Can an Ape Create a Sentence? Based on Comparisons Between Washoe and Children, As

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Can an Ape Create a Sentence? Based on Comparisons Between Washoe and Children, As 23 November 1979, Volume 206, Number 4421 SCIENCE quences produced and understood by their pongid subjects were governed by grammatical rules. The Gardners, for ex- ample, note that "The most significant results of Project Washoe were those Can an Ape Create a Sentence? based on comparisons between Washoe and children, as ... in the use of order H. S. Terrace, L. A. Petitto, R. J. Sanders, T. G. Bever in early sentences" (3, p. 73). If an ape can truly create a sentence there would be a reason for asserting, as Patterson (11) has, that "language is no longer the exclusive domain of man." The innovative studies of the Gardners song when asserting territory. Such ri- The purpose of this article is to evaluate (1-3) and Premack (4-6) show that a gidity is typical of the communicative be- that assertion. We do so by summarizing Downloaded from chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) can learn havior of other genera, for example, bees the main features of a large body of data substantial vocabularies of "words" of communicating about the location and that we have collected from a chim- visual languages. The Gardners taught quality of food or sticklebacks engaging panzee exposed to sign language during Washoe, an infant female chimpanzee, in courtship behavior (14). its first 4 years. A major component of signs of American Sign Language (ASL) Human language is most distinctive these data is the first corpus of the multi- (7, 8). Premack taught Sarah, a juvenile because of a second level of structure sign utterances of an ape. Superficially, http://science.sciencemag.org/ female, an artificial language of plastic that subsumes the word-the sentence many of its utterances seem like sen- tences. However, objective analyses of our data, as well as of those obtained by Summary. More than 19,000 multisign utterances of an infant chimpanzee (Nim) other studies, yielded no evidence o an were analyzed for syntactic and semantic regularities. Lexical regularities were ob- ape's ability to use a grammar............ .... Each in- served in the case of two-sign combinations: particular signs (for example, more) stance of presumed grammatical compe- tended to occur in a particular position. These regularities could not be attributed to tence could be explained adequately by memorization or to position habits, suggesting that they were structurally constrained. simple nonlinguistic processes. That conclusion, however, was invalidated by videotape analyses, which showed that most of Nim's utterances were prompted by his teacher's prior utterance, and that on October 22, 2019 Nim interrupted his teachers to a much larger extent than a child interrupts an adult's Project Nim speech. Signed utterances of other apes (as shown on films) revealed similar non- human patterns of discourse. Our subject was a male chimpanzee, Neam Chimpsky (Nim for short) (16, 17). Since the age of 2 weeks, Nim was raised chips of different colors and shapes. In a (15). A sentence characteristically ex- in a home environment by human surro- related study, Rumbaugh et al. (9) taught presses a complete semantic proposition gate parents and teachers who communi- Lana, also a juvenile chimpanzee, to use through a set of words and phrases, each cated with him and amongst themselves Yerkish, an artificial visual language. bearing particular grammatical relations in ASL (7,8). Nim was trained to sign by These and other studies (10), one of to one another (such as actor, action, ob- a method modeled after the techniques which reports the acquisition of more ject). Unlike words, most sentences can- that the Gardners (2) and Fouts (18) have than 400 signs of ASL by a female gorilla not be learned individually. Psycholo- referred to as molding and guidance. Our named Koko (11), show that the shift gists, psycholinguists, and linguists are methods of data collection paralleled from a vocal to a visual medium can in general agreement that using a human those used in studies of the development compensate effectively for an ape's in- language indicates knowledge of a gram- of language in children (19-24). During ability to articulate many sounds (12). mar. How else can one account for a their sessions with Nim, his teachers That limitation alone might account for child's ultimate ability to create an 'in- whispered into a miniature cassette re- earlier failures to teach chimpanzees to determinate number of meaningful sen- corder what he signed and whether his communicate with spoken words (13). tences from a finite number of words? Human language makes use of two Recent demonstrations that chim- H. S. Terrace is a professor of psychology at Co- levels of structure: the word and the sen- panzees and gorillas can communicate lumbia University, 418 Schermerhorn Hall, Colum- tence. The meaning of a word is arbi- with humans via arbitrary "words" pose bia University, New York 10027. L. A. Petitto is a graduate student in the Department of Human De- trary. This is in contrast to the fixed a controversial question: Is the ability to velopment at Harvard University, Cambridge, Mas- character of various forms of animal create and understand sentences unique- sachusetts 01238. R. J. Sanders is a graduate student in the Department of Psychology at Columbia Uni- communication. Many bird species, for ly human? The Gardners (1, 3), Premack versity and a visiting instructor at the State Univer- sity of New York in Utica. T. G. Bever is a professor example, sing one song when in distress, (6), Rumbaugh (9), and Patterson (11) of psychology and linguistics at Columbia Universi- one song when courting a mate, and one have each proposed that the symbol se- ty. SCIENCE, VOL. 206, 23 NOVEMBER 1979 0036-8075/79/1123-0891$02.00/0 Copyright C 1979 AAAS 899 signs were spontaneous, prompted, Table 1. Number of tokens and types of com- turn of the hands to a relaxed position molded, or approximations ofthe correct binations containing two, three, four, and five (29). Of Nim's combinations, 95 percent sign (25). or more signs. consisted of sequences of distinct signs Nim satisfied our criterion of acquiring Length of Tokens Types that occurred successively. These are re- a sign when (i) on different occasions, combination ferred to as "liiear sequences." Two three independent observers reported its Two signs 11,845 1,138 other kinds of combinations were not in- spontaneous occurrence and (ii) it oc- Three signs 4,294 1,660 cluded in the corpus: contractions of two curred spontaneously on each of five Four signs 1,587 1,159 or more signs and simultaneous combi- successive days. By spontaneously we Five or more signs 1,487 1,278 nations in which two distinct signs oc- mean that Nim signed the sign in an ap- curred at the same time. Even though propriate context and without the aid of such combinations can occur in ASL, molding, prompting, or modeling on the stances of more tickle, the conventional they were excluded from our corpus be- part of the teacher. As of 25 September English juxtaposition of these signs. Ac- cause it was impossible to specify the 1977, Nim had acquired 125 signs (26). cordingly, there is no basis for deciding temporal order of the signs they con- whether Washoe's multisign combina- tained. Figure 1 shows a typical linear tions obeyed rules of sign order (28). One combination, me hug cat, in which there Combinations of Signs could conclude that Washoe had learned is no temporal overlap between any of that both more and tickle were appropri- the signs. The Gardners' analyses of Washoe's ate ways of requesting that tickling reoc- In no instance were specific se- sign combinations prevents one from cur and that when Washoe signed both quences, contractions, or simultaneous studying their grammatical structure. signs it was because of her prior training combinations reihforced differentially. With but two minor exceptions, the to sign each sign separately. Indeed, Nim was never required to make Gardners did not report the order of We defined a combination of signs as a combination of signs as opposed to a Downloaded from signs of Washoe's multisign combina- the occurrence of two or more different single sign. However, Nim's teachers of- tions (27). For example, more tickle and signs that were not interrupted by the oc- ten signed to him in stereotyped orders tickle more were both reported as in- currence of other behavior or by the re- modeled after English usage, and they http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 22, 2019 I Fig. 1. Nim signing the linear combination, me hug cat to his teacher (Susan Quinby). (Photographed in classroom by H. S. Terrace.) 892 SCIENCE, VOL. 206 may also have unwittingly given him spe- consisting of all transitive verbs com- we observed. A conservative inter- cial praise when he signed an interesting bined with all references to himself (me pretation of these regularities, one that combination. Such unintentional reac- or Nim), is shown in Table 3 (32). The does not require the postulation of syn- tions do not, however, appear to differ number of tokens with the verb in the tactic rules, would hold that Nim used from the reactions parents exhibit when first position substantially exceeds the certain categories as relatively initial or their child produces an interesting utter- reverse order. Also, Nim combined tran- final irrespective of the context in which ance or one that conforms to correct sitive verbs as readily with Nim as with they occur. If this were true, it should be English. me (33). Nim's preference for using me possible to predict the observed frequen- Nim's linear combinations were sub- and Nim in the second position of two- cy of different constructions, such as jected to three analyses.
Recommended publications
  • A Lexicostatistic Survey of the Signed Languages in Nepal
    DigitalResources Electronic Survey Report 2012-021 ® A Lexicostatistic Survey of the Signed Languages in Nepal Hope M. Hurlbut A Lexicostatistic Survey of the Signed Languages in Nepal Hope M. Hurlbut SIL International ® 2012 SIL Electronic Survey Report 2012-021, June 2012 © 2012 Hope M. Hurlbut and SIL International ® All rights reserved 2 Contents 0. Introduction 1.0 The Deaf 1.1 The deaf of Nepal 1.2 Deaf associations 1.3 History of deaf education in Nepal 1.4 Outside influences on Nepali Sign Language 2.0 The Purpose of the Survey 3.0 Research Questions 4.0 Approach 5.0 The survey trip 5.1 Kathmandu 5.2 Surkhet 5.3 Jumla 5.4 Pokhara 5.5 Ghandruk 5.6 Dharan 5.7 Rajbiraj 6.0 Methodology 7.0 Analysis and results 7.1 Analysis of the wordlists 7.2 Interpretation criteria 7.2.1 Results of the survey 7.2.2 Village signed languages 8.0 Conclusion Appendix Sample of Nepali Sign Language Wordlist (Pages 1–6) References 3 Abstract This report concerns a 2006 lexicostatistical survey of the signed languages of Nepal. Wordlists and stories were collected in several towns of Nepal from Deaf school leavers who were considered to be representative of the Nepali Deaf. In each city or town there was a school for the Deaf either run by the government or run by one of the Deaf Associations. The wordlists were transcribed by hand using the SignWriting orthography. Two other places were visited where it was learned that there were possibly unique sign languages, in Jumla District, and also in Ghandruk (a village in Kaski District).
    [Show full text]
  • Sign Language Endangerment and Linguistic Diversity Ben Braithwaite
    RESEARCH REPORT Sign language endangerment and linguistic diversity Ben Braithwaite University of the West Indies at St. Augustine It has become increasingly clear that current threats to global linguistic diversity are not re - stricted to the loss of spoken languages. Signed languages are vulnerable to familiar patterns of language shift and the global spread of a few influential languages. But the ecologies of signed languages are also affected by genetics, social attitudes toward deafness, educational and public health policies, and a widespread modality chauvinism that views spoken languages as inherently superior or more desirable. This research report reviews what is known about sign language vi - tality and endangerment globally, and considers the responses from communities, governments, and linguists. It is striking how little attention has been paid to sign language vitality, endangerment, and re - vitalization, even as research on signed languages has occupied an increasingly prominent posi - tion in linguistic theory. It is time for linguists from a broader range of backgrounds to consider the causes, consequences, and appropriate responses to current threats to sign language diversity. In doing so, we must articulate more clearly the value of this diversity to the field of linguistics and the responsibilities the field has toward preserving it.* Keywords : language endangerment, language vitality, language documentation, signed languages 1. Introduction. Concerns about sign language endangerment are not new. Almost immediately after the invention of film, the US National Association of the Deaf began producing films to capture American Sign Language (ASL), motivated by a fear within the deaf community that their language was endangered (Schuchman 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • American Sign Language
    4-H 365.00 General OHIO STATE UNIVERSITY EXTENSION PROJECT IDEA STARTER American Sign Language by Marla Berkowitz, MA, CDI, ASLTA Certified, ASL Program, The Ohio State University; and Kara Detty, Clover Bees 4-H Club Member and Supporter of ASL, Ross County. Special thanks to Abby White, Deaf and Hard of Hearing Educator, Ohio School for the Deaf American Sign Language (ASL) is the official language used mostly by deaf and hard of hearing people who are immersed in the deaf community. The deaf community includes deaf and hard of hearing people, ASL interpreters and hearing people who use ASL and are familiar with deaf culture. Different sign languages such as French, Japanese, British and many more are used all over the world. ASL and its users have influenced our world. For bilingual, using ASL and English for all instruction, and instance, William “Dummy” Hoy (born in 1862) was is located in Washington, D.C. the first deaf baseball superstar and a graduate of As ASL became recognized as a language, it cleared the Ohio School for the Deaf. Hand signals became the path for various laws leading to the Americans necessary for Hoy to understand the plays during the with Disabilities Act in 1990. Most deaf and hard of games. Other players and the fans found them useful hearing people now have better opportunities in a and these signals became commonplace. The football wide array of jobs and careers. huddle was invented in 1892 by Paul Hubbard, a Today, awareness of ASL is growing rapidly and deaf student at Gallaudet University, who urged his classes are now offered in high schools, colleges and teammates to “huddle up” to prevent other teams in local libraries, agencies and other organizations.
    [Show full text]
  • What Sign Language Creation Teaches Us About Language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3
    Focus Article What sign language creation teaches us about language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3 How do languages emerge? What are the necessary ingredients and circumstances that permit new languages to form? Various researchers within the disciplines of primatology, anthropology, psychology, and linguistics have offered different answers to this question depending on their perspective. Language acquisition, language evolution, primate communication, and the study of spoken varieties of pidgin and creoles address these issues, but in this article we describe a relatively new and important area that contributes to our understanding of language creation and emergence. Three types of communication systems that use the hands and body to communicate will be the focus of this article: gesture, homesign systems, and sign languages. The focus of this article is to explain why mapping the path from gesture to homesign to sign language has become an important research topic for understanding language emergence, not only for the field of sign languages, but also for language in general. © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. How to cite this article: WIREs Cogn Sci 2012. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1212 INTRODUCTION linguistic community, a language model, and a 21st century mind/brain that well-equip the child for this esearchers in a variety of disciplines offer task. When the very first languages were created different, mostly partial, answers to the question, R the social and physiological conditions were very ‘What are the stages of language creation?’ Language different. Spoken language pidgin varieties can also creation can refer to any number of phylogenic and shed some light on the question of language creation.
    [Show full text]
  • Chimpanzees Use of Sign Language
    Chimpanzees’ Use of Sign Language* ROGER S. FOUTS & DEBORAH H. FOUTS Washoe was cross-fostered by humans.1 She was raised as if she were a deaf human child and so acquired the signs of American Sign Language. Her surrogate human family had been the only people she had really known. She had met other humans who occasionally visited and often seen unfamiliar people over the garden fence or going by in cars on the busy residential street that ran next to her home. She never had a pet but she had seen dogs at a distance and did not appear to like them. While on car journeys she would hang out of the window and bang on the car door if she saw one. Dogs were obviously not part of 'our group'; they were different and therefore not to be trusted. Cats fared no better. The occasional cat that might dare to use her back garden as a shortcut was summarily chased out. Bugs were not favourites either. They were to be avoided or, if that was impossible, quickly flicked away. Washoe had accepted the notion of human superiority very readily - almost too readily. Being superior has a very heady quality about it. When Washoe was five she left most of her human companions behind and moved to a primate institute in Oklahoma. The facility housed about twenty-five chimpanzees, and this was where Washoe was to meet her first chimpanzee: imagine never meeting a member of your own species until you were five. After a plane flight Washoe arrived in a sedated state at her new home.
    [Show full text]
  • DQP TISLR 10 Fingerspelling Rates
    Rates of fingerspelling in American Sign Language David Quinto-Pozos Department of Linguistics, University of Texas-Austin TISLR 10; Purdue University Methodology Introduction Main points Signers: 2 deaf native users of ASL (Kevin & James) Information in the text (examples of items that were fingerspelled): • Faster rates than previously reported Fingerspelling used often in American Sign Language (ASL) • Where Don lived (various states and cities such as Idaho, Indiana, and Means: 5-8 letters per second (125 – 200 ms/ltr) • Morford & MacFarlane (2003); corpus of 4,111 signs (27 signers) Task: deliver an ASL narrative (originally created in English) about Dallas) and worked (e.g. Gallaudet University, Model Secondary School • 8.7% of signs in casual signing the life of a Deaf leader in the US Deaf community (Don Petingill) for the Deaf, etc.) • 4.8% of signs in formal signing • Donʼs involvement in the Deaf community including advocacy work • Signers can differ in rates: Some signers are faster • 5.8% of signs in narrative signing Three audiences per signer: school children (ages 9-10) (e.g. for the Texas Commission for the Deaf) fingerspellers than other signers plus two audiences of adults • Anecdotes about Donʼs life (e.g., Donʼs joke-telling & humor) • Padden & Gansauls (2003) • 10% - 15% of signs in discourse • “Long” words are fingerspelled faster than “short” • > 50% of native signers fingerspelled 20% of time words • non-native signers: lower frequency of fingerspelling General Description of the Data: Reasons for “short” vs. “long”
    [Show full text]
  • 400 Years of Change In
    Digiti lingua: a celebration of British Sign Language and Deaf Culture Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre UCL 1 Structure of this talk • Introduction to BSL: its history and social context • Historical sources • What kind of language is BSL? • Change in BSL • BSL in the future Introduction to BSL: its history and social context Some myths about sign language • There is one universal sign language • Sign language consists of iconic gestures • Sign languages were invented by hearing people to help deaf people • Sign languages have no grammar • BSL is just English on the hands Truths about sign language • There are many different sign languages in the world • Sign languages are just as conventionalised as spoken languages • Sign languages are natural languages, the creation of deaf communities • Sign languages have their own complex grammars BSL – language of the British Deaf community • An estimated 50,000-70,000 sign language people • Forms a single language group with Australian and New Zealand sign languages • Unrelated to American Sign Language or Irish Sign Language Social context of BSL • A minority language used by a community with historically low status • Non-traditional transmission patterns • Extensive regional lexical variation • A bilingual community, but with variable access to the language of the majority • Has experienced active attempts at suppression over many centuries Home sign • Gestural communication systems developed during communication between deaf children and hearing adults • Unlike sign
    [Show full text]
  • International Sign and American Sign Language As Different Types of Global Deaf Lingua Francas
    International Sign and American Sign Language as Different Types of Global Deaf Lingua Francas Annelies Kusters Sign Language Studies, Volume 21, Number 4, Summer 2021, pp. 391-426 (Article) Published by Gallaudet University Press DOI: https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2021.0005 For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/799806 [ Access provided at 2 Oct 2021 13:47 GMT with no institutional affiliation ] ANNELIES KUSTERS International Sign and American Sign Language as Different Types of Global Deaf Lingua Francas Abstract International Sign (IS) and American Sign Language (ASL) have both been used as lingua francas within international deaf contexts. Perspectives on the uses of IS and ASL as lingua francas in such contexts are connected to discourses pertaining to the form, func- tion, status, value, languageness, and global reach of IS and ASL. While there are some historical and usage-based parallels between IS and ASL, they are different types of lingua francas, and their uses as lingua franca are evaluated differently in different contexts. International Sign (IS) and American Sign Language (ASL) are both used as global deaf lingua francas within international deaf encounters and events, associated with different patterns of emer- gence, spread, and circulation. ASL is a national sign language that emerged in the United States in the nineteenth century (Supalla and Clark 2014). ASL (or its lexicon) has been introduced in various forms by missionaries, educationalists, and developmental workers in a significant number of countries in Africa, Asia, and South America (Parsons 2005; Woodward 1996). Deaf people from all over the world have attended universities in the Annelies Kusters is associate professor in Sign Language and Intercultural Research at Heriot-Watt University.
    [Show full text]
  • Fingerspelling in American Sign Language
    FINGERSPELLING IN AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE: A CASE STUDY OF STYLES AND REDUCTION by Deborah Stocks Wager A thesis submitted to the faculty of The University of Utah in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Linguistics The University of Utah August 2012 Copyright © Deborah Stocks Wager 2012 All Rights Reserved The University of Utah Graduate School STATEMENT OF THESIS APPROVAL The thesis of Deborah Stocks Wager has been approved by the following supervisory committee members: Marianna Di Paolo , Chair 5/10/12 Date Approved Aaron Kaplan , Member 5/10/12 Date Approved Sherman Wilcox , Member 5/10/12 Date Approved and by Edward Rubin , Chair of the Department of Linguistics and by Charles A. Wight, Dean of The Graduate School. ABSTRACT Fingerspelling in American Sign Language (ASL) is a system in which 26 one- handed signs represent the letters of the English alphabet and are formed sequentially to spell out words borrowed from oral languages or letter sequences. Patrie and Johnson have proposed a distinction in fingerspelling styles between careful fingerspelling and rapid fingerspelling, which appear to correspond to clear speech and plain speech styles. The criteria for careful fingerspelling include indexing of fingerspelled words, completely spelled words, limited coarticulation, a slow signing rate, and even rhythm, while rapid fingerspelling involves lack of indexing, increased dropping of letters, coarticulation, a faster signing rate, and the first and last letter of the words being held longer. They further propose that careful fingerspelling is used for initial uses of all fingerspelled words in running signing, with rapid fingerspelling being used for second and further mentions of fingerspelled words.
    [Show full text]
  • Auslan Dictionary Topic : Colours
    Colours Black Blue How to Sign it : Not commonly used in : ACT. Stroke fingers of fist forward NSW. SA. along side of face. Blue Brown Not commonly used in : QLD. How to Sign it : NT. TAS. VIC. WA. Hold hand with fingers and thumb spread, palm toward chin. Move the fingers together as the hand moves out and down from face Not commonly used in : ACT. NSW. SA. Brown Colour How to Sign it : Form a circle with pointer finger and thumb. Slide fingertips along your other extended pointer finger, twice. Not commonly used in : QLD. NT. TAS. VIC. WA. Auslan (Australian Sign Language) Dictionary Topic: Colours Page : 1 Source: www.signplanet.net Copyright: Bilby Publishing & Consulting Pty Ltd www.bilby.net Green Green Not commonly used in : ACT. Not commonly used in : QLD. NSW. SA. NT. TAS. VIC. WA. Grey Maroon Mauve Orange How to Sign it : Bunched hand opens and closes near mouth, as if squeezing Auslan (Australian Sign Language) Dictionary Topic: Colours Page : 2 Source: www.signplanet.net Copyright: Bilby Publishing & Consulting Pty Ltd www.bilby.net Pink Purple How to Sign it : How to Sign it : Brush pointer finger backwards With pointer and middle fingers on cheek, with palm facing extended together, form a forward. circle with fingertips in palm of other hand. Rainbow Red How to Sign it : Not commonly used in : With fingers slightly spread, NSW. and palm facing you, form and arc with hand from one side to the other. Red White Not commonly used in : QLD. How to Sign it : NT. TAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Brief: the Importance of Fingerspelling for Reading
    VISUAL LANGUAGE & VISUAL LEARNING NSF supported Science of Learning Center on Visual Language and Visual Learning, SBE-0541953, RESEARCH BRIEF: Gallaudet University. THE IMPORTANCE OF FINGERSPELLING FOR READING JULY 2010 Key Findings on the Importance of Fingerspelling for Reading: LEARNING FROM RESEARCH • Deaf families fingerspell to their deaf children when they are very young. • Early exposure to fingerspelling helps these children become better # 1 readers. • Fingerspelling, reading, and writing are interrelated. • Fingerspelling facilitates English vocabulary growth, and larger the lexicon, the faster new vocabulary is learned. • Fingerspelling positively correlates with stronger reading skills. Deaf Written by: and hard of hearing children who are good fingerspellers are good Sharon Baker, Ed.D. readers, and vice versa. 1 of 8 NSF SCIENCE OF LEARNING CENTER ON VISUAL LANGUAGE AND VISUAL LEARNING RESEARCH BRIEF NO. 1: THE IMPORTANCE OF FINGERSPELLING FOR READING Fingerspelling and American Sign hearing children from deaf families tend to read at Language higher levels than deaf and hard of hearing children from hearing families.11 Fingerspelling likely On the most simplistic level, fingerspelling can be contributes to this success. Unfortunately, young defined as the use of handshapes to represent deaf and hard of hearing children from hearing letters of the alphabet. Indeed, before the families are not generally given the same early complexity of fingerspelling was documented, learning opportunity. Indeed, the absence of researchers thought fingerspelling was merely a fingerspelling is particularly evident in preschools manual representation of English orthography for deaf and hard of hearing children.9 To 1 (print). They believed fingerspelling was primarily understand the role of fingerspelling in language for representing proper nouns or for English words acquisition and later literacy, it is important to 2,3 without a sign equivalent.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cross-Linguistic Guide to Signwriting ® a Phonetic Approach
    A Cross-Linguistic ® Guide to SignWriting A phonetic approach Stephen Parkhurst Dianne Parkhurst A Cross-Linguistic Guide to SignWriting ®: A phonetic approach ©2008 Stephen Parkhurst Revision 2010 For use at SIL-UND courses during the summer of 2010. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, transmitted or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recorded or otherwise, without permission from the authors. Stephen and Dianne Parkhurst E-mail: [email protected] A note from the authors SignWriting®, also known as Sutton Movement Writing for Sign Language, was invented by Valerie Sutton in 1974. Over the years the system has changed significantly and has gradually grown in acceptance and popularity in more than 30 countries. While there are other writing systems and notation systems for writing signed languages, we have not found any system that is as useful for writing accurately any sign or movement (including all non-manual movements and expressions) with relative ease and speed. It is also the only writing system that we have tried that is possible to read faster than one can physically produce the signs (outpacing even photos and line drawings in ease and speed of reading). We have taken much of the material for this manual from a course we developed for teaching SignWriting (SW) to Deaf adults in Spain. That course in Spain focused on teaching the symbols of SW that are used in Spanish Sign Language (or LSE, for Lengua de Signos Española) with a heavy focus on reading.
    [Show full text]