The Effects of Maltreatment on Neuroendocrine Regulation And

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Effects of Maltreatment on Neuroendocrine Regulation And City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Howe, M. L. and Toth, S. L. (2010). The Effects of Maltreatment and Neuroendocrine Regulation on Memory Performance. Child Development, 81(5), pp. 1504-1519. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01488.x This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/4209/ Link to published version: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01488.x Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] The Effects of Maltreatment and Neuroendocrine Regulation on Memory Performance Dante Cicchetti Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota Fred A. Rogosch Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester Mark L. Howe Lancaster University Sheree L. Toth Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester IN PRESS: Child Development Abstract This investigation examined basic memory processes, cortisol, and dissociation in maltreated children. School-aged children, 143 maltreated and 174 nonmaltreated, were administered the California Verbal Learning Test – Children (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1994) in a week-long camp setting, daily morning cortisol levels were assessed throughout the duration of camp, and behavioral symptoms were evaluated. Maltreatment and cortisol regulation were not related to short- or long-delay recall or recognition memory. However, children experiencing neglect/emotional maltreatment and low cortisol evinced heightened false recognition memory. Dissociative symptoms were higher in maltreated children; however, high dissociation was related to recognition inaccuracy only among nonmaltreated children. Results highlight the interplay between maltreatment and hypocortisolism in children’s recognition memory errors. Do maltreated children’s basic memory processes operate in ways that are fundamentally different from those of nonmaltreated children? Despite the importance that answers to this question hold for advancing research, clinical, and socio-legal issues in the field of child maltreatment (Howe, 2000; Howe, Goodman, & Cicchetti, 2008; Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006; Toth & Cicchetti, 1998), there has been a paucity of studies, most of which have been published in recent years, that have examined basic memory processes in maltreated children (Howe, Cicchetti, & Toth, 2006). Child Maltreatment and Memory There are a number of reasons why the experience of child maltreatment might be expected to alter basic memory processes (Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006). Child abuse and neglect are stressful and threatening experiences that may bring about physiological changes in the neural mechanisms related to memory storage (Bremner & Narayan, 1998; Cicchetti, Rogosch, Gunnar, & Toth, in press; Howe, Cicchetti, & Toth, 2006). Moreover, a number of the socioemotional sequelae of child maltreatment, including the development of insecure, often disorganized, attachment relationships and deviations in symbolic and representational abilities (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005), may affect the processing of memories in abused and neglected children (Lynch & Cicchetti, 1998; Pollak, Cicchetti, & Klorman, 1998; Rieder & Cicchetti, 1989). In addition, it is conceivable that failures in caregiving may lead maltreated children either to defensively distort memories or to dissociate their memories of their abusive and neglectful experiences (Cicchetti & Valentino, 2006). Finally, maltreated children are at heightened risk for the development of trauma-related psychopathology, including Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and dissociation, that may contribute to deficits in memory performance (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005; Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006). The majority of the investigations on the effects of maltreatment experiences on memory have focused on examinations of adults who retrospectively report having been abused during their childhoods, adults with PTSD, nonmaltreated children participating in analog experiments, or individuals with relatively acute and/or routine stressors involving treatment for accidental injuries or visits to physicians’ offices (Howe, 2000; Toth & Cicchetti, 1998). Among the studies of memory in adults who report having been maltreated, several investigations have used the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm (Deese, 1959; Roediger-McDermott, 1995) to examine false memories in adults who report having been maltreated in childhood and in adults who did not experience maltreatment (for a review, see Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006). Each of these investigations found that adults who report histories of maltreatment evinced a higher frequency of false recognition memory errors (i.e., inaccurately recognizing words not previously presented as heard) than did nonmaltreated comparison individuals. Despite the fact that there have been relatively few studies of basic memory processes in maltreated children conducted to date, a review of the extant literature reveals that the experience of child abuse and neglect does not appear to exert any special effects on memory that fundamentally change the operation of memory from that observed in nonmaltreated children (Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006). For example, no differences have been found in investigations of maltreated children’s memory for eyewitnessed events or in their degree of susceptibility to misinformation and suggestion, unless accompanying comorbid psychopathology is present (Eisen, Goodman, Qin, & Davis, 2002; Eisen, Goodman, Qin, Davis, & Crayton, 2007; Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006). In an investigation of true and false recall memory in maltreated children, Howe, Cicchetti, Toth, and Cerrito (2004) examined the memory performances of maltreated and nonmaltreated children on the DRM task. The same pattern of results was found regardless of maltreatment status. Consistent with the normative literature on false memory (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005), both maltreated and nonmaltreated children exhibited false recognition memories and memory inaccuracies with age at the same rate. Similarly, Porter, Lawson, and Bigler (2005) found no significant differences in memory function between sexually abused and nonabused children, despite elevations in psychopathology and diminished performance on measures of attention and executive function for the abused group. Additionally, Beers and DeBellis (2002) did not find any between group differences in basic memory processes for maltreated children and adolescents with PTSD and normative comparison youth. Overall, because these investigations focused on group differences, studies that examine within group differences might yield different results. Not all maltreated individuals are equally affected by their traumatic experiences (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). Thus, investigations of basic memory processes in children who have experienced sexual and physical abuse, neglect, and emotional maltreatment are warranted. Toward this end, a depth-of-processing incidental recall task was used to examine self- relevant information among maltreated children and the relation between maltreatment and false recall on dissociation (Valentino, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2008b). Abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated children all demonstrated better recall for words when words were encoded under self-referent conditions rather than under structural conditions. These results suggest that self- representations serve as an organizing schema for memory and may be used to facilitate recall (cf. Howe, 1998, 2000). Consistent with prior research (Howe et al., 2004), differences were not found among abused, neglected, or nonmaltreated children in the occurrence or total amount of false recall errors. Maltreated children were not rendered more or less susceptible to false recall errors (cf. Howe et al., 2004). Notably, however, the younger group of neglected children demonstrated a greater proportion of negative false recall errors and less positive recall than did the younger abused children, suggesting a greater vulnerability for memory inaccuracy among neglected children. Among the older group, both abused and neglected children demonstrated less positive false recall than did the nonmaltreated children. Negative self-schemas were associated with increased dissociation among the abused children, whereas positive self-schemas were related to increased dissociation for the neglected children. In addition to addressing the role of dissociation on memory, it also is important to attend to possible neurobiological influences. HPA Axis Functioning and Memory Child abuse and neglect are stressful and threatening experiences that create adaptational challenges (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001a; DeBellis, 2001; Hart, Gunnar, & Cicchetti, 1996). One of the physiological systems that has evolved in mammals to help direct
Recommended publications
  • Reproductions Supplied by EDRS Are the Best That Can Be Made from the Original Document
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 458 019 PS 029 868 AUTHOR Reid, Pamela Trotman, Ed. TITLE Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter, 2001. INSTITUTION Society for Research in Child Development. PUB DATE 2001-00-00 NOTE 62p.; Published quarterly. For the 2000 issues, see ED 450 943. AVAILABLE FROM Society,for Research in Child Development, University of Michigan, 505 East Huron, Suite 301, Ann Arbor, MI 48104-1567. Tel: 734-998-6578; Fax: 734-998-6569; e-mail: [email protected]; Web site: http://srcd.org. PUB TYPE Collected Works Serials (022) JOURNAL CIT Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter; v44 n1-4 2001 EDRS PRICE MF01/PC03 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Child Development; *Developmental Psychology; Information Dissemination; Newsletters; *Organizations (Groups); Program Descriptions IDENTIFIERS Foundation for Child Development; *Society for Research in Child Development ABSTRACT This document consists of the four 2001 issues of a newsletter disseminating information on the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) and providing a forum for important news, research, and .information concerning advancements in child growth and development research. Each issue of the newsletter includes announcements and notices of conferences, workshops, position openings, fellowships, and member obituaries. The first 2001 issue includes the following articles: (1) "A Student Speaks..."; and (2)"2001 Biennial Meeting." The second 2001 issue includes the following articles: (1) "Focus on Students and Internet Research"; (2) "Taking the Road
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Psychopathology: Concepts and Challenges
    Development and Psychopathology, 12 (2000), 265–296 Copyright 2000 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United States of America Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges a b MICHAEL RUTTER AND L. ALAN SROUFE aUniversity of London; and bUniversity of Minnesota Abstract The defining features of developmental psychopathology concepts include attention to the understanding of causal processes, appreciation of the role of developmental mechanisms, and consideration of continuities and discontinuities between normality and psychopathology. Accomplishments with respect to these issues are reviewed in relation to attachment disorders, antisocial behavior, autism, depressive disorder, schizophrenia, and intellectual development. Major research challenges remain in relation to measurement issues, comorbidity, gender differences, cognitive processing, nature–nurture interplay, heterotypic continuity, continuities between normal variations and disorders, developmental programming, and therapeutic mechanisms in effective treatments. Origins of a Developmental of the processes underlying both continuity Psychopathology Perspective and change in patterns. The focus was on the how of developmental processes, rather than Karl Popper (1972), the distinguished philos- on the charting of age-related progressions. opher of science, made the astute point that Many, perhaps most, causal processes were definitions need to be read from right to left, viewed in terms of chain reactions operating and not left to right. In other words, it makes
    [Show full text]
  • Paul Everett Meehl: the Cumulative Record ᮢ
    Paul Everett Meehl: The Cumulative Record ᮢ Niels G. Waller University of Minnesota–Twin Cities ᮢ Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University In this brief biographical sketch of Paul Meehl, the authors examine the “cumulative record” of his scholarship and mentorship. This record sheds light on why Meehl is widely regarded as one of the most influential clinical psychologists of the 20th century, as well as on Meehl’s remark- able intellectual life. Time has proven that Meehl’s writings are exceptional in their quality, influence, breadth, and depth. In addition, Meehl’s cumu- lative record raises important questions regarding the reinforcement contingencies in major research-oriented psychology departments. © 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Clin Psychol 61: 1209–1229, 2005. Keywords: Paul E. Meehl; clinical psychology; Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; clinical prediction; statistical prediction, constructs; construct validity; psychopathology; schizophrenia; taxometrics; statistical significance testing In 1996, the American Psychological Association (APA) celebrated its 100th anniversary and presented Centennial Awards to two psychologists who made “the greatest lifetime contribution to clinical psychology.” One of these two awards was given to Paul Everett Meehl, professor of clinical psychology, psychiatry, philosophy, and law at the Univer- sity of Minnesota. The second was given to Hans J. Eysenck. Sadly, both men would die within 10 years of receiving this prestigious honor. Eysenck passed away on September 4, 1997. Paul Meehl died on February 14, 2003. The authors wish to express their deep appreciation to Ms. Gwen Rekerdres for research assistance and to Drs. Leslie Yonce and David Lubinski for helpful comments on drafts of this article.
    [Show full text]
  • Srcd Oral History Interview
    Norman Garmezy Born 6/18/1918 in New York, NY; Died 11/21/2009 Spouse – Edith Linick Garmezy Ph.D. from University of Iowa (1950); M.A. from Columbia University (1940); B.A. from City College of New York (1939) Major Employment: Duke University –1950-1961, Psychology University of Minnesota – 1961-1989, Psychology Institute of Child Development at University of Minnesota – 1961-1989, Psychology Major Areas of Work: Clinical Psychology, Risk and Resilience, Schizophrenia SRCD Affiliation: Member SRCD ORAL HISTORY INTERVIEW Norman Garmezy Interviewed by Ann Masten 1995 Masten: Why don't you begin by talking about where you were born and your family and any experiences growing up that you think had an influence on you? Garmezy: Well, I was born on June 18, 1918, in a small apartment on the ground floor of a Bronx, New York tenement. My elementary school days were lived in this Bronx enclave and by enclave I mean it was an entirely Jewish community. It’s now very much a black neighborhood. At that time it was all tenement houses, and all the children I knew there were kids in these tenements whose parents had either come over to the U.S. at a very early age and had gone to a certain degree in elementary school or others who had come over later. I remember it as entirely Jewish; if a gentile showed up everybody paused and looked around. That's the way the Bronx was to a very great extent. I just remember that ours was a very small apartment. It had a “dumb” waiter in it, which was used to send garbage up or down.
    [Show full text]
  • Dante Cicchetti Curriculum Vitae
    12/29/2017 Dante Cicchetti Curriculum Vitae I. Current Information Licenses: Licensed Psychologist in Massachusetts and New York University Address: Institute of Child Development University of Minnesota 51 East River Road Minneapolis, MN 55455 Contact Information: [email protected] Google Scholar h-index: 136 Google Scholar Citations: 74,114 II. Education B.S., University of Pittsburgh. Ph.D., University of Minnesota (Clinical Psychology and Child Development; Minors in Behavior Genetics, Neuroscience, and Psychophysiology), Department of Psychology and Institute of Child Development, Advisors: Paul E. Meehl and L. Alan Sroufe. III. Employment History January 2013- Present Director of Research, Institute of Translational Research for Children’s Mental Health University of Minnesota September 2010-Present McKnight Presidential Endowed Chair and William Harris Endowed Professor and Professor of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Institute of Child Development and Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota July 2005- Present McKnight Presidential Endowed Chair of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Professor, Institute of Child Development and Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota July 2005- Present Adjunct Professor in Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota July, 2000-June 2005 Shirley Cox Kearns Professor of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Pediatrics, University of Rochester July, 1995-2005 Professor of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, Psychiatry, and Pediatrics, University of Rochester. July, 1994-2005 Professor of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Pediatrics, University of Rochester. Dec., 1987-July, 1994 Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Rochester. July, 1985-Dec., 1987 Associate Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Rochester. July, 1985-June 2005 Director, Mt. Hope Family Center July, 1982-July, 1985 Norman Tishman Associate Professor of Psychology, Harvard University.
    [Show full text]
  • Normalizing the Development of Cortisol Regulation in Maltreated Infants Through Preventive Interventions
    Development and Psychopathology 23 (2011), 789–800 # Cambridge University Press 2011 doi:10.1017/S0954579411000307 Normalizing the development of cortisol regulation in maltreated infants through preventive interventions a,b b b c DANTE CICCHETTI, FRED A. ROGOSCH, SHEREE L. TOTH, AND MELISSA L. STURGE-APPLE aUniversity of Minnesota; bMt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester; and cUniversity of Rochester Abstract Longitudinal effects of child maltreatment on cortisol regulation in infants from age 1 to 3 years were investigated in the context of a randomized preventive intervention trial. Thirteen-month-old infants from maltreating families (N ¼ 91) and their mothers were randomly assigned to one of three intervention conditions: child–parent psychotherapy, psychoeducational parenting intervention, and a control group involving standard community services (CS). A fourth group of infants from nonmaltreating families (N ¼ 52) and their mothers comprised a nonmaltreated comparison (NC) group. The two active interventions were combined into one maltreated intervention (MI) group for statistical analyses. Saliva samples were obtained from children at 10:00 a.m. before beginning a laboratory observation session with their mothers when the children were 13 months of age (preintervention), 19 months (midintervention), 26 months (postintervention), and 38 months (1-year postintervention follow-up). At the initial assessment, no significant differences among groups in morning cortisol were observed. Latent growth curve analyses examined trajectories of cortisol regulation over time. Beginning at midintervention, divergence was found among the groups. Whereas the MI group remained indistinguishable from the NC group across time, the CS group progressively evinced lower levels of morning cortisol, statistically differing from the MI and NC groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Fostering Secure Attachment in Infants in Maltreating Families Through Preventive Interventions
    Development and Psychopathology 18 ~2006!, 623–649 Copyright © 2006 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.10170S0954579406060329 Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions DANTE CICCHETTI,a FRED A. ROGOSCH,b and SHEREE L. TOTHb aInstitute of Child Development, University of Minnesota; and bMt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester Abstract The malleability of insecure and disorganized attachment among infants from maltreating families was investigated through a randomized preventive intervention trial. Findings from research on the effects of maltreatment on infant attachment were incorporated into the design and evaluation of the intervention. One-year-old infants from maltreating families ~N ϭ 137! and their mothers were randomly assigned to one of three intervention conditions: ~a! infant–parent psychotherapy ~IPP!, ~b! psychoeducational parenting intervention ~PPI!, and ~c! community standard ~CS! controls. A fourth group of infants from nonmaltreating families ~N ϭ 52! and their mothers served as an additional low-income normative comparison ~NC! group. At baseline, mothers in the maltreatment group, relative to the nonmaltreatment group mothers, reported greater abuse and neglect in their own childhoods, more insecure relationships with their own mothers, more maladaptive parenting attitudes, more parenting stress, and lower family support, and they were observed to evince lower maternal sensitivity. Infants in the maltreatment groups had significantly higher rates of disorganized attachment than infants in the NC group. At postintervention follow-up at age 26 months, children in the IPP and PPI groups demonstrated substantial increases in secure attachment, whereas increases in secure attachment were not found for the CS and NC groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Ph.D. in Child Psychology
    Ph.D. in child psychology Join a close-knit group of faculty, staff, and peers who are driven to make a difference and who will support you as you grow as an academic and researcher. For more than 90 years, we’ve worked hand-in-hand to make discoveries that increase knowledge about human development and improve lives. In our program, you’ll not only study what you love, but also develop the skills needed to teach others and translate science into practical solutions for children, youth, and families. Program highlights • Study with award-winning faculty at the #1 developmental psychology program in the nation (U.S. News & World Report, 2018). • Choose to pursue one of two tracks: developmental science or developmental psychopathology and clinical science. • Earn your degree at a top public research university located in an urban center, offering diverse opportunities for interdisciplinary and community-based research. • Receive guaranteed funding for five academic years, including a monthly stipend, health insurance, and a full tuition waiver. • More than 50% of our students receive competitive fellowships from organizations like the National Science Foundation. Choose from two tracks Developmental science Developmental psychopathology & clinical science Estimated completion time: 5 years Estimated completion time: 6 years • Study human development across the lifespan and • Pursue a joint track in collaboration with the prepare for a career in academics or research. Department of Psychology focused on the study of psychopathology in the context of development. • Conduct research in areas including cognitive development, language, learning, executive function, • Take developmental psychology courses with your social development, or developmental psychobiology cohort at the Institute of Child Development, as and neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • Ddeeevvveeelllooopppm
    Division 7 Newsletter-Summer 2008 DDDEEEVVVEEELLLOOOPPPMMMEEENNNTTTAAALLL PPPSSSYYYCCCHHHOOOLLLOOOGGGIIISSSTTT Summer 2008 Table of Contents PRESIDENT’S COLUMN – LAURENCE STEINBERG ......................................................................................... 1 DIVISION 7 ANNOUNCEMENTS DIVISION 7 AWARD RECIPIENTS ................................................................................................................. 4 INTRODUCING DIVISION 7/DIVISION 20 DIALOGUES ..................................................................................... 6 CHECK OUT OUR WEBPAGE ....................................................................................................................... 6 DIALOGUE BETWEEN DIVISION 7 AND DIVISION 20 ........................................................................................... 7 TEACHERS DESCRIBE THEIR TRAINING NEEDS ................................................................................................. 11 SCIENCE POLICY AND FUNDING NEWS .......................................................................................................... 13 OTHER ANNOUNCEMENTS .......................................................................................................................... 14 UPCOMING CONFERENCES .......................................................................................................................... 17 DIVISION 7 EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE ............................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Chlld Maltreatment Theory and Research on the Causes and Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect
    Chlld maltreatment Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect Edited by DANTE CICCHETTI University ofRochester and VICKI CARLSON Washington University, St. Louis Th~ r/gltt of lh~ tlniunity of Combrlrlg~ (a print onJ ull (J//l1fI1ftMFojOOcb wu,t gl'tlfl/rdby HN1ty Villi's 1511. Tht: Unlm-1l1y Iras prln/rJ find publisMJ conlfnilOwsly nnu 1581. CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS Cambridge New York Port Chester Melbourne Sydney vi Contents Contents vii 9 Perceived similarities and disagreements about childrearing 21 Troubled youth, troubled families: the dynamics of adolescent practices in abusive and nonabusive families: maltreatment 685 intergenerational and concurrent family processes 280 James Garbarino Penelope K. Trickett and Elizabeth J. Susman 22 Child abuse, delinquency, and violent criminality 707 10 Cognitive foundations for parental care 302 Dorothy Otnow Lewis, Catherine Mallouh, and Victoria Webb Carolyn Moore Newberger and Kathleen M. White 23 The prevention of maltreatment 722 II Intergenerational continuities and discontinuities in serious David L. Olds and Charles R. Henderson, Jr. parenting difficulties 317 Name index 764 Michael Rutter 781 12 The construct of empathy and the phenomenon of physical Subject index maltreatment of children 349 Norma Deitch Feshbach Part III The developmental consequences of child maltreatment 13 How research on child maltreatment has informed the study of child development: perspectives from developmental psychopathology 377 Dante Cicchetti 14 Child maltreatment and attachment theory 432 Patricia M. Crittenden and Mary D. S. Ainsworth 15 Patterns of maternal behavior among infants at risk for abuse: relations with infant attachment behavior and infant development at 12 months of age 464 Karlen Lyons-Ruth, David B.
    [Show full text]
  • Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter, 2002. INSTITUTION Society for Research in Child Development
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 480 655 PS 031 538 AUTHOR Reid, Pamela Trotman, Ed.; Tucker, Thelma, Ed. TITLE Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter, 2002. INSTITUTION Society for Research in Child Development. PUB DATE 2002-00-00 NOTE 59p.; Published quarterly. For the 2001 issues, see ED 458 019. AVAILABLE FROM Society for Research in Child Development, University of Michigan, 3131 South State Street, Suite 302, Ann Arbor, MI 48108-1623. Tel: 734-998-6578; Fax: 734-998-6569; e-mail: [email protected]; Web site: http://srcd.org. PUB TYPE Collected Works Serials (022) JOURNAL CIT Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter; v45 nl- 4 2002 EDRS PRICE EDRS Price MF01/PC03 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Child Care; *Child Development; *Developmental Psychology; Information Dissemination; Newsletters; *Organizations (Groups); Program Descriptions; Psychological Studies; World Wide Web IDENTIFIERS Institutional History; *Society for Research in Child Development ABSTRACT This document consists of the four 2002 issues of a newsletter disseminating information on the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) and providing a forum for important news, research, and information concerning advancement in child growth and development research. Each issue of the newsletter includes announcements and notices of conferences, workshops, position openings, fellowship, and member obituaries. The January issue includes the following articles: (1) "Pioneering in Science and Application," on the history of the Institute of Child Development at the
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Psychopathology: Volume
    Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti ffirs.tex V2 - Volume I - 12/10/2015 12:39pm Page ii Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti ffirs.tex V2 - Volume I - 12/10/2015 12:39pm Page i DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti ffirs.tex V2 - Volume I - 12/10/2015 12:39pm Page ii Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti ffirs.tex V2 - Volume I - 12/10/2015 12:39pm Page iii DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY THIRD EDITION Volume One: Theory and Method Editor DANTE CICCHETTI Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti ffirs.tex V2 - Volume I - 12/10/2015 12:39pm Page iv This book is printed on acid-free paper. Copyright © 2016 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008.
    [Show full text]