Catching Words in a Stream of Speech

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Load more

Catching Words in a Stream of Speech:

Computational Simulations of
Segmenting Transcribed Child-Directed Speech

  • ˘
  • Çagrı Çöltekin

CLCG

The work presented here was carried out under the auspices of the School of Behavioural and

Cognitive Neuroscience and the Center for Language and Cognition Groningen of the Faculty of

Arts of the University of Groningen.

Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 97 ISSN 0928-0030

2011, Çag˘rı Çöltekin

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 3.0 License. To view a

copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/

or send a letter to Creative Commons, 444 Castro Street, Suite 900, Mountain View, California, 94041, USA.

Cover art, titled auto, by Franek Timur Çöltekin

A

Document prepared with LT X 2 and typeset by pdfT X

ε

  • E
  • E

Printed by Wöhrmann Print Service, Zutphen
RIJKSUNIVERSITEIT GRONINGEN

Catching Words in a Stream of Speech

Computational Simulations of Segmenting Transcribed Child-Directed Speech

Proefschrift ter verkrijging van het doctoraat in de
Letteren aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen op gezag van de
Rector Magnificus, dr. E. Sterken, in het openbaar te verdedigen op donderdag 8 december 2011 om 14.30 uur

door

Çag˘rı Çöltekin

geboren op 28 februari 1972
Çıldır, Turkije

  • Promotor:
  • Prof. dr. ir. J. Nerbonne

Beoordelingscommissie: Prof. dr. A. van den Bosch
Prof. dr. P. Hendriks Prof. dr. P. Monaghan

ISBN (electronic version): 978-90-367-5259-6

  • ISBN (print version):
  • 978-90-367-5232-9

Preface

I started my PhD project with a more ambitious goal than what might have been achieved in this dissertation. I wanted to touch most issues of language acquisition, developing computational models for a wider range of phenomena. In particular, I

wanted to focus on models of learning linguistic ‘structure’, as it is typically observed

in morphology and syntax. As a result, segmentation was one of the annoying tasks that

I could not easily step over because I was also interested in morphology. So, I decided

to write a chapter on segmentation. Despite the fact that segmentation is considered

relatively easy (in comparison to learning syntax, for example) by many people, and

it is studied relatively well, every step I took for modeling this task revealed another

interesting problem I could not just gloss over. At the end, the initial ‘chapter’ became

the dissertation you have in front of you. I believe I have a far better understanding of

the problem now, but I also have many more questions than what I started with.

The structure of the project, and my wanderings in the landscape of language acquisition did not allow me to work with many other people. As a result, this dissertation has been completed in a more independent setting than most other PhD dissertations. Nevertheless, this thesis benefited from my interactions with others. I

will try to acknowledge the direct or indirect help I had during this work, but it is likely

that I will fail to mention all. I apologize in advance to anyone whom I might have

unintentionally left out.

First of all, my sincere thanks goes to my supervisor John Nerbonne. Here, I do not use the word sincere for stylistic reasons. All PhD students acknowledge the

supervisor(s), but if you think they all mean it, you probably have not talked to many

of them. Besides the valuable comments on the content of my work, I got the attention

and encouragement I needed, when I needed it. He patiently read all my early drafts on

short notice, even correcting my never-ending English mistakes.

I would also like to thank Antal van den Bosch, Petra Hendriks and Padraic

Monaghan for agreeing to read and evaluate my thesis. Their comments and criticisms

improved the final draft, and made me look at the issues discussed in the thesis from

different perspectives. In earlier stages of my PhD project, I also received valuable comments and criticisms from Kees de Bot and Tamás Biró. Although focus of the project changed substantially, the benefit of their comments remain. Later, regular

discussions with fellow PhD students Barbara Plank, Dörte Hessler and Peter Nabende

vvi

kept me on track, and I particularly got valuable comments from Barbara on some of

the content presented here. Barbara and Dörte also get additional thanks for agreeing

to be my paranimphs during the defense.

A substantial part of completing a PhD requires writing. Writing at a reasonable

academic level is a difficult task, writing in a foreign language is even more difficult and

writing in a language in which you barely understand the basics is almost impossible.

First, thanks to Daniël de Kok for helping me with the impossible, and translating the

summary to Dutch. Second, I shamelessly used some people close to me for proof reading, on short notice, without much display of appreciation. My many mistakes

in earlier drafts of this dissertation were eliminated by the help of Joanna Krawczyk-

Çöltekin, Arzu Çöltekin, Barbara Plank and Asena and Giray Devlet. I am grateful for

their help, as well as their friendship.

My interest in language and language acquisition that lead to this rather late PhD

goes back to my undergraduate years in Middle East Technical University. I am likely

to omit many people here because of many years past since. However, the help I got

and things that I learned from Cem Bozs¸ahin and Deniz Zeyrek are difficult to forget. I

am particularly grateful to Cem Bozs¸ahin for his encouragement and his patience in

supervising my many MSc thesis attempts.

This thesis also owes a lot to people that I cannot all name here. I would like to

thank to those who share their data, their code, and their wisdom. This thesis would not be possible without many freely available sources of information, tools and data that we

A

take for granted nowadays—just to name a few: GNU utilities, R, LT X, CHILDES.
E

There is more to life than research, and you realize it more if you move to a new

city. Many people made the life in Groningen more pleasant during my PhD time here. First, I feel fortunate to be in Alfa-informatica. Besides my colleagues in the

department, people of foreign guest club and international service desk of the university made my life more pleasant and less painful. I am reluctant to name people individually because of certainty of omissions. Nevertheless here is an incomplete list of people that

I feel lucky to have met during this time, sorted randomly: Ellen and John Nerbonne,

Kostadin Cholakov, Laura Fahnenbruck, Dörte Hessler, Ildikó Berzlánovich, Gisi

Cannizzaro, Tim Van de Cruys, Daniël de Kok, Martijn Wieling, Jelena Prokic´, Aysa

Arylova, Barbara Plank, Martin Meraner, Gideon Kotzé, Zhenya Markovskaya, Radek

Šimík, Jörg Tiedemann, Tal Caspi, Jelle Wouda.

My parents, Hos¸naz and Selçuk Çöltekin, have always been supportive, but also encouraged my curiosity from the very start. My interest in linguistics likely goes back to a description of Turkish morphology among my father’s notes. I still pursue

the same fascination I felt when I realized there was a neat explanation to something

I knew intuitively. My sister, Arzu, has always been there for me, not only as a supportive family member, but I also benefited a lot from our discussions about my work, sometimes making me feel that she should have been doing what I do. Lastly, many thanks to two people who suffered most from my work on the thesis by being

closest to me, Aska and Franek, for their help, support and patience.

Contents

  • List of Abbreviations
  • xi

  • 1
  • 1

2
Introduction

  • The Problem of Language Acquisition
  • 5

2.1 The nature–nurture debate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
2.1.1 Difficulty of learning languages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
68
2.1.2 How quick is quick enough? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 2.1.3 Critical periods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 2.1.4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12
2.2 Theories of language acquisition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
2.2.1 Parametric theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 2.2.2 Connectionist models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 2.2.3 Usage based theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 2.2.4 Statistical models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
2.3 Summary and discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20

  • 3
  • Formal Models
  • 23

3.1 Formal models in science . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 3.2 Computational learning theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
3.2.1 Chomsky hierarchy of languages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 3.2.2 Identification in the limit . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 3.2.3 Probably approximately correct learning . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
3.3 Learning theory and language acquisition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
3.3.1 The class of natural languages and learnability . . . . . . . . 33 3.3.2 The input to the language learner . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 3.3.3 Are natural languages provably (un)learnable? . . . . . . . . 37
3.4 What do computational models explain? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 3.5 Computational simulations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 3.6 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

vii viii

Contents

  • 4
  • Lexical Segmentation: An Introduction
  • 45

4.1 Word recognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 4.2 Lexical segmentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49
4.2.1 Predictability and distributional regularities . . . . . . . . . . 50 4.2.2 Prosodic cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 4.2.3 Phonotactics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 4.2.4 Pauses and utterance boundaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 4.2.5 Words that occur in isolation and short utterances . . . . . . . 55 4.2.6 Other cues for segmentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56
4.3 Segmentation cues: combination and comparison . . . . . . . . . . . 57

  • 5
  • Computational Models of Segmentation
  • 59

5.1 The input . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 5.2 Processing strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
5.2.1 Guessing boundaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 5.2.2 Recognition strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
5.3 The search space . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67
5.3.1 Exact search using dynamic programming . . . . . . . . . . . 68 5.3.2 Approximate search procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 5.3.3 Search for boundaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
5.4 Performance and evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 5.5 Two reference models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76
5.5.1 A random segmentation model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 5.5.2 A reference model using language-modeling strategy . . . . . 76
5.6 Summary and discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82

  • 6
  • Segmentation using Predictability Statistics
  • 85

6.1 Measures of predictability for segmentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86
6.1.1 Boundary probability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 6.1.2 Transitional probability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 6.1.3 Pointwise mutual information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 6.1.4 Successor variety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 6.1.5 Boundary entropy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93 6.1.6 Effects of phoneme context . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 6.1.7 Predicting the past . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 6.1.8 Predictability measures: summary and discussion . . . . . . . 100
6.2 A predictability based segmentation model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102
6.2.1 Peaks in unpredictability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 6.2.2 Combining multiple measures and varying phoneme context . 106 6.2.3 Weighing the competence of the voters . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 6.2.4 Two sides of a peak . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 6.2.5 Reducing redundancy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112

Contents

ix

6.3 Summary and discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113

Learning from Utterance Boundaries 115

7.1 Related work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 116 7.2 Do utterance boundaries provide cues for word boundaries? . . . . . . 117 7.3 An unsupervised learner using utterance boundaries . . . . . . . . . . 119 7.4 Combining measures and cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 7.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124

7

  • 8
  • Learning From Past Decisions
  • 127

8.1 Using previously discovered words . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128
8.1.1 What makes a good word? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 8.1.2 Related work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
8.2 Using known words for segmentation: a computational model . . . . 132
8.2.1 The computational model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 8.2.2 Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134
8.3 Making use of lexical stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136
8.3.1 Related work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 8.3.2 An analysis of stress patterns in the BR corpus . . . . . . . . 139 8.3.3 Segmentation using lexical stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
8.4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143

  • 9
  • An Overview of the Segmentation Strategies
  • 147

9.1 The measures and the models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 9.2 Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 9.3 Making use of prior information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 9.4 Variation in the input . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 9.5 Qualitative evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157 9.6 The learning method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 161 9.7 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164

  • 10 Conclusions
  • 165

169 187 191 193 197
Bibliography Short Summary Samenvatting in het Nederlands A The Corpora B Detailed Performance Results

List of Abbreviations

List of Abbreviations
ANN APS
Artificial Neural Network Argument from poverty of stimulus
17 35

BF BP BR BTP
Boundary F-score Boundary precision Boundary recall
73 73 73

  • 99
  • Backwards or reverse TP

CDS CHILDES
Child Directed Speech Child language data exchange system
61 46

  • H
  • Boundary entropy
  • 94

IIL

Identification in the limit also used as ‘Identifiable 27

in the limit’
LAD LF LP
Language Acquisition Device Lexical (or word-type) f-score Lexical (or word-type) precision Lexical (or word-type) recall
673 73

  • 73
  • LR

MDL MI
Minimum description length (Pointwise) Mutual Information
65 90

PAC PM PUM
Probably approximately correct (learning) Predictability-based Segmentation model

Segmentation model that uses predictability and ut- 123

terance boundaries
29 113

PUWM

Segmentation model that uses predictabilty, utter- 135

ance boundaries and know words

xi xii

List of Abbreviations

PUWSM PV

Segmentation model that uses all cues: predictabilty, 143

utterance boundaries, known words and stress

  • Predecessor Value
  • 99

  • 76
  • RM
  • Random Model

SM SRN SV
Stress-based Segmentation model Simple recurrent network Successor Variety
142 18, 63 91

  • TP
  • Transitional probability
  • 88

UG UM
Universal Grammar Utterance boundary-based Segmentation model
6123

  • VC dimension Vapnik-Chervonenkis dimension
  • 30

WF WM WP WR
Word F-score Lexicon-based Segmentation model Word precision
73 132 73

  • Word recall
  • 73

1 Introduction

A witty saying proves nothing.
Voltaire

The aim of language acquisition research is to understand how children learn languages spoken in their environment. This study contributes to this purpose by

investigating one of the first steps of the language acquisition process, the discovery of

words in the speech stream directed to children, by means of computational simulations.

We take words for granted, we identify them effortlessly when listening to others

speaking a language we understand, and we use them to construct utterances possibly

never uttered before. We learn the sound forms of the words, associate them with

meanings, discover how to use them appropriately in company of other words, and in

presence of different people. Despite apparent ease with which we process and learn

words, learning a proper set of words, a lexicon, to effectively communicate with our

environment is a challenging task. The challenge starts with identifying these words in

a continuous speech stream. Unlike written text where we typically put white spaces

between the words, the speech signal does not contain analogous reliable markers for

word boundaries.

A competent language user is aided, to some extent, by his/her knowledge of

words to extract them from a continuous stream: itisannoyingbutyouprobablycanfigure-

outthewordsinthissequence. However, at the beginning of their journey to becoming

competent speakers, children do not know the words in the language they are acquir-

ing. As a result they cannot make use of words. If this is not convincing, try to

locate the word boundaries in this sentence: eg˘ertürkçebilmiyorsanızbudizidekisözcük-

leribulmanızçokzor. This is approximately what happens when you hear an unfamiliar

language (in this case, Turkish). Without knowing the words of the input language,

discovering words in a continuous speech stream does not seem possible, which leads

to a chicken-and-egg problem. In spoken language, we are not as helpless as in the

written stream of letters. There are several acoustic cues that indicate word boundaries.

However, although these cues correlate with the boundaries, they are known to be

insufficient, noisy and sometimes in conflict with each other. Furthermore, the cues are

language dependent, that is, one needs to know the boundaries to learn when and how

1
2

Introduction

these cues correlate with the boundaries. We are back to the chicken-and-egg problem

again.

Fortunately, there are also some simple and general segmentation strategies that seem to work universally for all languages. A byproduct of the fact that the natural speech stream is formed by concatenating words, the flow of basic units (such as

syllables or phonemes) in an utterance follows certain statistical regularities. Particu-

larly, the basic units within words predict one another in sequence, while units across

boundaries do not. It is even more encouraging that children seem to be sensitive to

these statistics at a very young age. Another source of information for word boundaries

that does not require knowledge of words in advance comes from utterance bound-

aries. Utterance boundaries are also word boundaries, and words are formed by certain

regularities, for example they share common beginnings and endings. This provides

another source for discovering words before knowing them. Once we start discovering

words using these general strategies, we can also learn to use the language-specific

Recommended publications
  • Downloaded by [New York University] at 06:54 14 August 2016 Classic Case Studies in Psychology

    Downloaded by [New York University] at 06:54 14 August 2016 Classic Case Studies in Psychology

    Downloaded by [New York University] at 06:54 14 August 2016 Classic Case Studies in Psychology The human mind is both extraordinary and compelling. But this is more than a collection of case studies; it is a selection of stories that illustrate some of the most extreme forms of human behaviour. From the leader who convinced his followers to kill themselves to the man who lost his memory; from the boy who was brought up as a girl to the woman with several personalities, Geoff Rolls illustrates some of the most fundamental tenets of psychology. Each case study has provided invaluable insights for scholars and researchers, and amazed the public at large. Several have been the inspiration for works of fiction, for example the story of Kim Peek, the real Rain Man. This new edition features three new case studies, including the story of Charles Decker who was tried for the attempted murder of two people but acquitted on the basis of a neurological condition, and Dorothy Martin, whose persisting belief in an impending alien invasion is an illuminating example of cognitive dissonance. In addition, each case study is contextualized with more typical behaviour, while the latest thinking in each sub-field is also discussed. Classic Case Studies in Psychology is accessibly written and requires no prior knowledge of psychology, but simply an interest in the human condition. It is a book that will amaze, sometimes disturb, but above all enlighten its readers. Downloaded by [New York University] at 06:54 14 August 2016 Geoff Rolls is Head of Psychology at Peter Symonds College in Winchester and formerly a Research Fellow at Southampton University, UK.
  • The Archidoxes of Magic, 2010, Theophrastus Paracelsus, 1163581216, 9781163581216, Kessinger Publishing, 2010

    The Archidoxes of Magic, 2010, Theophrastus Paracelsus, 1163581216, 9781163581216, Kessinger Publishing, 2010

    The Archidoxes of Magic, 2010, Theophrastus Paracelsus, 1163581216, 9781163581216, Kessinger Publishing, 2010 DOWNLOAD http://bit.ly/1x1etg0 http://goo.gl/RN6Tr http://www.powells.com/s?kw=The+Archidoxes+of+Magic Of the Supreme Mysteries of Nature; of the Spirits of Planets; Secrets of Alchemy; Occult Philosophy; Signs of the Zodiack, Magical Cure of Diseases; and Celestial Medicines; Partial Contents: Of Simple Fire; Multiplicity of Fire; The Metals of the Planets; Spirit of the Sun; Moon, Venus, Mars, Jupiter, Saturn; Of Tinctures how they are made; Conjunction of Male and Female; To make the Furnace; To place the Fire; The Red Colour; Of Consecrations; Of Ceremonies Magical; Of Conjurations; Supernatural Diseases must have Supernatural Cures; Visions and Dreams; Dreams natural and Supernatural; Of Imagination; Of Hidden Treasure; The Abuse of Magick; Preservatives against Witchcraft; Manner of helping persons bewitched; Of the mystery of the twelve Signs; Celestial Medicines. DOWNLOAD http://bit.ly/1jMAVSQ http://avaxsearch.com/?q=The+Archidoxes+of+Magic http://bit.ly/1lmpSnE , , , , . The Strangest Animals in the World , S. Talalaj, May 1, 1994, Animals, 221 pages. Describes the habits and characteristics of many peculiar animals. Also outlines mysteries and myths associated with certain animals such as crocodiles, rats and baboonsBetter How Jesus Satisfies the Search for Meaning, Tim Chaddick, Craig Borlase, Sep 1, 2013, Religion, 240 pages. “What does it take to find satisfaction? Will I ever find something in life that’s better than this?” Most people live a life they never would have planned. The good news is The 'Language Instinct' Debate Revised Edition, Geoffrey Sampson, Apr 1, 2005, Language Arts & Disciplines, 224 pages.
  • SAVING LITERACY SAVING Dr

    SAVING LITERACY SAVING Dr

    SAVING LITERACY Dr. Susan Rich Sheridan is a scholar/teacher with degrees in English, Art and Education. Her Neuroconstructive, brain-based theory and marks- based practice of literacy have a twenty-year history, including teaching first grade through college. Saving Literacy introduces the Scribbling/Drawing/ Writing program for children 10 months to 6 years, including developmental SAVING LITERACY benchmarks, lesson plans, evaluation tools, and research questions designed for professional caregivers: preschool and daycare providers, elementary school teachers, child psychologists, art teachers and art therapists, speech How Marks Change pathologists and researchers in child development and education. The goals of the program are sustained attention, emotional control and connection, expanded speech and literacy. Autism and the effects of technology are Minds discussed. Dr. Sheridan has published a companion book for parents, HandMade Marks. Attention “What we most need now… is a fresh perspective on the masses of data that neurobiologists have gathered, and on the puzzles those data pose… How do brains make sense of the world (?)… (A) new general theory… requires new assumptions and new definitions. I believe that the idea of meaning, a critical concept that defines the relations of each brain to the world, is central to current debates in philosophy and cognitive science, and will Connection become so in neurobiology… Doodling can and should accompany if not even precede speaking. Language derives from the dynamics and structure of intentional behavior... and the face and hand areas of the cortex lie side by side, undergoing the same patterns of neural development. They are inextricably linked, as we know from the necessity of Literacy moving our hands and fingers as we speak to communicate meaning most effectively.” Correspondence between Dr.
  • Chapter Five Considering Linguistic Knowledge

    Chapter Five Considering Linguistic Knowledge

    Chapter Five Chapter Five Considering Linguistic Knowledge At the end of the previous chapter, we noted that there may be principles underlying the acquisition of the lexicon that are not obvious at first glance. But isn’t that always the case in science? Who would have thought at the beginning of this century that general principles of physics govern the very way that all materials, living or otherwise, are constructed, or that principles of neurobiology control the very makeup of all living matter? From that perspective, it is perhaps not surprising that some rather non-obvious principles govern something as ubiquitous as the human’s implicit knowledge of language. This chapter is a pause of sorts from our usual head-long rush into technical detail, for here we cover some basic concepts and a few insights from language acquisition. These are important— indeed, have been important all along (except that we didn’t have time to bring them up)— because, as we shall see, for some linguists, they guide the very nature of linguistic theorizing, the general purpose of which is to ascertain what one’s implicit knowledge of language looks like in the first place. 1. Basic Concepts 1.1 Implicit knowledge of language All along in the course, we’ve been making reference to the native speaker’s knowledge, where with “native speaker” we mean any native speaker, not just one who happens to have had a lot of schooling, or one who can even read and write. But let’s be more precise now. When we speak of the native speaker’s implicit knowledge of language, we refer in linguistics to knowledge that many speakers may not even be aware of at all (hence implicit).
  • SCHOOL of EDUCATION, HEALTH and SCIENCE a Thesis Submitted

    SCHOOL of EDUCATION, HEALTH and SCIENCE a Thesis Submitted

    SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, HEALTH AND SCIENCE PROGRAMME CODE: PX3AA PROGRAMME TITLE: DOCTOR of EDUCATION (EdD) MODULE CODE: 8EU007 MODULE TITLE: Work Based Project (Independent Phase) A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education (EdD) award of the University of Derby . A case study of a school in Zimbabwe: Investigating challenges faced by rural O-level students and strategies used by teachers in the English Reading- comprehension classes. By: MAXWELL OBEDIAH GUNDUZA KANYOKA: B.Phil. M.Ed. ID NUMBER: 100087076 May: 2018 i Sensitivity: Internal ACKNOWLEDGEMENT My Director of studies, the late Dr Michael Flay, for his positive advice and never- ending constructive support in guiding me through the different stages of the research, and for his perceptive advice from whom I have learnt so much that will assist my own- going career; Then after his death his replacement, my programme co-ordinator Dr Neil Radford and first supervisor for his support and advice, my second supervisor Dr Codra Spencer for her unending zeal to help me include whatever she thought useful, advised and suggested as important to be included in the study; and my friend’s Dr Washington Karumazondo, Dr Kordwick Ndebele and Mr Onisimo Takaingofa for their unwavering support; The Ministry of Education Sport and Culture in Zimbabwe, which accommodated my study in one secondary school in Zimbabwe; The headmaster of the school where I carried out this study, the HoD and the department of English personnel who generously cooperated
  • ALTERNATIVE (RE)VIEWS the Language Myth

    ALTERNATIVE (RE)VIEWS the Language Myth

    ALTERNATIVE (RE)VIEWS Editors’ note: We invited six commentaries on Vyvyan Evans’s book The language myth from scholars with differing perspectives. The reviewers were free to choose what they focused on; that is, they were not asked to provide a complete review of the contents of the book. The reviews are listed in alphabetical order by the names of the authors. [Gregory N. Carlson and Helen Goodluck] The language myth: Why language is not an instinct. By V yvyan Evans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2014. Pp. xi, 304. ISBN 9781107619753. $29.99. Beyond caricatures: Commentary on Evans 2014 Farrell Ackerman Robert Malouf University of California, San Diego San Diego State University ‘In order to understand what another person is saying you must assume it is true, and try to imagine what it might be true of.’—George Miller, quoted by Ross (1982:8) As will be familiar to a Language audience, many of the central questions of language study were identified by luminaries of language—Wilhelm von Humboldt, Hermann Paul, Ferdinand de Saussure, Mikołaj Kruszewski, William Dwight Whitney, Edward Sapir, Leonard Bloomfield, John R. Firth, Roman Jakobson, and Charles Hockett, among others—who preceded both main - stream generative grammar (MGG) and modern cognitive/functional approaches. 1 These earlier generations of linguists were both awed and challenged by the intriguing properties of human languages. How can they be understood as biological, physiological, cognitive, and/or social ob - jects? What is the relation between the synchronic
  • True Stories of Childhood Misfortune and Meaningless Sex

    True Stories of Childhood Misfortune and Meaningless Sex

    TANGERINE VAGINA : TRUE STORIES OF CHILDHOOD MISFORTUNE AND MEANINGLESS SEX Author: Anjum Choudhury Number of Pages: 84 pages Published Date: 30 Jul 2020 Publisher: Bowker Identifier Services Publication Country: none Language: English ISBN: 9780578718156 DOWNLOAD: TANGERINE VAGINA : TRUE STORIES OF CHILDHOOD MISFORTUNE AND MEANINGLESS SEX Tangerine Vagina : True Stories of Childhood Misfortune and Meaningless Sex PDF Book It studies each of the 26 movements, breathing exercises and rest poses that form the basis of every Bikram yoga class. Inspired by traditional Chinese medicine, Glow outlines simple, classic diagnostic techniques and therapies, a whole food diet, and active lifestyle to realize balance and tranquilitythe keys to true beautyand undo what time and stress have done. Vespasian had started the construction of the Colosseum shortly after becoming emperor in 69 A. " As strange as it sounds, maggot therapy is often a patient's last chance to prevent amputation of a limb. Henty to the humble cigarette card. Directions and the exercises themselves are given in Spanish. The inclusion of a new classification on Chinese medicinal herbs is particularly innovative, offering information that appears here for the first time in English. A total of 128 sample flashcards that allow you to study on the go. Contextualising Narrative Inquiry: Developing methodological approaches for local contextsNarrative inquiry is growing in popularity as a research methodology in the social sciences, medicine and the humanities. Debt-Free Spending Plan"The only how-to guide to today's most lucrative, fast-moving investment opportunity. Bruce Hammond shows how discounts and deals can reduce the "sicker price" at top colleges nationwide--even for families who would not have qualified for need-based aid in the past.
  • Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice

    Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice

    Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice Alexander Simon Clark Submitted for the degree of D.Phil. University of Sussex September, 2001 Declaration I hereby declare that this thesis has not been submitted, either in the same or different form, to this or any other university for a degree. Signature: Acknowledgements First, I would like to thank Bill Keller, for his supervision over the past three years. I would like to thank my colleagues at ISSCO for making me welcome and especially Susan Armstrong for giving me the job in the first place, and various people for helpful comments and discussions including Chris Manning, Dan Klein, Eric Gaussier, Nicola Cancedda, Franck Thollard, Andrei Popescu-Beilis, Menno van Zaanen and numerous other people including Sonia Halimi for check- ing my Arabic. I would also like to thank Gerald Gazdar and Geoffrey Sampson for their helpful comments as part of my thesis committee. I would also like to thank all of the people that have worked on the various software packages and operating systems that I have used, such as LaTeX, Gnu/Linux, and the gcc compiler, as well as the people who have worked on the preparation of the various corpora I have used: in particular I am grateful to John McCarthy and Ramin Nakisa for allowing me to use their painstakingly collected data sets for Arabic. Finally I would like to thank my wife, Dr Clare Hornsby, for her unfailing support and encouragement (in spite of her complete lack of understanding of the subject) and my children, Lily and Francis, for putting up with my absences both physical and mental, and showing me how language acquisition is really done – over-regularisation of past tense verbs included.
  • Table 6: Correlations of Length of Audio Recording of Time with Accuracy and RT of Responses

    Table 6: Correlations of Length of Audio Recording of Time with Accuracy and RT of Responses

    A MULTI-METHOD INVESTIGATION OF THE EFFECTIVENESS AND UTILITY OF DELAYED CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK IN SECOND-LANGUAGE ORAL PRODUCTION By JAMES DUNCAN HUNTER, MA Module 3 (thesis) submitted to the School of Humanities of the University of Birmingham in partial fulfilment of the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in Applied Linguistics Centre for English Language Studies Department of English University of Birmingham Edgbaston Birmingham B15 2TT UK NOVEMBER 2011 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third parties in respect of this work are as defined by The Copyright Designs and Patents Act 1988 or as modified by any successor legislation. Any use made of information contained in this thesis/dissertation must be in accordance with that legislation and must be properly acknowledged. Further distribution or reproduction in any format is prohibited without the permission of the copyright holder. ABSTRACT A major challenge in second-language pedagogy and research is that of determining linguistic competence. Fluent oral production gives some indication of the state of a learner’s interlanguage, but the presence of non-target-like forms in such production confounds the analysis since the teacher or researcher cannot be certain whether such forms are random or systematic. Corrective feedback (CF) in oral production, usually in the form of recast or elicitation, can thus appear arbitrary and inconsistent. This thesis investigates the effectiveness of delayed CF, in which representative samples of learners’ non-target-like production are systematically collected and tracked.
  • New Perspectives on Language Development and the Innateness of Grammatical Knowledge Christophe Parisse

    New Perspectives on Language Development and the Innateness of Grammatical Knowledge Christophe Parisse

    New perspectives on language development and the innateness of grammatical knowledge Christophe Parisse To cite this version: Christophe Parisse. New perspectives on language development and the innateness of grammati- cal knowledge. Language Sciences, Elsevier, 2005, 27, pp.383-401. 10.1016/j.langsci.2004.09.015. halshs-00090997 HAL Id: halshs-00090997 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00090997 Submitted on 5 Sep 2006 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. This is the penultimate version of an article to appear in Language Sciences (2004) Title: New perspectives on language development and innateness of grammatical knowledge Author: Christophe PARISSE Affiliation: Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Paris, France Abstract : Chomsky (1965, 1986) presents a series of arguments for an innate syntactic component of the language faculty. Do the arguments proposed at that time still stand, or have they been overridden by newer proposals? The current paper emphasizes three research directions among the most recent advances in cognitive science. These directions lead to alternate proposals to the generative linguistic theory of language development. First, the 'item-based' theory of language development, which stresses that development of language knowledge goes from specific to general and is compatible with developing research in cognitive linguistics.
  • Fostering Diversity and Minimizing Universals: Toward a Non-Colonialist Approach to Studying the Acquisition of Algonquian Languages J

    Fostering Diversity and Minimizing Universals: Toward a Non-Colonialist Approach to Studying the Acquisition of Algonquian Languages J

    Native Studies Review 19, no. 1 (2010) Fostering Diversity and Minimizing Universals: Toward a Non-Colonialist Approach to Studying the Acquisition of Algonquian Languages J. Dean Mellow Seeking to determine valid and useful analyses of the acquisition of the Algonquian language Anihshininiimowin, this article critiques Chomsky’s Universal Grammar (CUG) approach and instead pro- poses the use of a construction-based, emergentist approach. CUG emphasizes hypothetical universals and minimizes linguistic diversity in a way that is Eurocentric and scientifically problematic. In contrast, emergentism fosters diversity and minimizes universals in a manner that is useful for analyzing acquisition, for documenting the rich and expressive constructions of Anihshininiimowin, and for assisting with curriculum development. Academic scholarship occurs within social and political contexts, and therefore the study of language should not perpetuate colonialist processes. Cherchant à déterminer des analyses valides et utiles de l’acquisition de la langue algonquine, l’anihshininiimowin, cet article critique l’approche grammaticale universelle de Chomsky et propose à la place l’utilisation d’une approche émergentiste basée sur la construc- tion. La grammaire universelle de Chomsky souligne des universels hypothétiques et minimise la diversité linguistique d’une manière eurocentrique et problématique au niveau scientifique. En revanche, l’émergentisme favorise la diversité et minimise les universels d’une manière qui est utile pour analyser l’acquisition, pour documenter les constructions riches et expressives de l’anihshininiimowin, et pour aider au développement du programme didactique. L’art professoral universitaire se déroule dans des contextes sociaux et politiques et, par conséquent, l’étude de la langue ne devrait pas perpétuer des procédés colonialistes.
  • New Foundations for General Linguistics Victor H. Yngve

    New Foundations for General Linguistics Victor H. Yngve

    Book Reviews From Grammar to Science: New Foundations for General Linguistics Victor H. Yngve (The University of Chicago) Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1996, xi+350 pp; hardbound, ISBN 1-55619-515-X, $89.00 Reviewed by Geoffrey Sampson University of Sussex In the course of his long career, Victor Yngve has become seriously disenchanted with the unempirical character of modern linguistics. Within the discipline as it has developed, there seems to be no scientific way of deciding among the many contenders .... We find positions and methods being promoted like a new movie or defended with withering polemics or taken up like the latest fad ... Most readers will recognize what Yngve is complaining about. For many of us who see it as a serious problem, the response has been to become "corpus linguists." The implications of the phrase seem a little odd (surely we would not expect to find that geography, say, included a specialism of "data geography" alongside a larger band of geographers who based their theories on guesswork or "intuition"?); but in practice "corpus linguistics" functions as a convenient shelter beneath which scientific linguistic research can proceed, undisturbed by the weird and wonderful things happening in areas of linguistics where people make their examples up out of their heads. To Yngve, this is an insufficiently radical response to the empiricism problem. Even corpus linguists routinely depend on concepts such as "word," "noun," "sentence," and "utterance," which have no clear correlates in observable reality. Yngve surveys the origins of grammatical thought in the classical tradition and points out, correctly, that the grammatical categories still used today derive largely from concepts devel- oped in connection with universal, a priori laws of logic rather than with description of contingent, concrete realities.