THE EFFECTS OF WIND ON FORAGING SRATEGIES OF CEPHALOTES LEAF- CUTTER

By MICHAEL JOHN RODRIGUEZ B.S., Southeastern Louisiana University, 2011

A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Colorado in partial fulfillment

of the requirement for the degree of Master of Science Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology 2014

This thesis entitled: The effects of wind on foraging strategies of leaf-cutter ants written by Michael John Rodriguez has been approved for the Department Ecology and Evolutionary Biology

Dr. Michael Breed

_____ Dr. Rebecca Safran

Date

The final copy of this thesis has been examined by the signatories, and we Find that both the content and the form meet acceptable presentation standards Of scholarly work in the above mentioned discipline.

iii

Rodriguez, Michael John (M.S., Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology)

The effects of wind on foraging strategies of Atta cephalotes leaf-cutter ants

Thesis directed by Professor Dr. Michael Breed

Abstract: Successful foragers alter their behavior in response to variation in local conditions, resulting in reduction of foraging costs and maximization of resource gain. In eusocial colonies, individuals may adjust their own efforts to maximize the productivity of the colony as a whole.

Maximization of colony productivity can be achieved through sub-maximal individual performance. Attine leaf-cutter foragers often cut leaf fragments shorter than the hind legs could allow, suggesting other factors contribute to load size determination. Several studies have shown reasons why leaf-cutter ants cut smaller loads than they could maximally carry. The effects of wind speed on leaf-cutter load size selection were examined in this study and showed conditions in which leaf-cutter foragers change their behavior to cut larger loads than normal. In response to wind treatments, foragers cut larger leaf loads and fewer minima workers hitchhiked on those leaves. This study highlights behavioral plasticity of leaf-cutter foragers in response to local conditions on the foraging trail, and it adds to our knowledge of resource allocation in central-place foraging systems.

Key words: behavioral flexibility, ethology, foraging, load size selection, parasitism, social

iv

Acknowledgements

Special thanks for funding from the Organization for Tropical Studies, the University of

Colorado, Boulder Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Colorado Diversity

Initiative, and Petridish.org contributors including: Sandra Blair, Michael Bonner, Kevin

Westmoreland, Jasmine Hegman, Nicole Gorden, James Waters, Jason Steiner, Samuel J. Ciurca,

Jr., James Jorasch, Gaurav Vaidya, Jaime Pawelek, Daniel Ewald, Kwame Hagan, Nicholas

Barbieri, Bob Peake, Kenneth Trease, Kelly Stewart, Mustafa, Susan Fay, and Alan F.

Rodriguez. Thank you to the staff and researchers of La Selva and the participants and instructors of OTS Tropical Biology course 2012-1. Thanks to my adviser, Dr. Michael Breed, and lab mates for guidance and reviews. Thank you to Dr. Catherine and Mac Macgregor.

v

CONTENTS

Introduction...... 1

Leaf-cutter ant background...... 2

Foraging efficiency ...... 3-7

Hitchhiker ants ...... 7

Purpose of the study ...... 8

Methods ...... 9-13

Results...... 13-19

Discussion ...... 20-24

Conclusion ...... 24-25

References ...... 26-30

vi

FIGURES

Figure

1. Leaf fragment area between wind treatments ...... 14

2. Leaf fragment area and forager head width...... 15

3. Forager head width between wind treatments ...... 15

4. Interaction of forager head width and leaf fragment area between wind treatments...... 16

5. Hitchhiker numbers between wind treatments ...... 17

6. Foraging trail traffic density and leaf fragment mass ...... 18

7. Leaf fragment area between leaf type ...... 19

8. Forager head width between leaf type ...... 19

1

Introduction

Successful foragers alter their behavior in response to a shifting external environment

(Charnov 1976). This results in reduction of foraging costs and maximization of resource gain if local conditions vary. In eusocial colonies, individuals specializing in certain tasks may adjust their own efforts to maximize the productivity of the colony as a whole (Burd 1996a; Constant et al. 2012). Theory predicts that individuals adjust their foraging efforts to achieve optimal resource input into the nest (Charnov 1976; Wetterer 1989; Burd 1996a; Burd and Howard

2008). Interactions among nestmates and external cues allow individuals to alter their behavioral effort, especially as different biotic (parasitism, predation, nest-mate interactions) and abiotic conditions (rain, wind, temperature) vary (Hölldobler and Wilson 1990; Feener and Moss 1990;

Gordon 2002; Angilletta et al. 2008; Elizalde and Folgarait 2012). Behavioral variability within a colony and behavioral flexibility displayed by individuals may contribute to the evolutionary success of colony phenotypes and to the ecological success of the eusocial (Wilson

1971; Constant et al. 2012). My results from studies of foraging in a leaf-cutter ant, Atta cephalotes, add to our understanding of the complex interplay between costs and benefits in central place foraging systems and how individual behavior and recruitment behavior of leaf- cutter ants is plastic in response to abiotic and biotic external conditions, such as wind (Rudolph and Loudon1986), traffic on the foraging trail (Farji-Brener et al. 2011), or the presence of very small workers, called minima, riding on a carried load (Feener and Brown 1992; Orr et al. 1995;

Porter et al. 1995).

Leaf-cutting ants 2

A small percentage of ant species have polymorphic workers (Wetterer 1994), and within the fungus growing ants, family Formicidae, subfamily , tribe Attini, only some species have workers with different size castes. Workers of the monomorphic Attini forage for carcasses, waste, and other detritus that do not require further processing. The polymorphic Attini are specialist foragers on vegetable matter (seeds, flowers, and leaves). This plant material needs special treatment to convert to a suitable substrate for fungal gardens grown by the ants and used as food. Handling of plant material in preparation for use in fungal gardening includes fragmentation, chewing, and regurgitation onto the fungal garden (Wilson

1980a, b).

Leaf-cutting ants in the genera Atta and Acromyrmex have highly polymorphic workers, resulting in a high division of labor with workers specializing on tasks appropriate for their size

(morphological polyethism) (Wilson 1980a, b). In Atta cephalotes leaf-cutter ants, the species addressed in this study, the largest workers can be 200 times the size of the smallest workers

(Weber 1972). The large, major, workers and intermediate, media, workers act as soldiers and foragers, made effective by their large bodies and mandibular muscles. The smaller, or minima, workers take care of the brood and garden and maintain the trails (Hughes and Goulson 2001).

Inside the nest, minima gardeners fragment the forage into a pulp that they feed to basiodiomycetes fungi which are cultured as the primary food source for their developing larvae.

To a lesser extent adults also feed on the fungi (Weber 1972; Quinlan & Cherrett 1979;

Benckiser 2011). The fungal gardens are analogous to human agriculture (Benckiser 2011).

Minima workers are primarily fungal caretakers but have a secondary role as defenders of adult ants against parasitic phorid flies (Phoridae: Pseudoacteon sp.). Minima workers hitchhike rides aboard leaf fragments carried back by larger workers (Wilson 1980a, b; Feener and Moss 3

1990; Vieira-Neto et al. 2006) and fend off these flies. Foragers can travel for hours over long distances on dedicated and manicured trails, recorded to span 300 m from the nest entrance, with a mean distance of 50 m (Lewis et al. 1974; Wetterer 1990). Foragers travelling back to the nest carrying loads are more vulnerable to parasitoid attack than unladen workers, and hitchhiking ants offer defense, rearing up on hind legs to grab the fly or to block oviposition (Feener and

Moss 1990; Vieira-Neto et al. 2006). Considering the long travel times of foragers and their vulnerability while carrying loads, hitchhikers offer a valuable defense from parasitoid enemies.

In addition to defense, hitchhikers sometimes work to remove trichomes on the leaf fragment, decrease parasitic fungal load, and retain sap leaking from the leaf fragment as it is carried

(Vieira-Neto et al. 2006; Kitayama et al. 2012).

Foraging Efficiency

In eusocial species, behaviors have evolved that maximize colony productivity over individual productivity (Burd 1996a). Optimal colony productivity can often be achieved through sub-maximal individual performance. Nuñez (1982) showed that Apis mellifera honeybees carry a less than maximal nectar load for faster recruitment of other workers to new resources. In a competitive environment with limited resources, scramble competition drives the location of, recruitment to, and monopolization of resources as quickly as possible (Hölldobler

1976; Brännström and Sumpter 2005). By spending only a moment to sample the nectar source and flying with a minimal load, the honeybee recruits nest-mates to the resource faster. Schmid-

Hempel et al. (1985) found that A. mellifera workers often return from a non-depleted nectar resource to their nest with half-filled crops, increasing flight efficiency at the expense of maximum nectar transport rate. Similarly, Jeanne (1999) showed that Polybia occidentalis 4 wasps bring back less nest construction material than they can maximally carry, increasing material handling efficiency at the nest and flying efficiency so that they can make more trips per unit time. Taylor (1977) demonstrated that Cataglyphis desert ants carry intermediate sized loads to decrease travel time and sun exposure. Burkhardt (1998) reported Pheidole ant workers from larger colonies spending less time drinking from a resource and more time recruiting than ants from smaller colonies. In each of these cases workers achieve maximum transport rate as a colony by balancing the costs that load mass creates for travel velocity.

Considering the energy required and the dangers associated with leaving the nest, central- place foraging theory predicts that leafcutter foragers should select loads that maximize resource delivery rate to the nest, (e.g. loads should be larger farther away from the central nest to compensate for longer travel times) (Wetterer 1989; Roces 1990b). As predicted by theory,

Roces (1990) found that Acromyrmex lundi leaf-cutter ants cut larger fragments the further they are from the nest, maximizing profitability. Cherrett (1972) found that smaller foragers cutting from a fruit resource give up cutting when faced with very tough surfaces, allowing them to switch to more time-efficient resources. Wetterer (1994) proposed that smaller workers disturb and displace larger workers at more tender surfaces ensuring that loads are cut by the smallest sized workers needed to accomplish the task. Counterintuitively, in leaf-cutter systems, the average loads carried by workers are below the size necessary to achieve maximal leaf tissue delivery rates (Burd 1996b, 2000, 2001).

Foraging leaf-cutter workers control the size of the leaf fragment they carry through their cutting behavior. Observers originally thought that the size of the leaf load was dictated by the size of the individual ant; early studies found a correlation between load mass and ant mass (Lutz

1929; Cherrett 1972; Rudolph and Loudon 1986; Waller 1986; Wetterer 1990). Lutz (1929) 5 proposed that leaf-cutter foragers pivot on their hind legs while cutting, establishing a fixed cutting arc determined by the length of the hind legs.

However, individuals often cut fragments shorter than the hind legs could allow, suggesting other factors contribute to determining load size (Roces and Nuñez 1993; Roces and

Hölldobler 1994; Burd and Howard 2005a,b; Dussutour et al. 2009a,b; Farji-Brener et al. 2011).

Comparing worker body length to the radius of the leaf fragment it carried, Rudolph and Loudon

(1986) found a weak correlation between fragment size and rear leg length, indicating that foragers often cut fragments smaller than their hind legs could allow. Also, foragers cutting denser leaves (the ratio of fragment area to volume) often cut smaller leaf fragments than foragers cutting less dense leaves, suggesting a behavioral mechanism whereby foragers might cut smaller loads than their body size would permit. Recent research demonstrates that patrilineal lineages within colonies vary significantly in foraging behavior and this can affect the size of the fragment cut, the time to cut it, and the transport velocity indicating genetic mechanisms altering cutting behavior (Constant et al. 2012).

Shape and size may be of great importance when selecting a load because of maneuverability. Dussutour et al. (2009b) found that placing an obstacle over the foraging trail affected the shape of leaves cut in a foraging arena. The foragers cut shorter (smaller) leaf fragments to fit back under the obstacle on their way back to the nest, showing individual behavioral plasticity in load size selection. Also, perhaps to compensate for the decreased biomass transport rate because of smaller fragments, more workers were recruited when the obstacle was present compared to when the foraging path was clear. Farji-Brener et al. (2011) found that traffic congestion on the foraging trail resulted in large foragers cutting lighter loads 6 to avoid slowing nestmates behind them like a semi-truck on a highway (the Truck Driver

Effect).

Previous studies on leaf-cutter ants establish that collection of larger fragments is more efficient in terms of biomass transported per unit of perimeter cut, but heavier loads decrease walking speed thereby increasing transport time to the colony (Rissing 1984; Rudolph and

Loudon 1986; Burd 1996a; personal observation). Maximal leaf transport rate occurs at the point where the load gets large enough to slow walking speed of laden ants enough to offset the biomass transport rate to the nest (Rudolph and Loudon 1986). The costs associated with larger loads slowing velocity are more pronounced at farther distances, and the ability to inform nest mates of a quality resource may affect load size in the early stages of foraging because of the relationship between load size and travel velocity.

Researchers have developed models to predict maximal biomass transport rate to the colony (Burd 1996a; Burd 2000; Burd and Howard 2005). The rest of this section discusses three models of biomass delivery in Atta cephalotes leaf-cutter systems. The first two models examine the foraging round trip outside the nest entrance, and the first model specifically focuses on traffic congestion on leaf cutting surfaces. The third model takes previous models into account and adds components of leaf processing time once inside the nest to determine optimal parameters maximal resource input rate.

Maximizing the load size for individual workers may result in sub-optimal colony-wide resource input because of queue time effects (waiting) in dense traffic situations (Burd 1996a).

There is a limited amount of surface area on a cutting surface (leaf), and leaf-cutter ant colonies may benefit from sub-optimal individual performance when congestion is high. Foragers cutting smaller leaf fragments experience a shorter cutting time. By cutting a smaller load than any 7 individual can maximally carry, server availability (cutting sites) is increased and more recruited individuals are able to access that resource per unit time, resulting in greater resource acquisition.

Cutting sites are vacated quickly and the colony receives greater biomass transfer rate. However,

Burd (1996a) admits that the model may not fit because in natural canopies, it is unknown if the density of workers reaches a high enough threshold to cause server queues and likely that workers spread out on individual leaves and within a canopy.

Burd (1996a) composed a second model for maximal biomass collection and transport back to the nest which removes any queue effects and takes into account colony size, ant mass, leaf density, cutting time as a function of ant size, and ant velocity as a function of ant and load mass. Burd (1996a) also took into account the physical space on a leaf that could be occupied for different distributions of ant size classes and set the parameters to maximize cutting speed.

Lastly, the model accounted for the distance from the nest to include outbound travel time, cutting time, and return travel time. Burd (1996a) compared this maximal biomass transfer model to Atta columbica ants naturally foraging on Barro Colorado Island, Panama and found that foragers cut loads below the size that would allow for maximal biomass transfer to the colony, implying more complex rules exist for foraging than obtaining the largest load possible.

Burd and Howard (2005b) presented a new model for leaf tissue delivery to and inside the nest after cutting and discovered optimality in the model when fragments were predicted to be intermediate in size. The model took several factors into account such as the number of laden foragers, the distance from the nest to the resource, the outbound speed of ants, the inbound speed as a function of load size (leaf fragment area), the transfer time through the nest as a function of load size, the capacity of fugal garden chambers, and the size and nutritional state of 8 the colony. Empirical data have not confirmed the model, but their finding points to potential effects of processing leaf fragments inside the nest on load size selection.

Hitchhiking minima

The presence or absence of hitchhiking leaf-cutter minima workers may also affect load size selection. These minima workers are found on the foraging trail and often ride back to the nest (“hitchhike”) on a leaf fragment carried by a larger worker. Griffiths and Hughes (2010) found evidence that minima hitchhikers act in a leaf cleansing role, observing hitchhikers riding longer on contaminant-treated loads and fewer fungal contaminants after hitchhikers had access to a load. However, another important theory is that hitchhiking behavior primarily evolved in defense against parasitoid phorid flies which lay eggs in the ant body (Hölldobler and Wilson

1990; Feener and Moss 1990; Orr 1992). The load carrier holding the leaf fragment in its mandibles cannot defend itself from harassing parasitoids and benefits from the protection provided by the hitchhiker. Feener and Moss (1990) conclude that the role of hitchhikers is to defend ant workers from phorid flies and that the presence of the flies can result in an increase in hitchhiking behavior by the minima caste of ants (see also Braganca et al. 1998). It stands to reason that the absence of the flies (which may not be able to fly in windy conditions) could result in a decrease in hitchhiking behavior.

Hypotheses

The focus of this study is the abiotic factor of wind. Wind and rain are common occurrences in the tropics and have consequences for ants foraging in tree canopies, potentially limiting the area and mass (respectively) of loads foragers cut and transport (Rudolph and 9

Loudon 1986). Attines carry loads several times their own body size and run a risk of falling large distances from trees to the ground or of losing their balance when walking on the ground

(Moll et al. 2010), but foragers should cut the maximal load that still permits stability while walking. To maximize resource input rate to the nest in windy conditions, foragers could cut smaller loads to maintain a stable walking course and minimize energy expenditure against wind resistance. I tested the hypothesis that variation in wind speed is correlated with the size of leaf fragments carried or the size of foragers recruited to the foraging site.

During initial observations, I noticed a marked absence of hitchhikers on leaf loads in windy conditions, so I also examined the effects of wind on the prevalence of hitchhikers riding on leaf fragments. The second goal of this study was to determine if hitchhiker density decreases in windy conditions.

Materials and Methods

I conducted this study at La Selva Biological Station, Heredia Province, Costa Rica in

June and July 2012 and January 2014. La Selva Biological station is located in the Northeastern lowlands of Costa Rica in premontane wet forest and disturbed lands, receiving an average of 4 m rainfall per year. The focal colonies in this study were well established and large colonies with heavy foraging patterns on leaf sources. The focal colonies foraged nocturnally, and I ran trials between 1400 and 2000 h each night on three A. cephalotes leaf-cutter nests in a short grass clearing, each separated by at least 50 m. Peak foraging occurred around 1730 h each night.

A. cephalotes is a good model for load size selection because individuals usually carry the load that they cut and must carry that burden back to the nest (Hubbell et al. 1980). The round trip journey to a resource can last for several hours in A. cephalotes (Lewis et al. 1974), 10 and mature colonies contain millions of individuals (Weber 1972, Hölldobler and Wilson 1990) with thousands out foraging at any given time (Fowler and Saes 1986), therefore it is unlikely I sampled the same individual more than once in the same night. Prior observers document A. cephalotes workers passing leaf fragments from one individual to another (task partitioning)

(Hubbell et al. 1980), but I found no evidence for leaf transfer in my observations and set experimental methodology to avoid this problem.

A. cephalotes colonies may harvest leaves from the same tree for weeks (Burd 2000), and

I used only vegetation sources that the colonies were actively foraging (Myrtaceae: Psidium friedrichstalianum and Hamelia patens). Leaf-cutter colonies can forage from multiple trees at once, so I controlled for potential differences in intraspecific leaf density by collecting treatment leaves from the same tree (Cherrett 1972; Rudolph and Loudon 1986). To control for any individual differences in leaf density between leaves from the same tree, I chose leaves at the same height and approximate age by focusing on young and tender leaves with similar light green coloration, however, other studies concluded that A. cephalotes do not cut smaller fragments from denser leaves regardless of body size (Wetterer 1990, 1994). Leaves with higher fungal contaminant loads are found lower in the canopy, and Atta prefer low fungal load leaves

(Griffiths and Hughes 2010). I collected all treatment leaves from about 5 m from the ground in the canopy, which is the height at which leaf-cutter foragers began foraging in a study with A. colombica (Griffiths and Hughes 2010). Sampling leaves higher in the canopy assumes low fungal loads on the treatment leaves used in this study which controls for possible effects of fungal contaminants on hitchhiker or forager behavior.

For each trial, I collected a branch from a focal tree to pull off leaves as they were needed and avoid desiccation before forager cutting. I pinned three of the collected leaves together on 11 each side of the trail 1.5 m from the nest entrance. By placing leaves on the trail edge, I avoided introducing any obstacles directly onto the foraging trail that could alter foraging behavior

(Dussutour 2009b).

I fixed wind treatments perpendicular to the foraging trail with a desk fan situated 1.5 m away from the nest entrance. Using a Kestrel 1000 anemometer to calibrate wind speed, I recorded data at wind speeds of 0, 1.3, and 2.0 m/s. A 30 min acclimation period with the fan running allowed the ants to adjust to the treatment and helped avoid effects of plastic load size selection in the initial recruitment phase to the resource (Roces and Nuñez 1993; Roces and

Hölldobler 1994; Bollazzi and Roces 2011). A scarf prevented disturbance of the ants with my breath.

Using forceps, I collected leaf fragments the ants harvested from the leaf piles. I only collected leaf fragments from foragers I saw finish their last cut to ensure that the leaf loads were from the leaf pile I provided and not from another foraging source or cut by another individual

(task partitioning). Additionally, I did not collect fragments from ants climbing down tree trunks to eliminate any potential for task partitioning. The shape of the fragment cut can determine cutting time (e.g. a circular interior cut or a semicircular edge cut) (Wetterer 1990), so I collected both but not cuts that included leaf mid-veins, which may be heavier and more difficult to cut.

When treatment leaves were 75% depleted, I replaced them with previously uncut leaves of similar size pulled from the sample branch. It is likely that my removal of leaf loads from foragers or foragers themselves had little to no effect on recruitment in this study. Burd (2000) removed leaf-cutter ants and their loads for the first 2 h of trials and found no effect on normal traffic conditions; the researchers concluded the absence of laden ants had no effect on recruitment. 12

After collecting freshly-cut leaf fragments from foragers, I placed the leaf fragments into a plastic bag for later measurement. I collected 2012 samples in 30 min increments to avoid the loss of mass from desiccation, which can be considerable. In 2014, I placed each sample in a separate 1.5 mL Eppendorf tube along with the ant carrying it and recorded the time for each sample. Image J software (v.1.45.s) calculated the surface area for each leaf fragment to the nearest 10-4 cm2 and ant morphometrics to the nearest 10-3 cm (head width, pronotal width, and metathoracic leg length (Rasband 1997-2011). I used head width as the best variable to represent ant size as it best correlated with fragment size, and measured as the widest length when viewed dorsally (Wilson 1980a, b). Metathoracic leg length was poorly correlated with head width, as many workers were missing entire segments of tarsi on the rear legs. Each time I collected a leaf fragment, I tallied the number of hitchhikers holding on during that 30 min interval, then divided that count by the number of leaves collected during the interval to get the average number of hitchhikers per leaf fragment in each interval (for 2012), or I recorded the number of hitchhikers riding on each leaf fragment (for 2014). If an ant dropped a leaf fragment after cutting, I did not record that sample. To record traffic density, I took pictures of the foraging trail and a meter tape with a digital camera and later recorded the number of ants per cm2. After every third interval (in 2012), I weighed the leaf fragments to the nearest 10-4 g for each time interval and photographed them. The weight allowed me to obtain the average mass of each fragment per time period. In 2014, I photographed each leaf fragment and the dry weight of the ant that carried it to the nearest 10-4 g. I recalibrated the scale after every five measurements. I did not weigh leaf fragments because of variability caused by desiccation at different time lengths. 13

I analyzed the data using R statistical software program (R ver. 2.15.1). To examine whether load size changes in response to forager size, wind speed, or type of leaf harvested (leaf toughness/density), I used a linear model comparison approach modeling leaf fragment size on forager head width, wind treatment, leaf type, and their interactions. To examine if larger ants recruit to windy sites or tougher leaves, I used a linear model comparison approach modeling head width by wind treatment, leaf type, and their interactions. To check for normality in the hitchhiker data, I performed a Shapiro-Wilk normality test (W = 0.87, P = 9.9e-05). Taking into account the non-normality of the data, I conducted a non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test on the average number of hitchhikers per time interval modeled by wind treatment to examine effects of wind on hitchhiker prevalence. To test whether load selection behavior changes in relation to traffic congestion on the foraging trail, I conducted a Pearson’s Product-

Moment Correlation examining the degree of linear association between the log of traffic density on the foraging trail and leaf fragment mean weight per time interval.

Results

In examining whether foragers cut different sized leaf fragment loads in windy conditions

I found evidence that leaf-cutter ants cut larger leaf fragments as wind speed increased (F1,513 =

6.67, P = 0.01) (Fig 1). This was contrary to my original predictions that foragers would minimize resistance and cut smaller loads. Leaf fragments were significantly larger in the 2.0 m/s wind treatment than with no wind (Tukey HSD test, Padj = 0.03). In the treatment with no wind, I collected 236 leaf fragments with a surface area of 1.40 ± 0.03 cm2 (mean ± SE) from ants foraging on the leaf pile. In the 1.3 m/s wind treatment, I collected 189 leaf fragments with 14 a surface area of 1.47 ± 0.036 cm2 (mean ± SE). In the 2.0 m/s wind treatment, I collected 90 leaf fragments with a surface area of 1.56 ± 0.073 cm2 (mean ± SE).

Fig 1 Bar plot showing mean leaf fragment area across wind treatments of 0, 1.3, and 2.0 m/s (n=236, n=189, n=90 leaf fragments, respectively) for 2012 data. Different lower case letters represent significantly different pairwise comparisons

I did not find significant evidence for a simple effect that larger workers cut larger leaves

(F1.132 = 1.73, P = 0.20) (Fig 2), and I found no evidence for the simple effect that larger workers recruit to windy sites (F1, 137 = -0.03, P = 0.85) (Fig 3). There was a significant interaction between head width and wind treatment predicting fragment size (F1, 132 = 6.45, P = 0.012) (Fig

4). There is a stronger relationship between forager size and leaf fragment size in windy conditions compared to no wind conditions (control). Larger ants cut larger leaves, but more so when wind is present. 15

Y = 0.76 + 2.7X

Fig 2 Regression plot with lne of best fit showing the relationship between forager head width and fragment area (n = 140 ants and fragments)

Fig 3 Box plot showing that wind treatment does not affect the size of the foragers cutting at that site (n = 70 no wind, n = 70 with wind). Boxes contain the interquartile data, whiskers indicate maximum and minimum values, and dots represent extreme values

16

Fig 4 Regression plot with lines of best fit showing the interaction between forager head width and leaf fragment size cut in wind and no wind treatments. Black dots and line indicate wind treatments and gray dots and line indicate no wind treatments

Fewer hitchhikers rode on the leaf fragments as wind speed increased (X2 = 14.9, df = 2,

P = 0.0006) (Fig 5). A Wilcoxon rank-sum pairwise comparison test with Holm adjusted p- values revealed that there were fewer hitchhikers present in the 1.3 m/s wind treatment compared with no wind treatments (Padj = 0.03), and there were fewer hitchhikers present in the 2.0 m/s wind treatment compared with no wind treatments (Padj = 0.0009). In each treatment, I counted the number of hitchhikers on collected leaf fragments for each 30 min interval. With no wind,

1.70 ± 0.25 (mean ± SE) ants hitchhiked per leaf fragment over 17 intervals. In the 1.3 m/s treatment 0.84 ± 0.22 (mean ± SE) hitchhikers were found per leaf fragment over 15 intervals, and in the 2.0 m/s treatment, 0.46 ± 0.23 (mean ± SE) ants hitchhiked per leaf fragment over 15 intervals. 17

Fig 5 Box plot indicating median number of hitchhikers per fragment across time intervals and wind treatments of 0, 1.3, and 2.0 m/s (n=17, n=15, n=15 time intervals, respectively) for 2012 data. Boxes contain the interquartile data, whiskers indicate maximum and minimum values, and dots represent extreme values. Different lower case letters represent significantly different pairwise comparisons

When I tested for a behavioral response to differences in traffic congestion in this study, there was no evidence for a correlation between traffic density on the foraging trail and leaf fragment weight (F1,15 = -1.68, P = 0.21) (Fig 6). Similarly, examining for evidence of change in leaf fragment size due to congestion on the foraging trail, there was no correlation between traffic density and leaf fragment size (F1,15 = -0.04, P = 0.84). 18

Fig 6 Scatter plot of log traffic density on the foraging trail with mean weight per leaf fragment per time interval (test of the Truck Driver Effect) (n=18)

Differences in leaf density/toughness between the two forage tree species in this study did not affect the size of the leaf fragment cut (F1,132 = -0.06, P = 0.80) (Fig 7) but did effect the size of the workers that recruited to the cutting surface (F1,136 = -6.18, P = 0.01) (Fig 8). There was no difference between the size of leaf fragments cut when 2012 and 2014 sampling seasons were compared (F1, 653 = 0.06, P = 0.80). 19

Fig 7 Box plot showing that leaf density/toughness did not affect the size of the fragment cut (n = 73 for cas, n = 65 for coralillo). Cas tree leaves are more dense and tough than coralillo tree leaves. Boxes contain the interquartile data, whiskers indicate maximum and minimum interquartile values, and dots represent extreme values

Fig 8 Box plot showing that leaf density/toughness did affect the size of the ant recruited to cut (n = 73 for cas, n = 65 for coralillo). Boxes contain the interquartile data, whiskers indicate maximum and minimum interquartile values, and dots represent extreme values

20

Discussion

I explored how local conditions on the foraging trail affect behavior in A. cephalotes leaf- cutter workers. Leaf-cutter ants alter behavior as local conditions vary. A. cephalotes leaf-cutter ant foragers and hitchhikers exhibit behavioral plasticity in response to wind conditions on the foraging trail. Contrary to my original prediction that foragers would cut smaller fragments in wind, I found that foragers cut larger area leaf loads in wind than without wind, controlling for ant size. The second finding of this study is that few minima hitchhike on leaf fragment loads carried by foragers in windy conditions. Lastly, I expected A. cephalotes foragers to cut smaller leaves as traffic density increased throughout the foraging cycle. In contrast with previous studies, I did not detect that leaf fragment mass or size is affected as traffic density on the foraging trail increases.

Much evidence exists for instances in which leaf-cutter ants cut smaller loads than maximally possible, yet I found an instance where leaf-cutter foragers cut loads larger than normal. A possible explanation for larger leaf fragment size in windy conditions is that foragers may offset the added mass of hitchhikers when there is less wind by cutting smaller loads.

Phorid flies locate their hosts using receptors sensitive to ant volatile pheromones (Feener et al.

1996), and wind likely prevents parasitic phorid flies from locating hosts by displacing volatile pheromone signals in the air. In the absence of parasitoid enemies foragers are free to cut larger loads because vulnerability to parasitism is not a factor, hitchhikers are not needed for defense, and foragers anticipate the non-recruitment and added mass of hitchhikers and subsequently cut a larger load. Foragers would need to change the load size selection prior to potential hitchhiker recruitment. Or, foragers could directly cue in to the absence of parasitoid phorid flies in windy conditions. The presence or absence of parasitoids near the foraging trail can have a substantial 21 impact on daily foraging activity. One ovipositing female phorid fly can alter the behavior of hundreds of ant workers if they are aware of her presence (Feener and Brown 1992; Orr et al.

1995; Porter et al. 1995).

The link between wind and rain may offer another explanation for larger load sizes cut by foragers in wind. Leaf-cutter ants drop their loads at the onset of rain abandoning them on the trail to quickly run back to the nest for cover (Rudolph and Loudon 1986, personal observations).

Wind may offer a warning to foragers that their trip will soon be cancelled because of bad weather. It is possible that in order to maximize their final effort until the rain stops, ants may cut one last large load when they sense wind, which is usually a precursor to rain (Lewis et al.

1974). Foragers may attempt to maximize their resource delivery for the last load of a foraging cycle by cutting the largest load they can to offset the cost of idly waiting in the nest for adverse conditions to cease. Future studies should examine individual foragers throughout a foraging cycle and track load size selection to see if foragers cut the larger fragments for their last trip of a foraging cycle.

Evidence in this study showed a stronger relationship between forager size and leaf fragment size in windy conditions compared to no wind conditions (control), and larger ants cut larger leaves, but more so in wind. It may be that size matching of a carrier to its load is less variable in windy conditions than when no wind is present to maintain stability.

The decrease in hitchhikers riding on loads carried by foragers in windy conditions was obvious by casual observation. Windy conditions may dampen or completely obscure signals needed to recruit hitchhikers to leaf fragment loads. Hitchhiking minima of A. cephalotes are likely attracted to leaf laden workers through stridulation that transfers through the ant’s head into the substrate (Roces and Hölldobler 1995). Foragers tend to mill around on the cutting 22 surface, antennating sisters engaged in cutting and searching for a suitable place to begin cutting their own fragment (personal observation). Minima workers milling around on a leaf are possibly searching for a fragment to board. The fewer number of hitchhikers found on leaf loads in wind treatments may result from the “noise” caused by the leaf fragment shaking in the wind, obscuring the vibratory hitchhiker recruitment signal. Therefore, hitchhikers may not be receiving the stimulus needed to board a freshly cut leaf fragment. It is intriguing that some wind would render the primary function of hitchhikers useless resulting in a waste of effort and energy travelling to and from a resource.

I conducted this study over two sampling efforts, and the sampling methods differed between the two differed in some important aspects. There was no difference in leaf fragment areas comparing 2012 and 2014 sampling seasons, but extreme values in 2012 for the larger fragments cut in wind treatments may be an artifact of unmeasured variables. In 2014, I calibrated wind speed using the same fan from the 2012 study based upon previous settings including distance and angle from the trail, compared to anemometer calibration of wind speed in 2012. In 2012, I displaced carriers from their loads without removing them from the foraging trail, while in 2014 I collected carriers and their loads without returning them to the foraging trail. Either sampling method could have an effect on nestmate interactions including recruiting, but the method used in 2014 allowed me to obtain needed data on forager size and load matching.

Maximum biomass transport rate may result more from the shape of the forage than the mass or density in leaf-cutter systems. Forage load size and shape affects the collector’s cutting effort, the transporter’s walking effort and ability to fit inside the nest entrance and chambers, and the minima gardener’s processing effort required to mince, regurgitate and inoculate the 23 fungal garden. Rudolph and Loudon (1986) and other researchers found that ant mass is more correlated with leaf fragment mass than area, but it is likely that fragments are selected for area as well as mass because of constraints caused by maneuverability. Moll et al. (2010) showed that the shape of a fragment, not its mass, has more of an effect on running speed in Atta vollenweiden grass cutter ants. Longer grass fragments were more metabolically costly to transport (as a measure of CO2 production) and slowed the carriers, and heavier loads did not slow workers but still held a higher metabolic cost. Atta cephalotes carry round or semicircular leaf fragments, not long grass blades, but Moll et al. (2010) point to the importance of size and shape in load size selection in the Attini.

Farji-Brener et al. (2011) found that highly laden ants walk slowly and decrease the walking speed of nest-mates behind them, similar to a large truck moving slowly on a highway

(the Truck Driver Effect). At high traffic densities, the proportion of A. cephalotes ants carrying heavy loads decreases (Farji-Brener et al. 2011). Ants that would normally carry a heavy load choose a lighter load to avoid slowing down nest-mates. Farji-Brener et al. (2011) showed that traffic density mechanisms result in A. cephalotes workers adjusting foraging behavior to regulate tissue transfer to the nest. I found no evidence for the Truck Driver Effect in this study.

Central-place foragers balance costs and benefits in the context of their local environment on the foraging trail and plant surfaces to maximize resource allocation to the colony. If we consider the nest interior as an added component of local environment, load size selection becomes more complicated. Burd and Howard (2005b) found a strong main effect for garden chamber size in their model with smaller chambers producing a negative feedback cue on foraging. They felt that smaller chambers are more likely to exist naturally because smaller chambers have a higher surface area to volume ratio and are better ventilated for CO2 removal 24 than larger chambers. They postulate that smaller leaf fragments might be transported to more distal garden chambers, and that larger fragments are deposited closer to the nest entrance, so it may be that substrate need in different chambers may have an effect on load size, as well.

Perhaps larger proximal chambers are left empty for events when foragers cut larger loads than normal, such as wind. Future work could empirically examine for evidence of differential filling in different sized garden chambers under varied experimental conditions and probe for feedback effects of chamber capacities on forager load size selection.

Conclusion

It is clear that leaf-cutter ants regulate their foraging effort based upon foraging context, and this study adds to our knowledge of resource allocation in central-place foraging systems. It is unclear why foragers cut larger than average leaf loads in wind, and future studies should examine flight potential of phorid flies and hitchhiker behavior in the presence and absence of parasitoids. More work needs to be done on the ability of phorid flies to effectively fly, locate, and oviposit eggs in windy conditions. Information of this type could be used to enhance biocontrol release efforts of phorid flies in the United States to combat the invasive Solenopsis fire ant infestation by timing parasitoid release during non-windy times of the year (Calcott et al

2011). In the Neotropics, leaf-cutter ant pests remove more vegetation than any other group (Cherrett 1986; Hölldobler and Wilson 1990). Research into effects of wind on phorid success and leaf-cutter vigilance may aid in biocontrol of leaf-cutter ant agricultural pests throughout the Neotropics.

Insights into traffic patterns, load delivery, and wind resistance in ants may help engineers and city planners determine ways to structure dense communities maximizing 25 efficiency (Burd 2002). For example, when leaf-cutter foragers on the trail are at a 50:50 laden to unladen ratio, traffic flows more smoothly (Burd et al. 2002). We use dedicated lanes in our highway system, but we should look to evolutionary solutions already present in the world to help inform our decisions that shape our future.

26

References

Angilletta MJ, Roth TC, Wilson RS, et al. (2008) The fast and the fractalous: speed and tortuosity trade off in running ants. Func Ecol 22:78-83.

Bailey KH, Polis GA (1987) Optimal and central-place foraging theory applied to a desert harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex californicus. Oecologia 72:440-448.

Benckiser G (2011) Ants and sustainable agriculture. Sust Ag 2:15-26.

Bollazzi M, Roces F (2011) Information needs at the beginning of foraging: grass-cutting ants trade off load size for a faster return to the nest. Plos Biol 6:1-9.

Braganca MAL, Tohasca A, Della Lucia TMC (1998) Reduction in the foraging activity of the leaf-cutting ant Atta sextens caused by the phorid Neodohhrniphora sp. Entomol Exp Appl 89:305-311.

Brännström A, Sumpter D (2005) The role of competition and clustering in population dynamics. Proc. R. Soc. B 272:2065-2072.

Burd M (1996a) Server system and queuing models of leaf harvesting by leaf-cutting ants. Am Nat 148:613–629.

Burd M (1996b) Foraging performance by , a leaf-cutting ant. Am Nat 148:597- 612.

Burd M (2000) Foraging behaviour of Atta cephalotes (leaf-cutting ants): an examination of two predictions for load selection. Anim Behav 60: 781-788.

Burd M (2001) Leaf tissue transport as a function of loading ratio in the leaf-cutting ant Atta cephalotes. Ecol Entomol 26:551-556.

Burd M, Archer D, Aranwela N, et al (2002) Traffic dynamics of the leaf-cutting ant, Atta cephalotes. Amer Nat 159:283-293.

Burd M, Howard JJ (2005a) Central-place foraging continues beyond the nest entrance: the underground performance of leaf-cutting ants. Anim Behav 70:737-744.

Burd M, Howard JJ (2005b) Global optimization from suboptimal parts. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:234-242.

Burd M, Howard JJ (2008) Optimality in a partitioned task performed by social insects. Biol Letters 4:627-629.

Burkhardt JF (1998) Individual flexibility and tempo in the ant, Pheidole dentata, the influence of group size. J Insect Behav 11:493-505. 27

Calcott A, Porter SD, Weeks RDJ, Graham F, Johnson SJ, Gilbert LE (2011) Fire ant decapitating fly cooperative release programs (1994-2008): two Pseudoacteon species, P. tricuspis and P. curvatus, rapidly expand across imported fire ant populations in the Southeastern United States. J Insect Sci 11:1-25.

Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor Pop Biol 9:129-136.

Cherrett JM (1972) Some factors involved in the selection of vegetable substrate by Atta cephalotes (L.) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) in tropical rain forest. J Anim Ecol 41:647- 660.

Cherrett JM (1986) The economic importance and control of leaf-cutting ants. In: Vinson B (ed) Economic impact and control of social insects, Praeger Publishers, New York, pp 165- 192.

Constant N, Santorelli LA, Lopes JFS, Hughes WHO (2012) The effects of genotype, caste, and age on foraging performance in leaf-cutting ants. Behav Ecol 23:1284-1288.

Dussutour A, Deneuborg JL, Beshers S, Fourcassie V (2009a) Individual and collective problem solving in a foraging context in the leaf-cutting ant Atta colombica. Anim Cogn 12:21-30.

Dussutour A, Beshers S, Deneubourg JL, Fourcassie V (2009b) Priority rules govern the organization of traffic on foraging trails under crowding conditions in the leaf-cutting ant Atta colombica. J Exp Biol 212:499-505.

Elizalde L, Folgarait PJ (2012) Behavioral strategies of phorid parasitoids and responses of their hosts, the leaf-cutting ants. J Insect Sci 12:135.

Farji-Brener AG, Chinchilla FA, Rifkin S, Sanchez Cuervo AM, Triana E, Quiroga V, Giraldo P (2011) The “truck driver” effect in leaf-cutting ants: how do individual load influences the walking speed of nest-mates. Physiol Entomol 36:128-134.

Feener DH, Brown BV (1992) Reduced foraging of Solenopsis geminta (Hymenoptera, Formicidae in the presence of parasitic Pseudoacteon spp (Diptera: Phoridae). Ann Ento Soc Amer 85:80-84.

Feener DH, Jacobs LF, Schmidt JO (1996) Specialized parasitoid attracted to pheromone of ants. Anim Behav 51:61-66.

Feener DH, Moss KAG (1990) Defense against parasites by hitchhikers in leaf-cutting ants - a quantitative assessment. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:17-29.

Fowler HG, Saes NB (1986) Dependence of the activity of grazing cattle on foraging grass- cutting ants (Atta sp) in the Southern Neotropics. J App Ent 101:154-158.

28

Gordon DM (2002) The regulation of foraging activity in red harvester ant colonies. Am Nat 159:509-518.

Griffiths HM, Hughes WOH (2010) Hitchhiking and the removal of microbial contaminants by the leaf-cutting ant Atta colombica. Ecol Entomol 35:529-537.

Hölldobler B (1976) Recruitment behavior, home range orientation and territoriality in harvester ants, Pogonomyrmex. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1:3-44.

Hölldobler B, Wilson EO (1990) The ants. Belknap Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts

Hubbell SP, Johnson LK, Stanislav E, Wilson B, Fowler H (1980) Foraging by bucket-brigade in leaf-cutter ants. Biotropica 12:210-213.

Hughes WOH, Goulson D (2001) Polyethism and the importance of context in the alarm reaction of the grass-cutting ant, Atta capiguara. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:503-508.

Jeanne RL (1999) Group size, productivity, and information flow in social wasps. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (ed) Information Processing in Social Insects, Birkhauser, Switzerland, pp 3-30.

Kitayama K, Sousa-Souto L, Uchoa de Aquino P (2012) Trichome removal by hitchhikers in two leaf-cutting ant species (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Sociobiol 59:935-943.

Lewis T, Pollard GV, Dibley GC (1974) Rhythmic foraging in the leaf-cutting ant Atta cephalotes (L.) (Formicidae: Attini). J Anim Ecol 43:129-142.

Lutz FE (1929) Observations on leaf-cutting ants. Am Museum Novitates 388:1-21.

Moll K, Roces F, Federle W (2010) Foraging grass-cutting ants (Atta vollenweideri) maintain stability by balancing their loads with controlled head movements. J Comp Physiol A 196:471-480.

Nuñez J (1982) Honeybee foraging strategies at a food source in relation to its distance from the hive and the rate of sugar flow. J Apicult Res 21:37-48.

Orr MR (1992) Parasitic flies (Diptera: Phoridae) influence foraging rhythms and caste division of labor in the leaf-cutter ant, Atta cephalotes (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav Ecol Sciobiol 30:395-402.

Orr MR, Seike SH, Benson WW, Gilbert LE (1995) Flies suppress fire ants. Nature 373:292- 293.

Porter SD, Vandermeer RK, Pesquero MA, Campiolo S, Fowler HG (1995) Solenopsis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) fire ant reactions to attacks of Pseudoacteon flies (Diptera, Phoridae) in Southeastern Brazil. Ann Entol Soc Amer 88:570-575. 29

Quinlan RJ, Cherrett JM (1979) The role of fungus in the diet of the leaf-cutting ant, Atta cephalotes (L.) Ecol Entomol 4:151-160.

Rasband, WS (1997-2011) ImageJ. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MY, USA. URL http://imagej.nih.gov/ij.

R Development Core Team (2011) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org/.

Rissing SW, Pollock GB (1984) Worker size variability and foraging efficiency in Veromessor Pergandei (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 15:121-126.

Roces F (1990) Leaf-cutting ants cut fragment sizes in relation to the distance from the nest. Anim Behav 40:1181-1183.

Roces F, Hölldobler B (1994) Leaf density and the trade-off between load size selection and recruitment behavior in the ant Atta cephalotes. Oecologia 97:1-8.

Roces F, Höllbobler B (1995) Vibrational communication between hitchhikers and foragers in leaf-cutting ants (Atta cephalotes). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 37:297-302.

Roces F, Nuñez J (1993) Information about food quality influences load-size selection in recruited leaf-cutting ants. Anim Behav 45:135-143.

Rudolph SG, Loudon K (1986) Load size selection by foraging leaf-cutter ants (Atta cephalotes). Ecol Entomol 11:401-410.

Schmid-Hempel P, Kacelnik A, Houston AI (1985) Honeybees maximize efficiency by not filling their crop. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17:61-66.

Taylor F (1977) Foraging behavior in ants: experiments with two species of myrmecine ants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:147-168.

Van Breda JM, Stradling DJ (1994) Mechanisms affecting load size determination in Atta cephalotes. Insect Soc 41:423-434.

Vieira-Neto EHM, Mundim FM, Vasconcelos HL (2006) Hitchhiking behavior in leaf-cutter ants: An experimental evaluation of three hypotheses. Instect Soc 53:326-332.

Waller DA (1986) The foraging ecology of Atta texana in Texas. In: Lofgren CS, VanderMeer RK (ed) Fire ants and leaf-cutting ants, Westview, Boulder, Colorado, pp 146-158

Weber NA (1972) Gardening ants: the Attines. Mem Am Phil Soc 92:1-146.

30

Wetterer JK (1989) Central place foraging theory: when load size affects travel time. Theor Pop Biol 36:267-280.

Wetterer JK (1990) Diel changes in forager size, activity, and load selectivity in a tropical leaf- cutting ant, Atta cephalotes. Ecol Entomol 15:97-104.

Wetterer JK (1994) Forager polymorphism, size matching and load delivery in the leaf-cutting ant, Atta cephalotes. Ecol Entomol 19:57–64.

Wilson EO (1971) The insect societies. Cambridge, Harvard University Press

Wilson EO (1980a) Caste and division of labor in leaf-cutter ants. I. The overall pattern in A. sexdans. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:143-156.

Wilson EO (1980b) Caste and division of labor in leaf-cutter ants. II. The ergonomic organization of leaf-cutting. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:157-165.