Linguishtik Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Linguishtik Review LinguiSHTIK Review Sentences: The player who rolls the dice must declare the type of sentence to be used that game. 1. Simple Sentence: A single independent clause. Examples: Sam likes pizza. The dog ran away from home. 2. Compound Sentence: Has two or more simple sentences joined together with a conjunction like FOR, AND, NOR, BUT, OR, YET (FANBOYS) Examples: John ate the pizza, but Eliza ate the hotdog. Ms. Shipley sang a song, and the dog ran away from her. 3. Complex Sentence: Has one main simple sentence and at least one subordinate clause (cannot stand alone). A subordinate clause often starts with since, although, until, however, therefore, or because. Examples: The boy wanted the football because it was his birthday. Since it is not a school night, you may stay up later. 4. Compound-Complex Sentence: Has two simple sentences with one subordinate clause. Examples: Because my homework was difficult, I had to get help, and I stayed up late. Remember: Sentences must begin with a capital letter, end with the correct punctuation. The word in the challenge MUST be underlines and spelled correctly. Demands: There are three types of demands: 1. Type: Part of Speech: noun, verb, adjective, adverb, pronoun, interjection, conjunction, preposition. 2. Function: Usage of the part of speech Examples: noun must be the predicate nominative, noun must be an object of the preposition, abstract noun. 3. General: This demand can be almost anything: must be a palindrome, must be an animal, etc. Part of Speech: Nouns Noun: a word that names a person, place or thing. Examples: pizza, cats, desk, flower, love Once a noun demand has been called there many other demands you can make for the function of the noun. o Nouns can be SINGULAR or PLURAL. Singular nouns name one thing, while plural nouns name more than one thing: House, houses; ox, oxen; kiss, kisses; goose, geese. *A good strategy is to call a plural noun, and then say it can’t have an s. Some plural nouns without an s are oxen, deer, mice, feet, geese, dice, hair, teeth o Nouns can be COLLECTIVE. Collective nouns name more than one person, place, or thing, even when even when they are singular Examples: army, class, herd, tribe, school, pride, army, flock, band, bevy, brood, cast, pack, yoke, hive o Nouns are often the SUBJECT (noun performing the action of a sentence; who or what the sentence is about) of the sentence: The balloon blew away. o Nouns can be the DIRECT OBJECT (noun receiving the action of a sentence): I dropped my money. o A PREDICATE NOUN comes after the verb and renames the subject: Benjamin Franklin was an inventor. o A noun can be the OBJECT OF THE PREPOSITION (the noun the preposition refers to): The ball is behind the bush. I talked over my brother. o A NOUN ADJUNCT is a noun that acts as an adjective: shoe store, paper route o A noun can be an APPOSITIVE (it renames or further describes another noun). It is always right next to the noun it is renaming. It is set off from the rest of the sentence by commas: Ms. Shipley, a teacher, made me study these words. o A noun can be CONCRETE (nouns you can know with your five senses): candy, thunder, desk, pizza o A noun can be ABSTRACT (nouns associated with emotions or ideas): love, pride, hope, anger o A noun can be POSSESSIVE (nouns that show ownership): The dog’s toy squeaked. Part of Speech: Pronouns Pronoun-Takes the place of one or more nouns: he, she, it, they, they, them, us, we, me When pronoun is called, any of the noun function demands can be called except noun adjunct. Pronouns can be PERSONAL (refer to specific people, places, things, or ideas): they, them Pronouns can be INDEFINITE (refer to unspecified people, places, things and ideas): another, anybody, anyone, both, each, either, many, most, none, some Prounouns can be POSSESSIVE (show ownership): The pizza is theirs. Parts of Speech: Verbs Verb-a word that expresses action: kick, jump, think, dive, stop or state of being: is, was, am, are, were, be or a linking verb (relates the subject to the predicate): seems, feels, tastes, appears Verbs can be SINGULAR: The bird flies. or PLURAL: The birds sing. Verbs can be REGULAR (uses “ed” to form a past tense): He passed me the chocolate. Verbs can be IRREGULAR (makes the past tense without using “ed”): We began the race.*Note: A good strategy when a verb and past tense have been called is to call “must not contain a D”. Here’s a list of past tense verbs that do not contain D:bore, beat, began, lost, blew, broke, brought, burst, caught, chose, came, crept, fell, flung, drove, said, lent, rang, rose, shook, shone, flew, froze, gave, grew, went, knew, sang, spoke, laid, stole, stung, swore, wore, threw, rode Verbs can be PAST TENSE: He threw the ball. Verbs can be PRESENT TENSE: He throws the ball. Verbs can be FUTURE TENSE: He will throw the ball. Verbs can be INFINITIVE (a verb preceded by the word “to): to wait, to jump, to walk. *Note-the first demand should be verb, and then you demand the word must be part of an infinitive. You can demand that a verb be part of an INFINITIVE PHRASE (anything after the word “to” in the sentence: I planned to walk to school in Thursday. Parts of Speech: Adjectives Adjective-a word that describes, or modifies a noun or pronoun. Examples: silly, rich, happy, green, three *Note-colors and numbers are adjectives! Demands you can call after an adjective demand: must modify a noun, must modify a pronoun, must be a predicate adjective, must be an adjacent adjective (right next to the noun) Adjectives can be POSITIVE (normal form you would find in the dictionary): pretty, kind, bald, funny Adjectives can be COMPARATIVE (compares two objects by adding “er”): prettier, kinder, balder, funnier Adjectives can be SUPERALATIVE by adding “est: prettiest, kindest, baldest, funniest Parts of Speech: Adverbs Adverb- an adverb describes or modifies a verb, adjective, or another adverb. A lot of adverbs are made by taking an adjective and adding “ly”: quickly, swiftly, excitedly, slowly, very, soon, next, fast After an adverb has been called, you can say it has to be a VERB MODIFIER (describes verb): She fell quickly. You can say it has to be an ADJECTIVE MODIFIER (describes adjective): The very old man died. or ADVERB MODIFIER (describes adverb): The man speaks very well. Parts of Speech: Prepositions Preposition-words that usually show something’s position in space. These can’t stand alone and must be part of a prepositional phrase. Examples: about, above, across, under, over, down, into, past, upon, from, below. The ball was above the net. The entire prepositional phrase here is “above the net”. You can call a PREPOSITION, the word must be part of a PREPOSITIONAL PHRASE, or the word must be an OBJECT OF THE PREPOSITION (the thing in the position the preposition refers to): The man is behind the tree. “behind the tree” is the prepositional phrase, “behind” is the preposition, and “tree” is the object of the preposition. Parts of Speech: Conjunctions Conjunction-conjunctions join words or groups of words: I bought a present after I saved enough money. The girl was upset because her toy broke. I was hungry, but I forgot my lunch. Conjunctions can be COORDINATING CONJUNCTIONS (they join two independent clauses in compound and compound-complex sentences) There are only seven of them and they all have fewer than four letters: for, and, nor, but, or, yet, so (FANBOYS) Conjunctions can be SUBORDINATING CONJUNCTIONS (they begin a dependent clause and are used to form complex and compound-complex sentences): after, although, because, though, unless, before, provided, since, until, while, however. Parts of Speech: Interjections Interjection- an exclamation at the beginning of a sentence, separated by a comma: Help, the bear is chasing me! Ouch, that hurt! Alas, he was a good friend! Other interjections: Cheers, dear, gosh, hail, hoot, oops, phew, poof, whee, zooks Other Useful Function and General Demands Gerund-a verbal noun formed by adding ing to a verb. Must be used as the subject, thus becoming a noun: Eating is fun. Singing makes me happy. *Note-first demand could be a noun, then a gerund, OR first demand could be a verb then a gerund. Palindromes- words spelled the same backwards and forwards: toot, noon, peep, level, sees, radar. *Note-some of these are great for multiple demands. Peep could be a verb or a noun, a palindrome, and onomatopoeia. Backwards/Forwards Words- a different word spelled backwards and forwards: step-pets, star-rats, stop-pots, stab-bats, deer-reed *Note-you only have to use one part in your sentence. Portmanteau- words combined to form two words: spork, skort, smog, brunch, twirl Onomatopoeia- words that sound like the actual sound: peep, boom, fizz, hiss, buzz, click Tautonym- words with repeating syllables: tutu, murmur, mama, gaga, papa *Note-you can’t use words with hyphens. 1st Degree Isogram-words with no repeating letters: cold, barn, walk, desk 2nd Degree Isogram-each letter appears twice: deed, noon, peep 3rd Degree Isogram- each letter appears 3 times: deeded Acronym-a word in which each letter stands for another word. You can’t use words with periods: scuba, laser, sonar, radar, snafu, news Heteronym- words with two pronunciations, separate meanings: dove, bass, wind, does, sewer, lead, wound Anagram- words with same letters, different order: dale-deal, star-rats-arts, teas-seat, teal-late Alliteration- at least 3 words in a row that begin with the same consonant or vowel sound: The big boy bowled.
Recommended publications
  • Long and Short Adjunct Fronting in HPSG
    Long and Short Adjunct Fronting in HPSG Takafumi Maekawa Department of Language and Linguistics University of Essex Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar Linguistic Modelling Laboratory, Institute for Parallel Processing, Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Sofia, Held in Varna Stefan Muller¨ (Editor) 2006 CSLI Publications pages 212–227 http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HPSG/2006 Maekawa, Takafumi. 2006. Long and Short Adjunct Fronting in HPSG. In Muller,¨ Stefan (Ed.), Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, Varna, 212–227. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Abstract The purpose of this paper is to consider the proper treatment of short- and long-fronted adjuncts within HPSG. In the earlier HPSG analyses, a rigid link between linear order and constituent structure determines the linear position of such adjuncts in the sentence-initial position. This paper argues that there is a body of data which suggests that ad- junct fronting does not work as these approaches predict. It is then shown that linearisation-based HPSG can provide a fairly straightfor- ward account of the facts. 1 Introduction The purpose of this paper is to consider the proper treatment of short- and long-fronted adjuncts within HPSG. ∗ The following sentences are typical examples. (1) a. On Saturday , will Dana go to Spain? (Short-fronted adjunct) b. Yesterday I believe Kim left. (Long-fronted adjunct) In earlier HPSG analyses, a rigid link between linear order and constituent structure determines the linear position of such adverbials in the sen- tence-initial position. I will argue that there is a body of data which sug- gests that adjunct fronting does not work as these approaches predict.
    [Show full text]
  • Animacy and Alienability: a Reconsideration of English
    Running head: ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 1 Animacy and Alienability A Reconsideration of English Possession Jaimee Jones A Senior Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation in the Honors Program Liberty University Spring 2016 ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 2 Acceptance of Senior Honors Thesis This Senior Honors Thesis is accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation from the Honors Program of Liberty University. ______________________________ Jaeshil Kim, Ph.D. Thesis Chair ______________________________ Paul Müller, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Jeffrey Ritchey, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Brenda Ayres, Ph.D. Honors Director ______________________________ Date ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 3 Abstract Current scholarship on English possessive constructions, the s-genitive and the of- construction, largely ignores the possessive relationships inherent in certain English compound nouns. Scholars agree that, in general, an animate possessor predicts the s- genitive while an inanimate possessor predicts the of-construction. However, the current literature rarely discusses noun compounds, such as the table leg, which also express possessive relationships. However, pragmatically and syntactically, a compound cannot be considered as a true possessive construction. Thus, this paper will examine why some compounds still display possessive semantics epiphenomenally. The noun compounds that imply possession seem to exhibit relationships prototypical of inalienable possession such as body part, part whole, and spatial relationships. Additionally, the juxtaposition of the possessor and possessum in the compound construction is reminiscent of inalienable possession in other languages. Therefore, this paper proposes that inalienability, a phenomenon not thought to be relevant in English, actually imbues noun compounds whose components exhibit an inalienable relationship with possessive semantics.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • Ed 205 72 Th 810 457
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 205 72 TH 810 457 AUTHOR Mullis, Ina V. S.: Mellon, John C. TTTL2 Guidelines for Describing Three.Aspects of Writing: .Syntax, Cohesion and Mechanics. INSTITUTION Education Commission of the States, 'Denver, Colo. National Assessment of Educational Progress. SPONS AGENCY, National Inst. of Education (ED1, Washington, D.C. REFORT NO NAEP-10-W-50 PUB DATE Jun 80' GIANT NIE-G-80-0003 NOTE 44. EDP! pPicr HF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. .D17SCRIPTORS Elementary Secondary Education: Essays; *Evaluation Methods: National Competency Tests: *National Programs: Scoring: *Writing Evaluation: *Writing Skills IDENTIFIEPS *National Assessment of Educational Progress: *Third Writing Assessment (19791 ABSTRACT The approaches used by the National Assessment of rducational Progress (NAEP1 to characterize syntax, cohesion and mechanics are described. These approaches are different fLom the Primary Trait System,-a scheme used by NAM) for rating essays. Part one of the publication describes the procedures NAEP used to characterize differences in syntactic structures. This Approach is bssed bn an analysis of the grammatical forms in which sentences are cast. Part two describes the procedure used to rate the cohesiveness of each essay. This approach is concerted with the number and variety of devices used to link-and7carry forward developing ideas. Part three,presents the procedures NAEP used to characterize writing mechanics. This approach accounts for matters of manuscript form. The systems used to tally error rates are essentially unchanged from those used in earlier assessments, with some.refinements, (Author/GKI 'Procedures (Analysis)(Conceptual) .1 *******************t************************************************ * 'Peproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can .be made .from *he original document.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax of Answers to Negative Yes/No-Questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University
    The syntax of answers to negative yes/no-questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University 1. Introduction This paper will argue that answers to polar questions or yes/no-questions (YNQs) in English are elliptical expressions with basically the structure (1), where IP is identical to the LF of the IP of the question, containing a polarity variable with two possible values, affirmative or negative, which is assigned a value by the focused polarity expression. (1) yes/no Foc [IP ...x... ] The crucial data come from answers to negative questions. English turns out to have a fairly complicated system, with variation depending on which negation is used. The meaning of the answer yes in (2) is straightforward, affirming that John is coming. (2) Q(uestion): Isn’t John coming, too? A(nswer): Yes. (‘John is coming.’) In (3) (for speakers who accept this question as well formed), 1 the meaning of yes alone is indeterminate, and it is therefore not a felicitous answer in this context. The longer version is fine, affirming that John is coming. (3) Q: Isn’t John coming, either? A: a. #Yes. b. Yes, he is. In (4), there is variation regarding the interpretation of yes. Depending on the context it can be a confirmation of the negation in the question, meaning ‘John is not coming’. In other contexts it will be an infelicitous answer, as in (3). (4) Q: Is John not coming? A: a. Yes. (‘John is not coming.’) b. #Yes. In all three cases the (bare) answer no is unambiguous, meaning that John is not coming.
    [Show full text]
  • Early Sensitivity to Telicity: the Role of the Count/Mass Distinction in Event Individuation
    W O R K I N G P A P E R S I N L I N G U I S T I C S The notes and articles in this series are progress reports on work being carried on by students and faculty in the Department. Because these papers are not finished products, readers are asked not to cite from them without noting their preliminary nature. The authors welcome any comments and suggestions that readers might offer. Volume 41(4) 2010 (April) DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA HONOLULU 96822 An Equal Opportunity/Affirmative Action Institution WORKING PAPERS IN LINGUISTICS: UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA, VOL. 41(4) DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS FACULTY 2010 Victoria B. Anderson Byron W. Bender (Emeritus) Benjamin Bergen Derek Bickerton (Emeritus) Robert A. Blust Robert L. Cheng (Adjunct) Kenneth W. Cook (Adjunct) Kamil Deen Patricia J. Donegan (Co-Graduate Chair) Katie K. Drager Emanuel J. Drechsel (Adjunct) Michael L. Forman (Emeritus) George W. Grace (Emeritus) John H. Haig (Adjunct) Roderick A. Jacobs (Emeritus) Paul Lassettre P. Gregory Lee Patricia A. Lee Howard P. McKaughan (Emeritus) William O’Grady (Chair) Yuko Otsuka Ann Marie Peters (Emeritus, Co-Graduate Chair) Kenneth L. Rehg Lawrence A. Reid (Emeritus) Amy J. Schafer Albert J. Schütz, (Emeritus, Editor) Ho Min Sohn (Adjunct) Nicholas Thieberger Laurence C. Thompson (Emeritus) ii EARLY SENSITIVITY TO TELICITY: THE ROLE OF THE COUNT/MASS DISTINCTION IN EVENT INDIVIDUATION YUKIE HARA1 This paper presents evidence that English-speaking children are sensitive to telicity based on the count/mass distinction of the object noun in verb phrases such as eat an apple (telic) vs.
    [Show full text]
  • The Serial Verb Construction in Chinese: a Tenacious Myth and a Gordian Knot Waltraud Paul
    The serial verb construction in Chinese: A tenacious myth and a Gordian knot Waltraud Paul To cite this version: Waltraud Paul. The serial verb construction in Chinese: A tenacious myth and a Gordian knot. Lin- guistic Review, De Gruyter, 2008, 25 (3-4), pp.367-411. 10.1515/TLIR.2008.011. halshs-01574253 HAL Id: halshs-01574253 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01574253 Submitted on 12 Aug 2017 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. The serial verb construction in Chinese: A tenacious myth and a Gordian knot1 WALTRAUD PAUL Abstract The term “construction” is not a label to be assigned randomly, but presup- poses a structural analysis with an associated set of syntactic and semantic properties. Based on this premise, the term “serial verb construction” (SVC) as currently used in Chinese linguistics will be shown to simply refer to any multi- verb surface string i.e,. to subsume different constructions. The synchronic consequence of this situation is that SVCs in Chinese linguistics are not com- mensurate with SVCs in, e.g., Niger-Congo languages, whence the futility at this stage to search for a “serialization parameter” deriving the differences between so-called “serializing” and “non-serializing” languages.
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER Nouns and Modifiers
    Ch14_pp152–162.qxd 6/13/06 9:43 AM Page 152 CHAPTER 14 Nouns and Modifiers Overview This chapter deals with many ways to modify a noun. It begins with the terminology “adjective” and “noun”; then it shows that a noun can modify another noun. Students learn the order of nouns and their modifiers, some of which come before and some of which follow the noun. Next, students practice using multiple modifiers and learn some new vocabulary. This is followed by practice with phrases of quantity that modify nouns. Students then study how to solve subject-verb agreement problems that arise when they use these phrases. Indefinite pronouns and the use of every follow. Finally, students are introduced to the way adjectives are used with linking verbs and how common adverbs are contrasted with adjectives. □ EXERCISE 1, p. 405. Noun and adjective practice. This exercise is a quick review of what students already know about nouns and adjectives. It can be done in class. Its purpose is to check on students’ understanding of the grammar terms “noun” and “adjective” before proceeding with the chapter. ANSWERS: 3. ADJ 4. NOUN 5. NOUN 6. NOUN 7. ADJ 8. NOUN 9. ADJ 10. NOUN 11. ADJ 12. NOUN CHART 14-1: MODIFYING NOUNS WITH ADJECTIVES AND NOUNS • The word modify is explained in Chart 6-2, p. 161, in the student book. • One unusual feature of English is that a noun can modify another noun. These nouns are called “noun adjuncts” and do not add any letters or sounds. Call attention to the incorrect example in (c) where the modifier has been given a plural form.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions: Argument Structural Uniformity and Event Structural Diversity
    SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTIONS: ARGUMENT STRUCTURAL UNIFORMITY AND EVENT STRUCTURAL DIVERSITY A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS AND THE COMMITTEE ON GRADUATE STUDIES OF STANFORD UNIVERSITY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Melanie Owens November 2011 © 2011 by Melanie Rachel Owens. All Rights Reserved. Re-distributed by Stanford University under license with the author. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution- Noncommercial 3.0 United States License. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/us/ This dissertation is online at: http://purl.stanford.edu/db406jt2949 ii I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Beth Levin, Primary Adviser I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Joan Bresnan I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Vera Gribanov Approved for the Stanford University Committee on Graduate Studies. Patricia J. Gumport, Vice Provost Graduate Education This signature page was generated electronically upon submission of this dissertation in electronic format. An original signed hard copy of the signature page is on file in University Archives. iii Abstract Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs) are constructions which contain two or more verbs yet behave in every grammatical respect as if they contain only one.
    [Show full text]
  • Structural Grammar* *เนื้อหาทั้งหมดของ Structural Grammar คัดลอกมาจากเ
    EFL-200: Analysis of the English Word and Sentence Structure / Page 1 Structural Grammar* *เนื้อหาทั้งหมดของ Structural Grammar คัดลอกมาจากเอกสารประกอบการสอนรายวิชา 2202326 วากยสัมพันธ์ภาษาอังกฤษ (English Syntax) (Chapter 1 Structural Grammar, หน้า 1-16) เรียบเรียงโดย ผศ.ดร.น ้าทิพย์ ภิงคารวัฒน์ และ ดร.รักสงบ วิจิตรโสภณ ภาควิชาภาษาอังกฤษ คณะอักษรศาสตร์ จุฬาลงกรณ์มหาวิทยาลัย In Structural Grammar, groups of words can be analyzed based on the relationship they hold. This can be represented in terms of structures and their major components. There are five main types of syntactic structures: 1. Structure of Modification: Head + Modifier M H H M H M Ex: large / house sing / sweetly the boy / who cried 2. Structure of Predication: Subject + Predicate S P S P S P Ex: they / left your brother / came yesterday It / is marvelous 3. Structure of Complementation: Verbal element + Complement VB C VB C VB C Ex: seems / a good place kept / it a secret give / him a call 4. Structure of Subordination: Subordination + Dependent Unit SUB D SUB D SUB D Ex: owing to / the delay in / the room after / they left 5. Structure of Coordination: 2 or more conjoins with a coordinator CN CO CN CO CN CO CN CN CN CO CN Ex: you / and / me neither / by bus / nor / by train read, / write / or / edit In the following sections, each of the five structures will be presented in terms of its major components, the principal pattern of order and a range of functions that it may hold. Structure of Modification A structure of modification consists of a head (H) and a modifier (M). The head carries the principal meaning while the modifier serves to describe, limit, intensify the meaning of the head.
    [Show full text]
  • Present As a Noun in a Sentence
    Present As A Noun In A Sentence anyIs Lovell hollands. tricyclic Interurban or priest-ridden Tammie when chaw drabble or Gnosticize some steersome peptonized ecclesiarch dankly? flip-flap, Ethelbert however remainslong-drawn unconscientious Barry excommunicating after Johnny glidingly syncs girlishlyor trauchling. or crinkling The noun as in present a sentence, it contains two people or an instrument is Email id below show a single sentence, sentence as a present noun in later in. Present Definition of mixture at Dictionarycom. Working was a group of four people five, could each pool member the custody of collecting one document each. One noun in present tense forms of nouns we presented at present? Not in sentence as nouns form of added at the reader to be presented itself from one that they function of the working together all the. The answer prove that row is the lens object enter the sentence. Time period still be past present income future formed with their VERB-ing. If they are you can i believe that you! Is there any other details about me that you would like to know? Sarah was afraid that she had provoked a wild goose chase. Effective Writing excellent Science at Scitable Nature. Compound Adjectives English Grammar Grammar CL. When two actions occur at the same time, and are done by the same person or thing, we can use a present participle to describe one of them. And the alternative evils are ambiguity and artificiality. What is verb example of subject noun in such sentence? Also called a noun adjunct. Share the roles in present participle modifies an action or edit sentences to suggest that? It warm the simplest, the sanest and it sounds most pleasant.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter Ii Review of Related Literature
    CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE This chapter discusses some theories related to the topics. It comprises the discussion of syntax, structure of modification, part of speech, phrase, genre, report, and previous study. A. Syntax Syntax is determining the relevant component part of sentence. It also means as describing these parts gramatically (Wekker, 1985:5). This definition of syntax implies that we start from what is regarded as the largest unit of syntactic description –the sentence- and proceed until we arrive at the smallest meaningful unit. This is called a ‘top to buttom’ analysis. The units smaller than the sentence will be referred to as clauses, phrases, words and morphemes respectively. However, instead of saying that a sentence can be broken down into smaller constituents, we might also look at the sentence the other way round –that is, ‘from bottom to top’ –and say that constituents at different levels can combine to form increasingly larger units: then proceed from the morpheme to the sentence as a whole. Constituents are like building blocks which pattern in certain ways to form larger units, and the largest unit being a sentence. In another word a phrase or sentence is always formed from the words which are organized by a certain rule. In English, the main device for showing this relationship is word order; for example, “The boy loves his dog” follows 9 10 standard subject-verb-object word order, and switching the order of such a sentence would change the meaning or make the sentence meaningless. If a word is combined in other words in English language and form of syntactic structure, there will be a wide range of structures with various groups that make up the structure of the word.
    [Show full text]