habitats. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 127:560–567. Dorfman, E. J. 1990. Distribution, behavior, and food habits of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in Monterey Bay. M.S. thesis, San Jose State University, San Jose, CA. 57 pp. Dudzik, K. J., K. M. Baker and D. W. Weller. 2006. Mark-recapture abundance estimate of California coastal stock bottlenose dolphins: February 2004 to April 2005. National Marine Fisheries Service Center Administrative Report LJ-06-02C (unpublished). 15 pp. Available from the Southwest Fisheries Science Center, 8901 La Jolla Shores Dr., La Jolla, CA 92037. Dunn, D., S. Barco and D. Pabst. 2002. Evidence for infanticide in bottlenose dolphins of the Western North Atlantic. Journal of Wildlife Diseases 38:505–510. Efron, B., and R. J. Tibshirani. 1993. An introduction to the bootstrap. Monographs on Applied Statistics and Applied Probability 57. Chapman and Hall, New York, NY. Fisher, F. H., and V. P. Simmons. 1977. Sound absorption in seawater. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 62:558–564. Forney, K. A. 1999. Trends in harbour porpoise abundance off central California, 1986–95: Evidence for interannual changes in distribution. Journal of Cetacean Research and Management 1:73–80. Forney, K. A., D. A. Hanan and J. Barlow. 1991. Detecting trends in harbor porpoise abundance from aerial surveys using analysis of covariance. Fishery Bulletin 89:367–377. Forney, K. A., S. R. Benson and G. A. Cameron. 2001. Central California gillnet effort and bycatch of sensitive species, 1990–98. Pages 141–160 in E. F. Melvin and J. K. Parrish, eds. Seabird bycatch: Trends, roadblocks and solutions. University of Alaska Sea Grant Publication AK-SG-01-01, Fairbanks, Alaska. 212 pp. Gallus, A., M. D€ahne, U. K. Verfuß, S. Br€ager, S. Adler, U. Siebert and H. Benke. 2012. Use of static passive acoustic monitoring to assess the status of the “Critically Endangered” Baltic harbour porpoise in German waters. Endangered Species Research 18:265–278. Hansen, L. J. 1990. California coastal bottlenose dolphins. Pages 403–420 in S. Leatherwood and R. R. Reeves, eds. The bottlenose dolphin. Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, CA. Hansen, M. T., and R. H. Defran. 1993. The behavior and feeding ecology of the Pacific coast bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Aquatic 19:127–142. Houser, D. S., and J. J. Finneran. 2006. Variation in the hearing sensitivity of a dolphin population determined through the use of evoked potential audiometry. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120:4090–4099. Hwang, A., R. H. Defran, M. Bearzi, D. Maldini, et al. 2014. Coastal range and movements of common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off California and Baja California, Mexico. Bulletin of the Southern California Academy of Sciences 113:1–13. Johnson, C. S. 1967. Sound detection thresholds in marine mammals. Pages 247–260 in W. N. Tavolga, ed. Marine bio-acoustics. Volume 2. Pergamon Press, New York, NY. Jepson, P. D., and J. R. Baker. 1998. Bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) as a possible cause of acute traumatic injuries in porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Veterinary Record 143:614–615. NOTES 397

Kaplan, J. D., B. J. Lentell and W. Lange. 2009. Possible evidence for infanticide among bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off St. Augustine, Florida. Marine Mammal Science 25:970–975. Kastelein, R. A., P. Bunskoek, M. Hagedoom, W. W. L. Au and D. de Haan. 2002. Audiogram ofaharborporpoise(Phocoena phocoena) measured with narrow-band frequency modulated signals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 112:334–344. Madsen, P. E., D. A. Carder, K. Bedholm and S. H. Ridgway. 2005. Porpoise clicks from a sperm nose—convergent evolution of 130 kHz pulses in sonars? Bioacoustics 15:195–206. Patterson, I., R. J. Reid, B. Wilson, K. Grellier, H. M. Ross and P. M. Thompson. 1998. Evidence for infanticide in bottlenose dolphins: An explanation for violent interactions with harbour porpoises? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 265:1167–1170. Popov, V. V., A. Y. Supin, M. G. Pletenko, M. B. Tarakanov, V. O. Klishin, T. N. Bulgakova and E. I. Rosanova. 2007. Audiogram variability in normal bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals 33:24–33. Rankin, S., F. Archer and J. Barlow. 2013. Vocal activity of tropical dolphins is inhibited by the presence of killer whales, Orcinus orca. Marine Mammal Science 29:679–690. Ross, H. M., and B. Wilson. 1996. Violent interactions between bottlenose dolphins and harbour porpoises. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 263:283–286. Sekiguchi, K. 1995. Occurrence and behavior of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) at Pajaro Dunes, Monterey Bay, California. Aquatic Mammals 21:91–103. Spitz, J., Y. Rosseau and Y. Ridoux. 2006. Diet overlap between harbour porpoise and bottlenose dolphin: An argument in favour of interference competition for food? Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 70:259–270. Szymanski, M. D., D. E. Bain, K. Kiehl, S. Pennington, S. Wong and K. R. Henry. 1999. (Orcinus orca) hearing: Auditory brainstem response and behavioral audiograms. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106:1134–1141. Teilmann, J., and J. Carstensen. 2012. Negative long term effects on harbour porpoises from a large scale offshore wind farm in the Baltic–evidence of slow recovery. Environmental Research Letters 7:045101. Tregenza, N. 2012. CPOD.exe: A guide for users. Available at http://www.chelonia.co.uk/ downloads/CPOD.pdf. Villadsgaard, A., M. Wahlberg and J. Tougaard. 2007. Echolocation signals of wild harbour porpoises, Phocoena phocoena. The Journal of Experimental Biology 210:56–64. Wahlberg, M., F. H. Jensen, N. Aguilar Soto, et al. 2011. Source parameters of echolocation clicks from wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus and Tursiops truncatus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 130:2263–2274. Wells, R., L. Hansen, A. Baldridge, D. Dohl, D. L. Kelly and R. H. Defran. 1990. Northward extension of the range of bottlenose dolphins along the California coast. Pages 421–431 in S. Leatherwood and R. R. Reeves, eds. The bottlenose dolphin. Academic Press, San Diego, CA. Wilkin, S. M., J. Cordaro and F. Gulland. 2012. An unusual mortality event of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) off central California: Increase in blunt force trauma rather than an epizootic. Aquatic Mammals 38:301–310. Zeileis, A., C. Kleiber and S. Jackman. 2008. Regression models for count data in R. Journal of Statistical Software 27:1–25.

Received: 27 September 2013 Accepted: 12 June 2014