EFFECTS OF EXPERIMENTAL FORAGER REMOVALS ON DIVISION OF LABOUR IN THE PRIMITIVELY EUSOCIAL INSTABILIS (: )

by

SEAN O'DONNELJYl (Department of Psychology, Box 351525, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA)

(Acc. 20-X-1997)

Summary Experimental forager removals were performed to assess the mechanisms by which Polistes instahilis colonies regulate their intake of nectar and water. Most foragers gathered nectar, while water was collected by a small number of fixated foragers. Removal of the most active water foragers led lo decreases in water foraging, followed by recruitment of a single replacement water forager. Replacement water foragers were usually recruited from among the workers that had previously collected water al low rates. Waler forager removals showed that some workers specialized on waler collection, but these workers differed in their thresholds of response to colony need for nest cooling. Removal of the most active nectar foragers led to longer-lasting (one to three days) decreases in colony nectar collection rates, and resulted in replacement nectar foragers being recruited away from other foraging tasks or from nest tasks. Nectar forager removals were followed by increases in rates of dominance interactions among nest ; this response was not observed after water forager removals. Dominance interactions among workers appear to regulate nectar foraging in P instabilis. The mechanisms of regulation of foraging differ among matelials, and correspond to their maximum rates of collection, predictability of resources, and on the costs of short-term changes in supply to the colony.

l) E-mail: [email protected] 2l Financial support was provided by the University of Washington Royalty Research Fund and by the US National Science Foundation (grant to Robert Jeanne). Bette Louiselle assisted with data collection. 111anks to Robert Jeanne, Laurence Phelps, and Andrew Bousma for further assistance and support. in the field.

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 1998 Behaviour 135, 173-193 174 SEAN O'DONNELL

Introduction

To the extent that colony-level selection has shaped social worker be­ haviour, division of labour (polyethism) is assumed to be adaptive. Workers are expected to modify which tasks they perform in response to changes in colony needs, such that colony survival and reproduction are maximized (Oster & Wilson, l 978; Calabi, 1988). However, insect colonies are com­ posed of idiosyncratic subunits (Jeanne, 1991 ): individual marking has shown thac workers are not behaviourally identical. For example, even similarly aged nest mates can vary in specialization on tasks (O'Donnell & Jeanne, 1992) and rates of behavioural development (O'Donnell & Jeanne, 1993, I 995a). Because workers are idiosyncratic, individual behavioural differences arc a key component to the structure of insect colonies (Robin­ son, 1992). A fuller understanding of insect colony function wi ll depend on analysis of the regulation of individual worker behaviour. Quantifica­ tion of how workers respond to experimental manipulations of colony need for task performance has become an important tool in studies of division of labour (Wilson, 1985). Social insect workers must make such decisions as which tasks to per­ form and the rate at which to perform a given task. However, workers' decisions are made based on incomplete information. Even in small insect societies, any individual worker is unlikely to be able to simultaneously and accurately assess the level of colony need for all tasks. This raises the question of which cues are used by workers when deciding which task(s) to perform. Workers may respond directly to physical cues that indicate the need to perform certain tasks (e.g. an increase in C02 concentration is perceived directly by workers and leads to nest fanning in honey bees; Seeley, 1974). Workers may also respond to social cues. For example, activity or task performance by nest mates (Dew & Michener, 1981), and dominance interactions (Akre et al., 1976; O'Donnell & Jeanne, I 995b ), may be used as indirect social cues indicating the level of colony need for particular tasks. Furthermore, workers often differ in their reactions to a given set of physical and social stimuli. Nest mates do not behave identically under the same or similar conditions, and recent models for the bases of indi vidual differences in task performance have posited that workers differ in their thresholds of response to a given change in colony conditions (Page & SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 175

Robinson, 1991 ). Different thresholds of response may be based on prior experience (Plowright & Plowright, 1988; Theraulaz et al., 1991) or on genetic variation among workers (Fewell & Page, 1993). Eusocial wasps of the genus Polistes (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) are im­ portant but underutilized subjects for studies of regulation of division of labour. Polistes spp. are primitively eusociaJ relatives of solitary and ad­ vanced eusocial Vespidae (Carpenter, 1982). Information on worker be­ haviour in Polistes spp. may provide the basis for comparative inferences on how polyethism has evolved along with changes in colony size and social complexity in wasps (O'Donnell, 1995). Polistes spp. colonies are typically small (mature populations of fewer than 150 adults; Reeve, 1991 ), allowing accurate measurement of colony-wide behaviour. The exposed combs of Polistes spp. nests permit detailed records of individual behaviour (West-Eberhard, 1969). The present study focused on regulation of forag­ ing behaviour, which has been the subject of prior studies of polyethism in Polistes spp. (Dew & Michener, 1981; Reeve & Gamboa, 1983; Theraulaz et al., 1992) and in other eusocial Vcspidae (O'Donnell & Jeanne, 1990; Jeanne, 1996). Foragers must respond to both intrinsic (social and devel­ opmental) and extrinsic (environmental) changes in colony needs in order to maintain levels of food and building material input. I quantified short-term individual and colony-level responses to experi­ mental forager removals in the wasp Polistes instabilis de Saussure. For­ ager removal mimicked changes in colony social structure that might occur naturally when foragers are lost to senescence or to predators. I minimized the effects of long-term changes in colony foraging requirements that oc­ cur during colony development (West-Eberhard, 1969; Hoshikawa, 1981; Tsuchida, 1991) by quantifying behavioural changes that occurred over short periods of time (several hours to several days). As noted by Jeanne (1991), failure to account or control for colony developmental changes have made some studies documenting individual differences in Polistes spp. forager behaviour difficult to interpret. Removal of foragers changed the level of colony need for performance of foraging tasks by temporarily reducing the rate of arrival of the materials they had collected. I assumed that the colonies' need for the affected material would remain constant over the course of each experimental trial, and therefore expected changes in foraging behaviour of nest mates that 176 SEAN O'DONNELL would adjust the colonies' material collection rates toward pre-manipulation levels (the corrective response, in the terminology of Jeanne, 1996). The first goal of this study was to assess the roles of two aspects of individual behaviour (task specialization and thresholds of response) in the colonies ' corrective responses. The second goal was to test whether changes in social interactions among wasps on the nest, especially dominance interactions, play a role in the colonies' adjustment of foraging rates.

Forager specialization. an.d thresholds of response Although Polistes spp. workers often engage in a diversity of tasks and exhibit little age polyethism, task specialization occurs in a number of species. Forager specialization on materials has been documented in un­ manipulated Polistes spp. colonies (Owen, 1962; Strassmann et al., I 984; Post et al., 1988), but whether fo rager specialization persists following changes in colony conditions (O' Donnell & Jeanne, 1990) has not been tested in Polistes wasps. I tested fo r the existence of fo rager specialization on materials in P instabilis through observation and experimental manipu­ lations. During control observations, I asked whether individuals devoted most of their foraging effort to collecting a single material, and whether a disproportionate amount of colony foraging effort for that material was due to a subset of the acti ve foragers. I tested experimentally whether forager specializations would persist following changes in colony need by removing nectar and water foragers. If forager specialization persisted, the individuals responding to forager removals would be recruited from among those that had performed the affected task, aJbeit at lower rates, prior to the manipulation (O'Donnell & Jeanne, 1990). Alternatively, if specializa­ tion did not persist, then switching among foraging tasks and activation of non-foragers would occur in response to fo rager removals. Independently of task specialization, workers could differ in their thresh­ olds of response to a given task-inducing stimulus (Page & Robinson, 1991). I tested for individual differences in response thresholds by remov­ ing the most active water foragers. If thresholds of response were similar or identical among the workers that collected water, then foragers would be replaced immediately following removal, assuming a constant or in­ creasing level of colony need for nest cooling. Furthermore, the recruited workers would forage at the same rates as the original foragers. Alterna- SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 177 tively, if water foragers differed in response thresholds, the recruitment of new foragers would be delayed and/or their foraging rate would be dimin­ ished relative to the removed foragers, even though the stimulus or need for water foraging remained constant or increased following removal.

Dominance interactions

Gamboa et al. ( 1990) posited that queens are the principle organizers of colony foraging activity in Polistes spp. colonies. Queens appear to play a major role in regulating the foraging behaviour of workers in the temperate species P fuscatus (Reeve & Gamboa, 1983, 1987). Queen activity, partic­ ularly aggression (dominance behaviour) directed toward certain workers, has been shown experimentally and observationally to influence foraging rates in Polistes spp. workers (Strassmann & Meyer, 1983; Gamboa et al., 1990). However, dominance interactions among workers have also been implicated in stimulation of foraging in wasps (reviewed in O'Donnell & Jeanne, 1995b). I tested for queen and worker dominance effects by mea­ suring whether changes in rates of dominance interactions corresponded to the changes in foraging behaviour that followed nectar and water forager removals. If dominance interactions lead to increases in foraging, then rates of dominance would increase following forager removals, and would correspond to increases in foraging rates.

Methods

Subject colonies

Data were collected from five post-worker emergence Polistes instabilis colonies in Gua­ nacaste Province, Costa Rica. Colony A was located at the Organization for Tropical Studies field station at Palo Verde ( 10° I 61 N, 85° I 41 W); colonies B through E were located at Centro Eco16gico La Pacifica (I 0°251 N, l\5°07'W), 5 km NW of the town of Canas. Subject nests had been constructed on vegetation 0.5 to 1.5 m above the ground. Some obscuring vegetation was clipped from around colonies B, C, and E, which may have in­ creased exposure to the sun; nest sites were otherwise unmanipulated. All adults present upon discovery of the colonies were individually marked with either model airplane elope (colony A) or paint pens (other colonies). Additional adults were marked on the days they emerged. The study was conducted between 19 July and 27 September 1988 (colonies A through D), and between 13 and 18 August 1997 (colony E). Colonies were observed one at a time for 3 to 11 days each. Queens were distinguished by the performance of egg laying; one female per colony laid eggs

Control observations

Control observations were conducted on l to 3 days immediately preceding each experi­ mental manipulation. During observation periocls J recorded behavioural data while scaled 0.5-1 m from the nests, facing the cell openings. Data on all forager arrivals were recorded into a portable tape cassette player, comprising time to the nearest min, forager identity, and which material was transferred by the forager to nest mates, larvae, or onto the nest within 5 min of arrival. Times of forager departures were also noted to the nearest min in colonies B through E. Observations were continuous except during periods of heavy rain, when foraging usually ceased (pers. obs.). Daily observation periods (including experimen­ tal observations, below) lasted approximately 3:40 h on average. Behaviour of all wasps on the nests was recorded during scan samples every JO min, following an ethogram similar to those of Post et al. (1988) and Tsuchida ( 1991 ). At colony B, only presence/absence of adult wasps was recorded during scans conducted every 15 min. Other than allogrooming, all behavioural acts involving physical contact among workers (e.g. biting and grappling), as well as chasing and lateral vibrations of the gastcr against the nest (Gamboa & Dew, 1981), were classified as dominance interactions. Rates of foraging and of dominance interactions during the control observations were used as a colony-specific baseline against which to compare rates of performance following experimental forager removals (below). Workers to be removed during experimental trials were selected on the basis of their high foraging rates du ring the control observations. Data on forager specialization and task partitioning collected during control periods were presented elsewhere (0 ' Donnell, 1995).

Water forager removals

One or two workers identified as the most active water foragers during control observations were removed from their nests. Foragers were removed by grasping them with forceps and allowing them to climb on, then lifting them from the nest (West-Eberhard, 1969). This was usually accomplished without visibly alarming their nest mates. Removed water foragers were held in a small mesh enclosure with ad libitum waler and sugar solution until they were returned to their nests. Behavioural data were collected during the experimental trials as described above for 1:45 to 2:45 h following removals. During water forager removal experiments, the number of wasps engaged in fruming the nest was used as an additional measure of the colony's need for nest thermoregulation. A total of four water removal trails were conducted on colonies A, B, and C (two trials). In tri al 1 a second water forager was removed after she responded to the first removal.

Nectar fora{!er r('movals

The two or three most active nectar foragers, which together had accounted for at least 25% of the total colony nectar foraging effort during control observations, were removed from their nests. Nectar foragers were removed either the evening before the trial began, or on the day of the trial within 0.5 h of the start or experimental observations. Behavioural data were collected as above for one to three days following nectar forager removals. A total of four trials were conducted on colonies C (two trials), D, and E. SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 179

Nest wasp removal

To test for effects of dominance interactions among workers on nectar foraging, two workers engaging in dominance interactions at high rates were removed from their nest in one trial. The workers that were selected for removal spent most of their time on the nest and rarely foraged (they did not collect nectar) during control observations. They were highly aggressive during control observations, biting and chasing nest mates more often than other wasps.

Statistical analyses

Foraging rates were calculated using time elapsed after the lirst observed water or nectar forager arrival at the start of each day's observations. Following Jeanne (1996), I divided the experimental observation period following each water forager removal into initial and corrective response periods. The initial response period began at the onset of observations and ended with the first ten min period where the number of forager arrivals equaled or exceeded the mean number per 10 min during control observations. The corrective response period followed the initial response period, and lasted until the end of a trial or until the next manipulation. Nectar foraging rates were analyzed on a per-day basis. I used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (Sokal & Rohlf, 1981) to test the null hypothesis that the cumulative distribution of water forager arrivals over time during each initial and corrective response period did not differ from the distribution of arrivals during the control period. The effects of water forager removals on colony-wide rates of nest fanning were assessed with the Kruskal-Wallis test corrected for ties (Sokal & Rohlf, 1981). For nectar forager (N = 4 trials) and nest wasp (N = 1 trial) removals, I tested for equality of nectar foraging rates between treatment days using survival analysis (SAS, 1985). I tested whether nectar foraging rates on the first day following forager or nest wasp removal were lower than rates on control days, and whether rates on subsequent days were higher than on the first day following removal (one-tailed tests). For nectar forager removals, multiple linear regression (GLM procedure; SAS Institute, 1985) was used to test for effects of the number of days elapsed since forager removal on rates of dominance interactions across colonies.

Results Patterns offorager behaviour Foraging rates were relatively low in the late afternoon (pers. obs.); all behavioural data presented were collected in the morning to early afternoon (between 06:40 and I 2:45 h local time). Foragers collected four materials: wood pulp for nest construction, water for nest cooling, and food (nectar and insect prey). Water foragers spread their loads directly onto the nest surface; liquid loads that were spread on the nest were never shared with nest mates, nor were they offered to larvae. Nectar and prey loads were shared with other adults and/or given to larvae. Wood pulp loads were 180 SEAN O'DONNELL either partitioned with nest mates or used by the fo rager to lengthen the walls of brood cells. Prey and pulp foraging rates were not analyzed. Within colonies, individuals varied widely in their rates of nectar and water collection (Fig. 1).

Water forager removals

Water was collected by foragers from colonies A, B, C, and E whose nests were exposed to direct sun for at least part of the day. Relative to nectar, few foragers (24-50% of all foragers in colonies that collected water; Fig. I) collected water. When the nests were insolated, water was

8 3.0 Colony A Colony B 2.5 6 2.0 5 1.5 4 3 1.0 2 ...... 0.5 ::::> 0 0.0 O .l.IJl.l.ll!"11llt!r:.tl.lia.oL.W111Jl.<2Jaa...~....,..~~ ..c: ..... 2.5 1.25 I/)a. Colony C Colony D ·c: 2.0 1.00 ~ _. 1.5 0.75 .....ro O> 1.0 0.50 c: O> 0.5 .....ro 0 u.. 0.0 0.7 Colony E 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 -

Fig. 1. Distributions of nectar (hatched bars) and water (black bars) foraging rates of Polistes instabilis workers from five subject colonies. Each bar represents the foraging rate of one worker; workers are presented in decreasing order of their summed nectar and water foraging rates. Numbers of additional foragers that collected only prey and pulp were: colony A - 1, colony B - I, colony C - 0, colony D - 5, colony E - 3. SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 181 collected at high rates, with a load arriving at the colony every 3.21 to I 0.37 min on average during control periods (range of mean elapsed time between water load arrivals across colonies A, B, & C; O'Donnell, 1995). Foragers spent an average of 1.91 to 6.26 min away from the nest on water trips (mean time to collect a water load, colonies B & C, respectively; O'Donnell, 1995). During all control periods, a single worker accounted for a dispropor­ tionate amount (at least 40%) of the colony's water collection (Table 1). When the most active water foragers were removed, they were not im­ mediately replaced. Water foraging rates decreased following every water forager removal, and were significantly lower than the control rate during three of the five initial response periods (Fig. 2). In trials 1 and 4, water foraging rates did not recover to control levels following the first forager removal, and a corrective response period could not be defined. However, water foraging rates recovered and exceeded control foraging rates during corrective response periods, significantly so in trial 2. Two types of evidence suggest that decreases in need for nest cooling did not cause lower water foraging rates following forager removals. First, weather conditions did not change noticeably, and nests remained insolated, throughout the experimental trials. Water droplets placed by the removed foragers evaporated within a few minutes, long before the end of the initial response periods (pers. obs.). Second, rates of nest fanning suggested that the need for cooling remained high following forager removals. In two of the three trials where nest fanning behaviour was recorded, median rates of fanning (number of wasps fanning per scan) did not differ significantly among treatments (Kruskal-Wallis test with correction for ties; trial 3, H = 4.72, df = 2, p > 0.05; trial 4, H = 0.03, df = 2, p > 0.75). In trial I, the fanning rate did not differ between the control and first forager removal treatments (H = 0.10, df = l, p > 0.75), but was lower during the second forager removal treatment (H = 16.30, df = 1, p < 0.001). However, water foraging rates were not significantly depressed during the second forager removal treatment (Fig. 2). Although queens typically spent more time on the nests than other wasps (O'Donnell, 1995), they were never observed fanning. A single worker replaced the removed water forager(s) as the water specialist (accounting for > 50% of the colony's water trips) following 182 SEAN O'DONNELL

TABLE 1. Water foraging rates of Polistes instabilis workers during exper- imental manipulations

Trial 1 Control Firs! forager removal Second forager removal Id For. rate % effort Id for. rate % effort Id For. rate % effort 15 7.86 (41.9%) * * 6 3.49 (18.6%) * * 12 2.18 (l l.6%) 12 0 12 l 1.85 (57.1%) 14 1.3 1 (7.0%) 14 12.00 (94.l %) * Total 18.79 Torn.I 12.75 Total 20.75

Trial 2 Control Forager removal Forager returned Id For. rate % effort Id For. rate % effort ld For. rate % effort 124 10.99 (76.5%) * 124 22.00 (6 1.1 %) 33 3.10 (21.6%) 33 3.33 (19.2%) 33 8.00 (22.2%) 54 0 54 11 .33 (65.4%) 54 4.00 (11.1 %)

Total 14.37 Total 17.32 Total 36.00

Trial 3 Control Forager removal Forager returned Id For. rate % effort Id For. rate % effort Id For. rate % effort 3 10.22 (90.5%) * 3 1.00 (8.3%) 11 0.54 (4.8%) 11 5.54 (100%) 11 10.00 (83.3%)

Total 11.29 Total 5.54 Total 12.00

Trial 4 Control Forager removal Id For. rate % effort Id For. rate % effort 1 19.23 (83.3%) * 3 1.92 (8.3%) 3 3.24 (100%) Total 23.08 Total 3.24 For each trial, the most active water foragers' identity numbers are shown (Id), followed by their water foraging rate in trips/hour, and the percent of the colony's water trips they contributed (% effort). Within a trial, manipulations arc ordered temporally from left to right, starting with the control observation period. foragers that were removed in a given manipulation are represented by an asterix (*). The colony total water foraging rate (trips/hour) is shown for each treatment period. SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 183

50 60 Trial 1 40 50 40 n; 30 "' / 30 > t ·c... 20 .._C1I ...0 10 10 Q) .D 0 0 E :J 0 2 0 2 3 c Q) 35 25 .2: Trial 3 Trial4 ca 30 :; 20 E 25 :J (.) 20 15 15 .. t t 10 10 5 5 0 0 2 3 2

Time (hours)

Fig. 2. Effecls of water forager removals on water foraging rates in Polistes i11stabilis. Cumulative numhers of water loads arriving at nests are plotted againsl time; the slope of each curve indicates the rate of water foragi ng. Observation time began with the first water forager arrival for each trial. Ex perimental manipulations are marked al the time of their occurrence with an arrow pointing up in the case of forager removals, and down in the case of foragers being returned to the nest. Treatments and response periods (see Methods) arc indicated by symbols as follows: control observations - filled circles; first manipulation initial response - open circles; first manipulation corrective response - filled triangles; second manipulation initial response - open triangles; second manipulation corrective response - filled squares. Response periods where the water foraging rate differed significantly from the control rate arc indicated by ** (Kolmogorov-Smimov test, p < 0.01). every water forager removal (Table 1). In some cases, a secondary water specialist also became active, accounting for ~ 20% of the colony's water trips (Table 1). Replacement water foragers were usually individuals that had collected water at a low rate prior to the manipulation. The only exception to this pattern was seen in trial 2, where a worker that had previously collected nectar at a low rate became the water specialist. In trial I, the first replacement water forager was removed after 1 :20 h of observation, and was in tum replaced by another active water specialist 184 SEAN O'DONNELL

(Fig. 2; Table I). In trials 2 and 3, the original water specialists were returned to the nest after a replacement forager had become active. In both cases, the original forager resumed water collection. Replacement foragers also continued to collect water in both cases, leading to increases in the colony-wide rates of water foraging (Fig. 2; Table 1). Colony-wide rates of dominance interactions did not change consistently following water forager removals (effect of treatment period nested within trials on per-scan rate

of dominance interactions: F3,4 = 3.47, p > 0. 10).

Nectar forager removals

ln contrast to water, nectar was gathered by most foragers in each colony (85-96% of foragers; Fig. I). Nectar trips often required greater time away from the nest for individual foragers (29.97 to 46.71 min; range of mean time to collect a nectar load across colonies B, C, & D; O'Donnell, 1995) than did water trips (see above). Consequently, individual nectar foragers returned to the nest less frequently than did water foragers. The removed nectar foragers contributed more of the nectar collection during control periods than expected if all workers had foraged at equal rates (percent of nectar foragers removed/percent of nectar foraging they contributed; trial 1: 33178; trial 2: 50170; trial 3: 12.5/25; trial 4: 20/39). Removals of the two or three most active nectar foragers were always followed by decreases in nectar foraging rates on the following day; these decreases were significant in 3 of 4 trials (Fig. 3; survival analysis Wilcoxon test, df = 1; trial 1: x2 = 9.53, p < 0.01; trial 2: x2 = 8.86, p < 0.01; trial 3: x2 = 2. 10, NS; trial 4: x2 = 4.62, p < 0.05). In the three trials where foraging was monitored for three days follow­ ing nectar forager removal, colony-wide nectar foraging rates eventually increased significantly above the first post-manipulation day rates (Fig. 3; survival analysis Wilcoxon test comparing nectar foraging rate on the first day after manipulation with the third day after manipulation, df = 1; trial 2: x2 = 13.08, p < 0.001; trial 3: x2 = 3.96, p < 0.05; trial 4: x2 = 3.59, p < 0.05). Nectar foraging rates eventually surpassed control rates in two trials (Fig. 3). Increases in individual nectar foraging rates accounted for little of the colonies' recovery from forager removals. Only on the third day following removal in trial 2 did previously active foragers increase their total foraging rate by more than I 0% above the control rate (Fig. 3). SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIO UR 185

Trial 1 5 6 Trial 2

4

3

2

0 0 Conltol Removal Control Removal Removal Removal (1) Q) (1) (2) (3) ~ c:Cl NS '6> Trial 4 __• _ ~ 8 Trial 3 8 0 u. 7 7 6 6 NS

2 2 1 0 0 Control Removal Removal Removal (1) (2) (3) Control Removal Removal Removal (1) (2) (3) Treatment (Day)

Fig. 3. Effects of nectar forager removals on nectar foraging ralcs in Polistes irzstabilis. Contributions lo total colony nectar foraging arc indicated in each treatment period for the removed workers (black bars), other nectar foragers active during the control period (single-hatched bars), and new nectar foragers recruited after removal (double-hatched bars). Lines above the bars indicate results of statistical tests (survival analysis Wilcoxon test) for differences in foraging rates between the treatment periods overlapped by ends of tbe lines. Significant differences in nectar foraging rates are indicated by * (p < 0.05) and ** (p < 0.01).

Instead, the increased nectar foraging rates were due in large part to the recruitment of new workers (number of new nectar foragers fo llowing re­ moval/total number of nectar fo ragers; trial 2: 11114; trial 3: 711 5; trial 4: 611 5. In trial 3, the queen foraged for nectar on the third day after forager removal. This was the only observation of queen foraging in the study). Newly recruited foragers contributed substantially to the recovery of nectar foraging rates following removals (Fig. 3). Some of the recruited workers either switched from foraging for other materials or added nectar foraging to the set of materials they collected (trial 2: 2 foragers; trial 3: 3 foragers; 186 SEAN O'DONNELL

1.00 0 Q) ~ 1 - Trla12 ~ Trial3 l ~~ 0.75 ~ Trial4 c .Q ~o VJ~ ~ Q) ~.s 0.50 E 4> ::I 0 E~ ·->< ·-c 0.25 ns E :E 0 I '1::'"0 a. 0.00 ContJOI Removal RemovaIl Removal ( 1) (2) (3) Treatment (Day)

Fig. 4. Effects of nectar forager removals on rat.es of dominance interactions among work­ ers in Polistes instabilis. Because absolute rates of dominance interactions varied among colonies, the per scan-sample rates are shown as proportions of the maximum rate observed for each colony. Dominance interaction rates increased significantly over time following nectar forager removals (linear multiple regression). trial 4: 4 foragers). The remainder of the recruited nectar collectors had not foraged previously. Queens did not engage in dominance interactions with workers in scan samples during nectar forager removal experiments. Workers engaged in dominance interactions, and rates per scan-sample of dominance interac­ tions among workers increased over time following nectar forager removals (Fig. 4; effect of days from start of trial nested within trials on per-scan rate of dominance interactions F3,8 = 5.39, p < 0.05). One or two work­ ers that spent most of their time on the nests became aggressive following nectar forager removals, biting and chasing nest mates at high rates and apparently stimulating forager departures (pers. obs.). In the trial where two aggressive nest wasps were removed, nectar foraging rates declined significantly on the following day (Fig. 5; survival analysis Wilcoxon test x2 = 5.62, df = I, p < 0.05). On the third day after removal, these wasps were returned to the nest and nectar foraging increased significantly (Fig. 5; survival analysis Wilcoxon test x2 = 4.87, df = 1, p < 0.05). Colony­ wide rates of dominance interactions also declined following removal of the aggressive workers, and increased again following their return. SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 187

8.0

0+---

0.2 fl ­ u~! 0.4 oa.g c: 0.6 .. ~ ~! 0.8-1----..------,,-----,.- - ---.---- -. Control Removal Removal Return (1) (2)

Treatment (Day)

Fig. 5. Effect of removing dominant workers on nectar foraging rate (u pper bars) and rate of dominance interactions (lower bars) in Polisres insrabilis. Lines above the top bars indicate results of statistical tests (survival analysis Wilcoxon test) for differences in foraging rates between the treatment periods overl apped by ends of the lines. Significant differences in nectar foraging rates are indicated by * (p < 0.05).

Discussion

Forager specialization on. materials

Specialization on water collection by a small number of fi xated foragers, at least on short time scales of several hours to several days, appears to be wide spread in eusocial Vespidae, both in primitively eusocial (Ito & Ya­ mane, 1992; this study) and in advanced eusocial species (Forsyth, 1978; O' Donnell & Jeanne, 1990, l 992). Polistes instabilis water forager special­ ization was an important component of the colony response to decreased water supply. Water was collected by a small number of fixated foragers, and the response to water forager removal usually involved changes in foraging rate, rather than switching among tasks. It is often assumed that differences in individual experience lead to worker fix ation on tasks, but the possibility that genotypic differences among individuals play a role in determining task specialization (Page et al. , 1995; O'Donnell , 1996) is untested in primitively eusociaJ wasps. Polyandry (queens mating with 188 SEAN O' DONNELL more than one male) increases genotypic diversity within colonies, and is documented or suspected in a number of Polistes spp. (Metcalf & Whitt, 1977; Metcalf, 1980; Strassmann et al. , 1989). In contrast to water, nectar collection did not depend on the efforts of a few, fixated foragers. In response to nectar forager removals, behavioural flexibility on the part of nest mates was important. Many recruited nectar foragers switched from on-nest tasks or from collecting other materials. Changes in the size and sugar concentration of loads collected by foragers were not measured, and increases in these variables may have compen­ sated for reduced rates of nectar load arrival at the colony level caused by forager removals. However, bumble bee foragers did not change load size or concentration in response to manipulation of colony food stores that affected their colonies' foraging rates (Cartar, 1992), suggesting that active foragers may already be collecting nectar as rapidly as possible.

Individual behaviour and regulation offoraging rates

Previous studies of polyethism in Polistes spp. have treated foraging as a single task (Dew & Michener, 198 1; Reeve & Gamboa, 1983; Theraulaz et al., 1991, J992; Giannotti & Machado, 1994). However, like other eusocial Vespidae, Polistes spp. foragers in the wild collect mateiials that fulfill different colony needs: insect prey as nourishment for the brood, nectar/honeydew as an adult energy source, wood pulp for nest building, and water for nest cooling (Owen, 1962; Post et al., 1988; Tsuchida, 199 1; O'Donnell , 1995). These materials require different amounts of time in the field for collection (O'Donnell, 1995). Foraging is not a single task in P instabilis. The materials that workers collect differ in degree of short-term individual task fixation, collection time, and the effects of decreases in collection rate on the colony. The results of this study suggest that the collection of nectar and water have distinct mechanisms of regulation at the colony level. Differences in the regulation of foraging correspond to the short-term effects on colony fitness of changes in the arrival rate of a given material. For example, there is an almost immediate cost to not cooling the nest, especially when the nest is insolated. Overheating can cause brood mortality and possibly failure of the colony in a very short span of time. Water for cooling can be collected rapidly from relatively stable, large sources by experienced foragers. The SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 189 need for a quick response to changes in the water supply to the nest, and the ability of a small number of individuals to collect large amounts of water quickly, may have selectively favored colony reliance on individual water specialists to respond to changes in water arrival rates. Conversely, the rate of flow of nectar to the nest may be more vari­ able, depending on the availability and ease of location of temporally and spatially unpredictable nectar and honeydew sources. Active P. instabilis nectar foragers showed little or no ability to increase their foraging rates in response to forager removals. If nectar is difficult to locate and collect, ac­ tive nectar foragers may not be able to greatly increase their foraging rates. Changes in the rate of arrival of one or a few nectar foragers, such as those induced by this studies' manipulations, may often indicate changes in nec­ tar resource availability. Although Polistes spp. do not usually store large amounts of food per se in their nest (but see Strassmann, 1979), changes in the incoming food supply may be temporarily buffered by the larvae, which can store and secrete nutritious fluids for adult nourishment (Hunt, 1984). These factors may have selected for decreased colony reliance on specialized nectar foragers, and favored task switching over changes in collection rate at the individual level.

Response thresholds The colony responses to P. instabilis water forager removals suggest that nest mates differed in their perception or assessment of colony need for water collection. The stimuli indicating need for nest cooling, including degree of exposure to the sun and presumably temperature, remained con­ stant or increased following water forager removals, yet there was a lag before replacement foragers became active. In some cases, the recruited forager(s) collected water at lower rates than the initial water specialist. Most of the recruited foragers had previously gathered water, and therefore were familiar with the location and handling of water. The lag in response cannot be explained by the need for recruits to learn an unfamiliar task, and suggests that water foragers differed in their thresholds of response to colony need for water collection. It appears that the responses to cues used by P. instabilis foragers in deciding when to stop working are different from those that they use in de­ ciding when to start. Individuals' thresholds of response to a given colony 190 SEAN O'DONNELL need (nectar or water collection) seemed to decrease once they began for­ aging, and they stayed active even in the face of increased overall levels of supply to their colonies. Positive feedback from successful task perfor­ mance, as proposed by Plowright & Plowright (1988), could play a role in sustaining foraging activity in P. instabilis. Interestingly, Jeanne (1996) found the opposite pattern in foragers responding to experimental nest damage: decreased need for foraging rapidly led to work­ ers dropping out of the foraging force, relative to the amount of time of increased need for foraging that elapsed before they became active. It may be the case that the P. instabilis water and nectar foraging rates would have returned to pre-treatment levels if given sufficient time. However, changes in forager behaviour appear to last much longer in P. instabilis than in P occidentalis. One possible reason for this difference is that P. instabilis foragers rely more heavily on information about colony need that they per­ ceive directly from environmental cues. Unlike P. occidentalis, tasks are rarely partitioned among P. instabilis workers (Jeanne, 1986; O'Donnell, 1995), allowing less opportunity for social feedback among nest mates that could indicate changes in colony need for a given foraging task.

The role of the queen and dominance interactions

In small insect societies, the queen can play a large role in regulating worker behaviour via direct interactions. Queens are thought to act as regulators of colony activity (Gamboa et al., 1990). However, this study does not support a direct role of queen dominance in regulating the activity of P instabilis foragers. The con-elative and experimental data suggest instead that dominance interactions among workers regulate nectar foraging rates, and the colony's ability to respond to changes in resource input. These results must be interpreted with caution because the scan sampling method I employed may have missed important social interactions that were rare and short in duration. The role of the queen in regulating responses to forager removals could be studied further by measuring all occurrences of dominance at the nest and recording the identities of participants in dominance interactions. Responses to water forager removals were nol dependent on changes in rates of dominance interactions. Water collection must recover more quickly from changes in supply than nectar collection, and a corrective SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 191 response brought about through dominance interactions may be too slow to provide sufficient thermoregulation. The data presented suggest that water collection is regulated by direct assessment of need, which varies among individuals, and negative feedback from the results of nest mates' labour.

References

Akre, R.D., Garnett, W.B., MacDonald, J.F., Greene, A. & Landolt, P. (1976). Behavior and colony development of Vespula. pensylva.nica

- - (I 991 ). Polyethism. - In: The social biology of wasps (K.G. Ross & R. W. Mall hews, eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p. 398-425. - - (1996). Regulation of nest constrnction behaviour in Polybia occidentalis. - Arum. Behav. 52, p. 473-488. Metcalf, R.A. (1980). Sex ratios, parent-offspring conflict, and local compet ition for mates in the social wasps Polistes metricus and Polistes variallls. - Am. Nat. 116, p. 642- 654. - - & Whitt, G.S. (I 977). Intra-nest relatedness in the social wasp Polistes metricus. A genetic analysis. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2, p. 353-360. O'Donnell, S. ( 1995). Division of labor in post-emergence colonies of the primitively eu­ social wasp Polistes insrahilis de Saussure (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). - Ins. Soc. 42, p. 17-29. - - (1996). RAPD markers suggest genotypic effects on forager specialization in a eu­ social wasp. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38, p. 83-88. - - & Jeanne, R.L. (1990). Forager specialization and the control of nest repair in Polyhia occidentalis Olivier (Hymenoptcra: Yespidae). - Bchav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27, p. 359- 364. I - - & - - (1992). Lifelong patterns of forager behaviour in a tropical swarm-founding wasp: effects of speciali zation and activity level on longevity. - Anim. Bchav. 44, p. 1021-1027. - - & - - ( I 993). Methoprene accelerates age polyethism in workers of a social wasp (Polybia occidentalis). - Physiol. Entomol. 18, p. I 89-194. - - & - - (1995a). Implications of senescence patterns for the evolution of age polyethism in eusocial insects. - Behav. Ecol. 6. p. 269-273. - - & - - (1995b). The roles of body size and dominance in division of labor among workers of the eusocial wasp Polybia occidentalis (Olivier) (Hymenoptcra: Yespidac). - J. K an~. Ent. Soc. 68, p. 43-50. Oster, G.F. & Wilson, E.0. (1978). Caste and ecology in the social insects. - Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ , 352 pp. Owen, J. (1962). The behavior of a social wasp Polistes fuscatus (Vespidac) al the nest, with special reference to differences between individuals. - PhD thesis, Un iversity of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Page, R.E., Jr. & Robinson, G.E. (1991). The genetics of division of labour in honey bee colonies. - Adv. Insect Physiol. 23, p. 11 7-169. - - . Waddi ngton. K.D., Hunt, G.J. & Fondrk, M.K. (1995). Genetic determinants of honey bee foraging behaviour. - Anim. Behav. 50, p. 1617-1625. Plowright, R.C. & Plowright, C.M.S. (1988). Elitism in social insects: a positive feedback model. - In: lnlerindividual behavioral variability in ~oc i al insects (R.L. Jeanne, ed.). Westvicw Press, Boulder, CO, p. 419-431. Post, D.C., Jeanne, R.L. & Erickson, [UL, Jr. ( 1988). Variation in behavior among workers of the primitively social wasp Polistes fuscat11s variaws. - In: lnterindividual be­ havioral variability in social insects (R.L. Jeanne, ed.). Westview Press, Boulder, CO, p. 283-321. Reeve, H.K. (199 1). Polistes. - In: The social biology of wasps (K.G. Ross & R.W. Mallhews, eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p. 99-148. SOCIAL WASP FORAGER BEHAVIOUR 193

- - & Gamboa, G.J. (1983). Colony activity integration in primitively eusocial wasps: the role of the queen (Polistes fuscatus, Hymenoptera: Vespidae). - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 13, p. 63-74. - - & - - ( 1987). Queen regulation of worker foraging in paper wasps: a social feedback control system (Polistes fuscatus, Hymenoptera: Vespidae). - Behaviour 102, p. 147-167. Robinson, G.E. (1992). Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. - Ann. Rev. Ent. 37, p. 637-665. SAS Institute (1985). SAS users guide: statistics. Version 5 fain. - SAS Institute, Cary, NC, 956 pp. Seeley, T.D. (1974). Atmospheric carbon dioxide regulation in honey-bee (Apis mellifera) colonies. - J. Insect Physiol. 20, p. 2301-2305. Sokal, R.R. & Rohlf, F.J. (1981). Biometry. - W.H. Freeman & Co., NY, 859 pp. Strassmann, J.E. (1979). Honey caches help female paper wasps (Po/isles annularis) survive Texas winters. - Science 204, p. 207-209. - -, Hughes, C.R., Queller, D.C., Turillazzi, S., Cervo, R., Davis, S.K. & Goodnight, K.F. (1989). Genetic relatedness in primitively eusocial wasps. - Nature 342, p. 268-269. - - & Meyer, D.C. (1983). Gerontocracy in the social wasp, Polistes exclamans. - Anim. Behav. 31 , p. 431 -438. - - , - - & Matlock, R.L. ( 1984). Behavioral castes in the social wasp, Polistes excla­ mans. - Sociobiology 8, p. 211-224. Theraulaz, G., Gervet, J. & Semenoff-Tian-Chanski, S. (1991). Social regulation of for­ aging activities in Polistes dominulu.1· Christ: a systematic approach lo behavioural organization. - Behaviour 116, p. 292-320. - -, - -, Thon, B., Pratte, M. & Semenoff-Tian-Chanski, S. (1992). The dynamics of colony organization in the primitively eusocial wasp Polistes dominulus Christ. - Ethology 91, p. 177-202. Tsuchida, K. (1991). Temporal behavioral variation and division of labor among workers in the primitively eusocial wasp, Polistes jadwigae Dalla Torre. - J. Ethol. 9, p. 129-134. West-Eberhard, M.J. (1969). The social biology of Polistine wasps. - Misc. Pub!. Mus. ZooI. Michigan 140, p. 1-10 I. Wilson, E.O. (1985). The soc.:iogenesis of insect colonies. - Science 228, p. 1489-1495.