Visual System: How Does Blindsight Arise?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Visual System: How Does Blindsight Arise? Current Biology Vol 20 No 17 R702 6. Revenkova, E., Herrmann, K., Adelfalk, C., and 12. Revenkova, E., Eijpe, M., Heyting, C., 18. Perez, G.I., Jurisicova, A., Wise, L., Lipina, T., Jessberger, R. (2010). Oocyte cohesin Hodges, C.A., Hunt, P.A., Liebe, B., Kanisek, M., Bechard, A., Takai, Y., Hunt, P., expression restricted to pre-dictyate stages Scherthan, H., and Jessberger, R. (2004). Roder, J., Grynpas, M., et al. (2007). Absence provides full fertility and prevents aneuploidy. Cohesin SMC1 beta is required for meiotic of the proapoptotic Bax protein extends Curr. Biol. 20, 1529–1533. chromosome dynamics, sister chromatid fertility and alleviates age-related health 7. Chiang, T., Duncan, F.E., Schindler, K., cohesion and DNA recombination. Nat. Cell complications in female mice. Proc. Natl. Schultz, R.M., and Lampson, M.A. (2010). Biol. 6, 555–562. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 5229–5234. Evidence that weakened centromere 13. Hodges, C.A., Revenkova, E., Jessberger, R., 19. Garcia-Cruz, R., Brieno, M.A., Roig, I., cohesin is a leading cause of age-related Hassold, T.J., and Hunt, P.A. (2005). Grossmann, M., Velilla, E., Pujol, A., Cabero, L., aneuploidy in oocytes. Curr. Biol. 20, SMC1beta-deficient female mice provide Pessarrodona, A., Barbero, J.L., and Garcia 1522–1528. evidence that cohesins are a missing link in Caldes, M. (2010). Dynamics of cohesin 8. Lister, L., Kouznetsova, A., Hyslop, L., age-related nondisjunction. Nat. Genet. 37, proteins REC8, STAG3, SMC1{beta} and Kalleas, D., Pace, S., Barel, J., Nathan, A., 1351–1355. SMC3 are consistent with a role in sister Floros, V., Adelfalk, C., Watanabe, Y., et al. 14. Subramanian, V.V., and Bickel, S.E. (2008). chromatid cohesion during meiosis in (2010). Age-related meiotic segregation errors Aging predisposes oocytes to meiotic human oocytes. Hum. Reprod. 25, in mammalian oocytes are preceded by nondisjunction when the cohesin subunit 2316–2327. depletion of cohesin and Sgo2. Curr. Biol. 20, SMC1 is reduced. PLoS Genet. 4, e1000263. 20. Hassold, T., and Chiu, D. (1985). Maternal 1511–1521. 15. Hassold, T., and Hunt, P. (2009). Maternal age age-specific rates of numerical chromosome 9. Suja, J.A., and Barbero, J.L. (2009). Cohesin and chromosomally abnormal pregnancies: abnormalities with special reference to complexes and sister chromatid cohesion what we know and what we wish we knew. trisomy. Hum. Genet. 70, 11–17. in mammalian meiosis. Genome Dyn. 5, Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 21, 703–708. 94–116. 16. Oliver, T.R., Feingold, E., Yu, K., Cheung, V., 10. Wood, A.J., Severson, A.F., and Meyer, B.J. Tinker, S., Yadav-Shah, M., Masse, N., and School for Molecular Biosciences, (2010). Condensin and cohesin complexity: the Sherman, S.L. (2008). New insights into human expanding repertoire of functions. Nat. Rev. nondisjunction of chromosome 21 in oocytes. Washington State University, WA 99164, Genet. 11, 391–404. PLoS Genet. 4, e1000033. USA. 11. Sakuno, T., and Watanabe, Y. (2009). 17. Hodges, C.A., Ilagan, A., Jennings, D., Keri, R., *E-mail: [email protected] Studies of meiosis disclose distinct roles Nilson, J., and Hunt, P.A. (2002). Experimental of cohesion in the core centromere and evidence that changes in oocyte growth pericentromeric regions. Chromosome Res. influence meiotic chromosome segregation. 17, 239–249. Hum. Reprod. 17, 1171–1180. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.011 Visual System: How Does Blindsight patients had been tested for decades. Patients were asked whether they saw Arise? anything in their field defects and with the exception of motion — and perhaps not even that — they said ‘‘No’’. But Some patients can discriminate unseen visual stimuli within a field defect monkeys were not asked this question. caused by damage to the primary visual cortex. The pathways for this Instead, and in order to get a reward, ‘blindsight’ have never been established, but recent studies implicate hitherto they had to choose between two overlooked cells in the thalamic LGN. visual stimuli — to make forced-choice decisions. This difference was Alan Cowey and pervades much of the research highlighted when several investigators on what is now called blindsight. [4,5], using forced-choice guessing, The primary visual cortex, or V1, is the Several investigators have studied demonstrated that patients were just as major cortical destination of the input the role of V1 in monkeys, the visual good as monkeys, and Cowey and from the eyes and contains a ‘map’ pathways of which closely resemble Stoerig [6] showed that monkeys of the image on the retina. Hardly our own. They have found an ever categorized visual stimuli that they surprising, then, that when it is partly increasing range of residual visual could detect as being not like a light, but destroyed, as often happens following sensitivity and discrimination within the invisible. In both cases the subjects stroke or traumatic injury to the back visual field defect caused by removing were showing ‘blindsight’, excellent of the brain, the patient has a visual part, or even all, of V1 (see [3] for forced choice performance in the face of field defect in which he is clinically review) — not just reflexes such as denial of consciously seeing anything. blind — part of the map has been the pupillary response to light, but Once the relevance of investigations deleted. Why the term ‘clinically blind’? also learned voluntary responses to on monkeys to human blindsight was Why not just blind? The answer lies in the orientation, shape, brightness, established the search was renewed in a controversy that began almost size and motion of visual stimuli. monkeys for the pathways that underlie a century ago between two eminent Unsurprisingly, monkeys, unlike human it. Did all the pathways shown in Figure 1 British neurologists. Gordon Holmes [1] patients, were considered to have contribute or only one or a few? Early concluded that, in the absence of part genuine residual vision and this was work [7] showed that monkeys with part of the striate cortex, the blindness is attributed to the many other pathways of V1 removed could still move their eyes complete, the field defect is absolute. from the eye into the brain, as shown to targets confined to the field defect but But George Riddoch [2], contemporary schematically in Figure 1. that this ability was destroyed if the and colleague, disagreed and argued But a huge puzzle remained: why corresponding part of the superior that such patients could perceive don’t patients have the same abilities colliculus, which also has a ‘map’ of the motion within their otherwise blind given that they too have these other retina, was subsequently extirpated. field. This controversy, like old pathways? The answer lies in the This still remains strong evidence that volcanoes, has rumbled on ever since different ways in which monkeys and the pathway from the eye to the superior Dispatch R703 colliculus [8], and perhaps onwards LGN LGN from there — which consists of more V1 than 104 fibres from each eye — is PARVO (P) responsible for blindsight, a conclusion V2 V2 that anatomical demonstrations of other MAGNO (M) pathways from eye to cortex that do not V3 V3 INTER (K) INTER (K) involve the superior colliculus have done little to modify. The recent paper by V4 V4 Schmid and colleagues [9] is bound to rekindle this debate. By combining behavioural, functional neuroimaging, V5 V5 and neuropharmacological methods, these authors provide the most compelling evidence yet that a direct TEO TEO pathway from the retina to the thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and from there to extrastriate visual cortex, TE TE is the key to understanding blindsight. The LGN has sometimes been likened IP to a jumbo club sandwich. It has six IP prominent layers of neurons, shown in Figure 2. The top 4, called ‘parvocellular’ because the neurons are small, receives input from the colour-opponent P SC SC ganglion cells of the eyes. The two bottom, ‘magnocellular’ layers receive PGN PGN their input from the retinal M ganglion ON ON cells, which are not colour opponent, or only slightly so. Almost every P and M NOT NOT cell in the LGN, about a million of them, degenerates within a few weeks of DTN DTN removing V1. It was previously assumed LTN LTN that all blindsight must therefore derive from the retinal ganglion cells that MTN MTN project to the midbrain, as shown in Figure 1. SCN SCN But what about the layers of the LGN between the P and M layers? In these interlaminar layers, the cells are Current Biology small and so pale in conventional Nissl stained sections that they Figure 1. The known pathways from the eye to the brain. were mistaken for interneurons or non-neuronal glial cells by many The right side of the diagram shows the normal arrangement; the left side shows the effect of removing V1. Thicker lines show the heavier projections. Dotted lines show numerically weaker investigators for many years. That this projections. The many onward cortical pathways from V2, V3, V4, etc. are omitted. On the left, lack- was incorrect became clear only ing V1, the major visual input to the temporal lobe processing stream is diminished but that from 10 years ago [10] with the discovery the K cells remains. The subcortical nuclei within the square outline at the bottom are chiefly con- that the K, or ‘konio’ retinal cells project cerned with reflexive responses to light. Labelling from bottom upwards: SCN, suprachiasmatic to these interlaminar layers, the cells nucleus; MTN, LTN and DTN, medial, lateral and dorsal terminal accessory optic nuclei; NOT, of which are as abundant as the large nucleus of the optic tract; ON, olivary nucleus; PGN, pregeniculate nucleus; SC, superior collicu- lus; IP, inferior pulvinar; LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus.
Recommended publications
  • Consciousness:** Here,*There*But*Not*Everywhere*
    Tononi%&%Koch%*%Consciousness% % % % Consciousness:** Here,*There*but*Not*Everywhere* by* Giulio*Tononi1*and*Christof*Koch2* % 1Department%of%Psychiatry,%University%of%Wisconsin,%Madison%WI%USA% 2Allen%Institute%for%Brain%Science,%Seattle,%WA%USA% % May%26,%2014% Tononi%&%Koch%–%Consciousness%*%2% Abstract% The%science%of%consciousness%has%made%great%strides%by%focusing%on% the% behavioral% and% neuronal% correlates% of% experience.% However,% correlates%are%not%enough%if%we%are%to%understand%even%basic%facts,%for% example,%why%the%cerebral%cortex%gives%rise%to%consciousness%but%the% cerebellum%does%not,%though%it%has%even%more%neurons%and%appears%to% be%just%as%complicated.%Moreover,%correlates%are%of%little%help%in%many% instances% where% we% would% like% to% know% if% consciousness% is% present:% patients% with% a% few%remaining%islands%of%functioning%cortex,%pre*term% infants,% non*mammalian% species,% and% machines% that% are% rapidly% outperforming% people% at% driving,% recognizing% faces% and% objects,% and% answering% difficult% questions.% To% address% these% issues,% we% need% not% only%more%data,%but%also%a%theory%of%consciousness%–%one%that%says%what% experience%is%and%what%type%of%physical%systems%can%have%it.%Integrated% Information%Theory%(IIT)%does%so%by%starting%from%conscious%experience% itself% via% five% phenomenological% axioms% of% existence,% composition,% information,% integration,%and%exclusion.%From%these%it%derives%five% postulates% about% the%properties%required%of%physical%mechanisms%to% support% consciousness.%
    [Show full text]
  • (Homonymous Hemianopsia): Tapping Into Blindsight
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector Journal of Medical Hypotheses and Ideas (2015) 9,S8–S13 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Medical Hypotheses and Ideas journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jmhi REGULAR ARTICLE Enhancing visual performance in individuals with cortical visual impairment (homonymous hemianopsia): Tapping into blindsight Faith A. Birnbaum a, Steven A. Hackley b, Lenworth N. Johnson a,* a Neuro-Ophthalmology Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, The Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University/Lifespan/Rhode Island Hospital, Providence, RI, United States b Department of Psychological Sciences of the University of Missouri Columbia, Columbia, MO, United States Received 2 October 2015; revised 29 November 2015; accepted 15 December 2015 Available online 22 January 2016 KEYWORDS Abstract Homonymous hemianopsia is a type of cortical blindness in which vision is lost Blindsight; completely or partially in the left half or the right half of the field of vision. It is prevalent in Cortical blindness; approximately 12% of traumatic brain injury and 35% of strokes. Patients often experience Homonymous hemianopsia; difficulty with activities such as ambulating, eating, reading, and driving. Due to the high prevalence Augmented virtual reality; of homonymous hemianopsia and its associated difficulties, it is imperative to find methods for Vision restoration therapy visual rehabilitation in this condition. Traditional methods such as prism glasses can cause visual confusion and result in patient noncompliance. There is a large unmet medical need for improving this condition. In this article, we propose that modifying visual stimuli to activate non-cortical areas of visual processing, such as lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus, may result in increased visual awareness.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Sense of Blindsense-Author's Response To: Commentary by Ian
    Making sense of blindsense-Author’s response to: Commentary by Ian Phillips, Clémentine Garric, Florent Caetta, Claire Sergent, Sylvie Chokron To cite this version: Clémentine Garric, Florent Caetta, Claire Sergent, Sylvie Chokron. Making sense of blindsense- Author’s response to: Commentary by Ian Phillips,. Cortex, Elsevier, 2020, 127, pp.393-395. 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.03.004. hal-03031396 HAL Id: hal-03031396 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03031396 Submitted on 30 Nov 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Authors’ Response to: Commentary by Ian Phillips, Making sense of blindsense Clémentine Garrica,, Florent Caettab,c, Claire Sergentc & Sylvie Chokronb,c a Univ. Lille, Inserm, CHU Lille, U1172 - LilNCog - Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, F- 59000 Lille, France b Institut de Neuropsychologie, Neurovision et Neurocognition, Fondation Ophtalmologique Rothschild, Paris 75019, France c Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center - UMR 8002 CNRS/Université Paris Descartes, Paris 75006, France. Abstract We recently published the results of a study on the occurrence of blindsight among eight, post-stroke homonymous hemianopic (HH) patients (Garric et al., 2019), in whom we measured blindsight through forced-choice tasks and assessed perceptual experiences by a new awareness scale, the Sensation Awareness Scale (SAS).
    [Show full text]
  • Disentangling Mechanisms, Brain Systems, and Behavioral Responses
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Repositório Institucional UNIFESP 10882 • The Journal of Neuroscience, November 8, 2017 • 37(45):10882–10893 Symposium Consciousness Regained: Disentangling Mechanisms, Brain Systems, and Behavioral Responses X Johan F. Storm,1 XMe´lanie Boly,2,3 X Adenauer G. Casali,4 XMarcello Massimini,5,6 XUmberto Olcese,7,8 X Cyriel M.A. Pennartz,7,8 and XMelanie Wilke9,10,11 1Division of Physiology, Department of Molecular Medicine, Institute of Basal Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, 0317, Norway, 2Department of Neurology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin, 53705, 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin, 53719, 4Institute of Science and Technology, Federal University of Sa˜o Paulo, Sa˜o Jose´ dos Campos, SP, 12231-280, Brazil, 5Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences Luigi Sacco, University of Milan, Milan, 20157, Italy, 6Istituto Di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico, Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi ONLUS, 20162 Milano, Italy, 7Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Group, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences and 8Research Priority Program Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1098XH, The Netherlands, 9Department of Cognitive Neurology, University Medicine Goettingen, Goettingen, 37075, Germany, 10German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, 37077, Germany, and 11Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Goettingen, 37077, Germany How consciousness (experience) arises from and relates to material brain processes (the “mind-body problem”) has been pondered by thinkers for centuries, and is regarded as among the deepest unsolved problems in science, with wide-ranging theoretical, clinical, and ethical implications. Until the last few decades, this was largely seen as a philosophical topic, but not widely accepted in mainstream neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • A Traditional Scientific Perspective on the Integrated Information Theory Of
    entropy Article A Traditional Scientific Perspective on the Integrated Information Theory of Consciousness Jon Mallatt The University of Washington WWAMI Medical Education Program at The University of Idaho, Moscow, ID 83844, USA; [email protected] Abstract: This paper assesses two different theories for explaining consciousness, a phenomenon that is widely considered amenable to scientific investigation despite its puzzling subjective aspects. I focus on Integrated Information Theory (IIT), which says that consciousness is integrated information (as φMax) and says even simple systems with interacting parts possess some consciousness. First, I evaluate IIT on its own merits. Second, I compare it to a more traditionally derived theory called Neurobiological Naturalism (NN), which says consciousness is an evolved, emergent feature of complex brains. Comparing these theories is informative because it reveals strengths and weaknesses of each, thereby suggesting better ways to study consciousness in the future. IIT’s strengths are the reasonable axioms at its core; its strong logic and mathematical formalism; its creative “experience- first” approach to studying consciousness; the way it avoids the mind-body (“hard”) problem; its consistency with evolutionary theory; and its many scientifically testable predictions. The potential weakness of IIT is that it contains stretches of logic-based reasoning that were not checked against hard evidence when the theory was being constructed, whereas scientific arguments require such supporting evidence to keep the reasoning on course. This is less of a concern for the other theory, NN, because it incorporated evidence much earlier in its construction process. NN is a less mature theory than IIT, less formalized and quantitative, and less well tested.
    [Show full text]
  • Blindsight: What Happens When V1 Is Damaged?
    Blindsight: What happens when V1 is damaged? • Individuals who have experienced a stroke that has selectively damaged area V1, or who have had V1 removed surgically, report being blind in the affected part of visual space (e.g., damage to left V1 affects the right visual field). • However, if these individuals are asked to point to an object to reach out and grasp an object in the “blind” field, they can do so accurately, even though they claim they are just guessing. Blindsight: What happens when V1 is damaged? • Similarly if they are asked to report on an object in the good visual field, the presentation of objects in the impaired field can influence those reports (e.g., responses to objects in the good field are slowed down when another object is presented at the same in the bad field compared to when it is shown alone). • Some visual information is getting through to the parts of the brain that control action, but not entering awareness. • Does this mean that V1 is necessary for visual awareness? Blindsight patient GY has a lesion in left V1. He reports being blind to objects in the right visual field, yet he can correctly point to and grasp them. These fMRI scans show robust activity in extrastriate visual areas when stimuli were presented in the ‘blind’ region. Without V1, how did these signals get to the dorsal pathway? There’s a second pathway to the dorsal stream that bypasses V1! Retina – Superior Colliculus - Pulvinar So damage to V1 can lead to sight without awareness (blindsight).
    [Show full text]
  • A Higher-Order Theory of Emotional Consciousness. Joseph Ledoux and Richard Brown
    1 SUPPORTING INFORMATION Feeling It: A Higher-Order Theory of Emotional Consciousness. Joseph LeDoux and Richard Brown Table of Contents Page Number Box 1: Defining Cortical vs. Subcortical Circuits 2 Box 2: The Consciousness Patients: How Neurological 2 Cases Helped Revive Interest in Consciousness as a Scientific Topic Box 3: Measuring Introspective Consciousness Using 3 Verbal and other Reporting Methods in Humans and Other Animals Box 4: Nonconscious Cognition (Especially Nonconscious 7 Working Memory) Box 5: Rich versus Sparse views on Phenomenology of 10 Perception Box 6: Role of Awareness in First-Order Phenomenal 12 Consciousness Box 7: Higher-Order Theories and their Relation to Other 15 Cognitive Theories of Consciousness Box 8: Phenomenal Consciousness in Higher-order 19 Thought Theories of Consciousness Box 9: Relational versus Non-Relational Higher-Order 19 Theories Box 10: Research Questions 21 2 BOX 1: DEFINING CORTICAL VS. SUBCORTICAL CIRCUITS. For the purposes of this article we distinguish “cortical” and “subcortical” circuits of the cerebral hemispheres in the following way. “Cortical” is used in reference to laminated structures in the outer shell (i.e. the cortex) of cerebral hemispheres. Particular emphasis is placed on cortical regions that contribute to sensory processing (areas of visual cortex) and higher cognitive processing (especially lateral and medial prefrontal cortex, but also parietal and insular cortex). “Subcortical” is used to refer to structures that lie within, but underneath the cortical mantle, of the cerebral hemispheres, and that lack prominent or consistent lamination. Circuits centered on the amygdala that detect innate and learned threats and control the expression of defensive behaviors and supporting physiological responses in response to theses stimuli are emphasized.
    [Show full text]
  • The Timing and Neuroanatomy of Conscious Vision As Revealed by TMS-Induced Blindsight
    The Timing and Neuroanatomy of Conscious Vision as Revealed by TMS-induced Blindsight Christopher P. G. Allen, Petroc Sumner, and Christopher D. Chambers Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/26/7/1507/1781339/jocn_a_00557.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Abstract ■ Following damage to the primary visual cortex, some patients dicating that such pathways, which include those to the superior exhibit “blindsight,” where they report a loss of awareness while colliculus, are not critical. We also found that the modulation of retaining the ability to discriminate visual stimuli above chance. conscious vision was time and pathway dependent. TMS applied Transient disruption of occipital regions with TMS can produce either early (0–40 msec) or late (280–320 msec) after stimulus a similar dissociation, known as TMS-induced blindsight. The onset modulated detection of stimuli that did not bypass R/M neural basis of this residual vision is controversial, with some pathways, whereas during an intermediate period (90–130 msec) studies attributing it to the retinotectal pathway via the superior the effect was pathway independent. Our findings thus suggest colliculus whereas others implicate spared projections that origi- a prominent role for the R/M pathways in supporting both the nate predominantly from the LGN. Here we contrasted these preparatory and later stages of conscious vision. This may help accounts by combining TMS with visual stimuli that either acti- resolve apparent conflict in previous literature by demonstrating vate or bypass the retinotectal and magnocellular (R/M) path- that the roles of the retinotectal and geniculate pathways are ways.
    [Show full text]
  • From Affective Blindsight to Emotional Consciousness
    Consciousness and Cognition 36 (2015) 414–425 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Consciousness and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/concog From affective blindsight to emotional consciousness ⇑ Alessia Celeghin a,b,c, Beatrice de Gelder d, Marco Tamietto a,b,e, a Department of Psychology, University of Torino, Italy b Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, CoRPS – Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic Diseases, Tilburg University, The Netherlands c GCS fMRI, Koelliker Hospital, Torino, Italy d Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands e Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK article info abstract Article history: Following destruction or denervation of the primary visual cortex (V1) cortical blindness Received 18 January 2015 ensues. Affective blindsight refers to the uncanny ability of such patients to respond cor- Revised 12 May 2015 rectly, or above chance level, to visual emotional expressions presented to their blind Accepted 13 May 2015 fields. Fifteen years after its original discovery, affective blindsight still fascinates neurosci- Available online 6 June 2015 entists and philosophers alike, as it offers a unique window on the vestigial properties of our visual system that, though present in the intact brain, tend to be unnoticed or even Keywords: actively inhibited by conscious processes. Here we review available studies on affective Cortical blindness blindsight with the intent to clarify its functional properties, neural bases and theoretical V1 Emotion implications. Evidence converges on the role of subcortical structures of old evolutionary Awareness origin such as the superior colliculus, the pulvinar and the amygdala in mediating affective Amygdala blindsight and nonconscious perception of emotions.
    [Show full text]
  • Qt5495t34x Nosplash 48C6640
    Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2016, 1–8 doi: 10.1093/nc/niw001 Opinion Paper Who’s afraid of response bias? Megan A. K. Peters1,*, Tony Ro2 and Hakwan Lau1,3 1Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, 1285 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563 USA; 2Programs in Psychology and Biology, The Graduate Center of the City University of New York, 365 Fifth Ave., New York, NY 10016 USA; 3Brain Research Institute, University of California Los Angeles, 695 Charles E. Young Dr. South, Los Angeles, CA 90095 USA *Correspondence address. Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, 1285 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563 USA, Tel: þ1 323-596-1093, E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Response bias (or criterion) contamination is insidious in studies of consciousness: that observers report they do not see a stimulus may not mean they have absolutely no subjective experience; they may be giving such reports in relative terms in the context of other stimuli. Bias-free signal detection theoretic measures provide an excellent method for avoiding response bias confounds, and many researchers correctly adopt this approach. However, here we discuss how a fixation on avoiding criterion effects can also be misleading and detrimental to fruitful inquiry. In a recent paper, Balsdon and Azzopardi (Absolute and relative blindsight. Consciousness and Cognition 2015; 32:79–91.) claimed that contamination by response bias led to flawed findings in a previous report of “relative blindsight”. We argue that their criticisms are unfounded. They mistakenly assumed that others were trying (and failing) to apply their preferred methods to remove bias, when there was no such intention.
    [Show full text]
  • Dennett on the Blind Spot, Blindsight, and Sensory Qualia
    CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 2, 155-164 (1993) Why the Blind Can't Lead the Blind: Dennett on the Blind Spot, Blindsight, and Sensory Qualia RoBERT N. McCAULEY Department of Philosophy, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322 In Consciousness Explained Dan Dennett proposes a deflationary treatment of sensory qualia. He seeks to establish a continuity among both the neural and the conscious phenomena connected with the blind spot and with the perception of repetitive patterns on the one hand and the neutral and conscious phenomena connected with blindsight on the other. He aims to analyze the conscious phenomena associated with each in terms of what the brain ignores. Dennett offers a thought experiment about a blindsight patient who has the sensory information that normals do , but who seems not to have their sensory qualia. What is it that normals know that this blindsight patient does not? Dennett' s answer is "nothing." Dennett's denial of " filling-in " accounts of repetitive patterns and the blind spot constitute a Rylean intuition pump for this thought experi­ ment. Research by Ramachandran raises important problems for Dennett's account. Moreover, Dennett's attempt to discount the significance of "artificial scotomas" inad­ vertently employs a principle that undermines his case for establishing the continuity between the phenomen.a in question. © 1993 Academic Press, Inc. I. INTRODUCTION In Consciousness Explained Dan Dennett offers a welcome corrective to much loose talk about consciousness by dualists and materialists alike. He maintains that our conscious mental life is neither a report on a single stream of representa­ tions from a homunculus in the mind's central clearinghouse nor the registration of such representations at a comparable clearinghouse somewhere in the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Blindsight Revisited Lawrence Weiskrantz
    215 Blindsight revisited Lawrence Weiskrantz Some human patients with lesions to their primary visual methods originally designed to test animal vision, it was (striate) cortex o/l) demonstrate residual visual capacity, discovered that some of these patients can perform visual but without acknowledged perceptual awareness. This discriminations at a high level, such as making a saccade phenomenon has been termed blindsight. Recent results from to a visual stimulus or a correct forced-choice response work on blindsight patients suggest that it is unlikely to be between two or more alternatives [9-111. Nevertheless, attributable to intact residual areas (tags) of Vl . Previous the patients persist in saying they are ‘blind’ in the research has reported that blindsight patients can retain tested region of the field, and comment that they are the ability to detect monochromatic light and grating stimuli, ‘just guessing’. This phenomenon-visual discrimination and to discriminate orientation and direction of movement in the absence of acknowledged awareness -has been in their ‘blind’ fields. These findings have been joined by dubbed ‘blindsight’ [lo]. reports that these patients also are sensitive to, and are able to discriminate, wavelength in the absence of any experience of ‘colour’. This reveals that retinal pathways Figure 1 other than those to the striate cortex are crucially involved in vision. Conditions can be controlled for obtaining either acknowledged awareness or unawareness of discrimination of the direction of a small moving target
    [Show full text]