Oxytocin, Vasopressin, and Human Social Behavior. (PDF

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Oxytocin, Vasopressin, and Human Social Behavior. (PDF YFRNE 404 No. of Pages 10, Model 5G ARTICLE IN PRESS 18 June 2009 Disk Used Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology xxx (2009) xxx–xxx 1 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yfrne 2 Review 3 Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior 4 Markus Heinrichs *, Bernadette von Dawans, Gregor Domes 5 Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland 6 article info abstract 238 9 Article history: There is substantial evidence from animal research indicating a key role of the neuropeptides oxytocin 24 10 Available online xxxx (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) in the regulation of complex social cognition and behavior. As social 25 interaction permeates the whole of human society, and the fundamental ability to form attachment is 26 11 Keywords: indispensable for social relationships, studies are beginning to dissect the roles of OT and AVP in human 27 12 Neuropeptides social behavior. New experimental paradigms and technologies in human research allow a more nuanced 28 13 Oxytocin investigation of the molecular basis of social behavior. In addition, a better understanding of the neuro- 29 14 Arginine vasopressin biology and neurogenetics of human social cognition and behavior has important implications for the 30 15 Social behavior current development of novel clinical approaches for mental disorders that are associated with social def- 31 16 Stress 17 Anxiety icits (e.g., autism spectrum disorder, social anxiety disorder, and borderline personality disorder). This 32 18 Attachment review focuses on our recent knowledge of the behavioral, endocrine, genetic, and neural effects of OT 33 19 Affiliation and AVP in humans and provides a synthesis of recent advances made in the effort to implicate the 34 20 Approach behavior oxytocinergic system in the treatment of psychopathological states. 35 21 Psychobiological therapy Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 36 22 37 38 39 1. Introduction amygdala are activated by OT and AVP receptor stimulation, 62 through which these peptides modulate the integration of excit- 63 40 In non-human mammals, receptors for the neuropeptides oxyto- atory information from the amygdala and cerebral cortex in oppo- 64 41 cin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) are distributed in various site manners. These results suggest that the endogenous balance 65 42 brain regions [94] associated with the central nervous control of between OT and AVP receptor expression and activation may set 66 43 stress and anxiety and with social behavior, including parental care, distinct, individually tuned levels for the activation of the auto- 67 44 pair-bonding, social memory, and social aggression. Specifically, OT nomic fear response. In general, centrally active AVP seems to be 68 45 seems both to enable animals to overcome their natural avoidance of associated with increased vigilance, anxiety, arousal, and activa- 69 46 proximity and to inhibit defensive behavior, thereby facilitating ap- tion, while OT has behavioral and neural effects associated with re- 70 47 proach behavior [24,26,28,45,84,124,147,164]. AVP has primarily duced anxiety, relaxation, growth, and restoration [25]. Thus, both 71 48 been implicated in male-typical social behaviors, including aggres- peptide hormones are important in social stress and in social inter- 72 49 sion, pair-bond formation, scent marking, and courtship [24,28,45, action, and in turn, a dysregulated activity may be associated with 73 50 104,165]. mental disorders of psychosocial relevance. While much of the 74 51 Aside from its effects on social behavior, OT shows significant knowledge regarding the ability of OT and AVP to regulate social 75 52 binding in the limbic system, including the amygdala [80,81, interactions is based on data from animals using centrally admin- 76 53 94,132], and decreases anxiety and the neuroendocrine response istered agonists and antagonists or knockout mice, initial studies 77 54 to stress in social interactions [11,27,120,123,158,159]. In contrast, suggest similar social and stress-related effects of both neuropep- 78 55 AVP seems to play an anxiogenic role, with elevated AVP expres- tides in humans (for review, see [12,68]). 79 56 sion in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus being associated Here, we review recent advances in the endeavor to understand 80 57 with increased behavioral and neuroendocrine anxiety levels the role of OT and AVP in human social behavior. In the first part of 81 58 [117]. In addition, Ferris and colleaguesUNCORRECTED[49] showed that the orally this review, we summarize PROOF the methodological approaches in hu- 82 59 active AVP V1a receptor antagonist SRX251 selectively blocks man neuropeptide research and examine the significance of OT 83 60 aggressive behavior in hamsters. At a cellular level, Huber and col- in stress-responsiveness, anxiety and prosocial behavior. In the 84 61 leagues [80] reported that distinct populations of neurons in the second part, we address the role of AVP in social behavior. Finally, 85 we conclude by outlining the clinical implications for mental disor- 86 ders that are associated with social deficits, and provide a synthesis 87 * Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Zürich, Binzmühlestrasse 14/Box 8, CH-8050 of the interactions of anxiety and stress, social approach behavior, 88 Zürich, Switzerland. Fax: +41 44 635 7159. and the oxytocinergic system. 89 E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Heinrichs). 0091-3022/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005 Please cite this article in press as: M. Heinrichs et al., Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior, Front. Neuroendocrinol. (2009), doi:10.1016/ j.yfrne.2009.05.005 YFRNE 404 No. of Pages 10, Model 5G ARTICLE IN PRESS 18 June 2009 Disk Used 2 M. Heinrichs et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology xxx (2009) xxx–xxx 90 2. Methodological approaches in human neuropeptide research if breast-feeding starts 30–60 min before stress exposure, depend- 151 ing on the kind of stressor [3,6,70]. As no effect of stress has been 152 91 Our current knowledge of the behavioral effects of neuropep- found on OT plasma levels, OT does not seem to mediate the atten- 153 92 tides in humans is based on: (i) correlational studies measuring uation of cortisol stress responses at the adrenal level [72]. Thus, 154 93 OT or AVP in urine, saliva, blood or CSF, (ii) correlational studies the inhibitory effect of OT on HPA axis responsiveness points to a 155 94 involving genotyping of receptor polymorphisms, and (iii) experi- more central modulation and could, in fact, be localized in the 156 95 mental studies manipulating the availability of OT or AVP using paraventricular nucleus and in the septum, as demonstrated in rats 157 96 intravenous or intranasal administration. All of these approaches [120,121]. Interestingly, breast-feeding mothers with increased 158 97 bear different levels of invasiveness and side effects and do not plasma OT in response to a speech stressor that immediately fol- 159 98 have an equivalent informative value in terms of the underlying lowed baby-holding were found to have lower blood pressure than 160 99 central nervous mechanisms of the peptides. mothers with a decrease in OT after stress [103]. Furthermore, non- 161 100 Whereas the assessment and interpretation of urine or saliva postpartum healthy women who showed increased plasma OT lev- 162 101 measures provide inconsistent findings and need further investiga- els in response to positive emotion and massage and who main- 163 102 tion [9,29,51,79,157], CSF levels of OT or AVP are accompanied by tained OT levels during negative emotion were less likely to 164 103 high invasiveness. Besides the endogenous stimulation of OT during report interpersonal problems associated with intrusiveness 165 104 breast-feeding and positive physical contact, leading to attenuated [146]. Maintaining OT levels during sadness has also been associ- 166 105 endocrine responses to stress in women [3,38,66,70,72,103,146], ated with lower anxiety in close relationships [146]. Recently, Dit- 167 106 studies in humans have also been carried out with exogenous zen and colleagues [38] showed that women receiving 168 107 administration of OT and AVP. Intravenous OT infusion has been standardized physical contact from their partner (neck and shoul- 169 108 shown to induce significant behavioral effects [76,77], but it appears der massage) before stress exposure exhibited significantly lower 170 109 that only a small fraction of the neuropeptide passes the blood-brain cortisol and heart rate responses to stress compared with women 171 110 barrier [87], and possible side effects are more likely following intra- who received verbal social support or no social interaction from 172 111 venous infusion of neuropeptides. the partner. Another study by Holt-Lunstad and colleagues com- 173 112 Recent neuropharmacological research has shown that neuro- pared a warm touch intervention in couples with a monitoring- 174 113 peptides gain access to the human brain after intranasal adminis- only control groupPROOF[78]. Touch resulted in increased salivary OT 175 114 tration [18,41,66,129], providing a useful method for studying and a subsequent reduction in sympathetic tone indicated by low- 176 115 the central nervous effects of OT and AVP in humans [68]. In par- er systolic blood pressure as well as reduced alpha amylase. Alto- 177 116 ticular, a potential clinical use is dependent on a more direct and gether, these results from human studies suggest a possible 178 117 secure pathway to the human brain. In addition, a neurogenetic ap- protective effect of endogenous OT stimulation. 179 118 proach provides new insight into the individual variation of social Within this context, however, it should be noted that there are a 180 119 behavior and can easily be combined with behavioral measures variety of confounding factors, in particular the release of other 181 120 and functional imaging [92,113].
Recommended publications
  • Strategies for Social Skills for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder
    National Association of Special Education Teachers AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES Strategies for Social Skills for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder Introduction One of the most important areas for intervention for children with autism will be in the area of social skills development. Most students with ASD would like to be part of the social world around them. They have a need to interact socially and be involved with others. However, one of the defining characteristics of ASD is impairment in social interactions and social skills. Students with ASD have not automatically learned the rules of interaction with others, and they are unable to follow these unwritten rules of social behavior. This article will focus on this very important and relevant issue. Most students with ASD would like to be part of the social world around them. They have a need to interact socially and be involved with others. However, one of the defining characteristics of ASD is impairment in social interactions and social skills. Students with ASD have not automatically learned the rules of interaction with others, and they are unable to follow these unwritten rules of social behavior. Many people with ASD are operating on false perceptions that are rigid or overly literal. Recognizing these false perceptions can be very helpful in understanding the behavior and needs of these students in social situations. The misperceptions include: • rules apply in only a single situation • everything someone says must be true • when you do not know what to do, do nothing. Imagine how overly literal misconceptions could seriously limit social interaction.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emotional and Social Intelligences of Effective Leadership
    The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0268-3946.htm The intelligences The emotional and social of effective intelligences of effective leadership leadership An emotional and social skill approach 169 Ronald E. Riggio and Rebecca J. Reichard Kravis Leadership Institute, Claremont McKenna College, Claremont, California, USA Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to describe a framework for conceptualizing the role of emotional and social skills in effective leadership and management and provides preliminary suggestions for research and for the development of leader emotional and social skills. Design/methodology/approach – The paper generalizes a dyadic communications framework in order to describe the process of emotional and social exchanges between leaders and their followers. Findings – The paper shows how emotional skills and complementary social skills are essential for effective leadership through a literature review and discussion of ongoing research and a research agenda. Practical implications – Suggestions for the measurement and development of emotional and social skills for leaders and managers are offered. Originality/value – The work provides a framework for emotional and social skills in order to illustrate their role in leadership and their relationship to emotional and social intelligences. It outlines a research agenda and advances thinking of the role of developable emotional and social skills for managers. Keywords Emotional intelligence, Social skills, Leadership development Paper type Conceptual paper In his classic work on managerial skills, Mintzberg (1973) listed specific interpersonal skills (i.e. the ability to establish and maintain social networks; the ability to deal with subordinates; the ability to empathize with top-level leaders) as critical for managerial effectiveness.
    [Show full text]
  • Toward an Integrative Understanding of Social Behavior: New Models and New Opportunities
    REVIEW ARTICLE published: 28 June 2010 BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00034 Toward an integrative understanding of social behavior: new models and new opportunities Daniel T. Blumstein1*, Luis A. Ebensperger 2, Loren D. Hayes 3*, Rodrigo A. Vásquez 4, Todd H. Ahern 5, Joseph Robert Burger 3†, Adam G. Dolezal 6, Andy Dosmann7, Gabriela González-Mariscal 8, Breanna N. Harris 9, Emilio A. Herrera10, Eileen A. Lacey11, Jill Mateo 7, Lisa A. McGraw12, Daniel Olazábal 13, Marilyn Ramenofsky14, Dustin R. Rubenstein15, Samuel A. Sakhai16, Wendy Saltzman 9, Cristina Sainz-Borgo10, Mauricio Soto-Gamboa17, Monica L. Stewart 3, Tina W. Wey1, John C. Wingfield14 and Larry J. Young5 1 Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA 2 CASEB and Departamento de Ecología, Facultad de Ciencias Biológicas, P. Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile 3 Department of Biology, University of Louisiana at Monroe, Monroe, LA, USA 4 Institute of Ecology and Biodiversity, Departamento de Ciencias Ecologicas, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile 5 Department of Psychiatry, Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, Yerkes National Primate Center, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA 6 School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, USA 7 Committee on Evolutionary Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA 8 Centro de Investigacion en Reproduccion Animal, Centro de Investigación y de Estudios Avanzados del Instituto Politécnico Nacional, Universidad
    [Show full text]
  • Social Psychology Glossary
    Social Psychology Glossary This glossary defines many of the key terms used in class lectures and assigned readings. A Altruism—A motive to increase another's welfare without conscious regard for one's own self-interest. Availability Heuristic—A cognitive rule, or mental shortcut, in which we judge how likely something is by how easy it is to think of cases. Attractiveness—Having qualities that appeal to an audience. An appealing communicator (often someone similar to the audience) is most persuasive on matters of subjective preference. Attribution Theory—A theory about how people explain the causes of behavior—for example, by attributing it either to "internal" dispositions (e.g., enduring traits, motives, values, and attitudes) or to "external" situations. Automatic Processing—"Implicit" thinking that tends to be effortless, habitual, and done without awareness. B Behavioral Confirmation—A type of self-fulfilling prophecy in which people's social expectations lead them to behave in ways that cause others to confirm their expectations. Belief Perseverance—Persistence of a belief even when the original basis for it has been discredited. Bystander Effect—The tendency for people to be less likely to help someone in need when other people are present than when they are the only person there. Also known as bystander inhibition. C Catharsis—Emotional release. The catharsis theory of aggression is that people's aggressive drive is reduced when they "release" aggressive energy, either by acting aggressively or by fantasizing about aggression. Central Route to Persuasion—Occurs when people are convinced on the basis of facts, statistics, logic, and other types of evidence that support a particular position.
    [Show full text]
  • Investigating the Framing Effect in Social and Behavioral Science
    SOCIAL BEHAVIOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE ISSN 2474-8927 http://dx.doi.org/10.17140/SBRPOJ-1-106 Open Journal Opinion Investigating the Framing Effect in Social *Corresponding author and Behavioral Science Research: Joseph E. Trimble, PhD Professor Potential Influences on Behavior, Cognition Department of Psychology Western Washington University and Emotion Bellingham, WA 98225, USA E-mail: [email protected] * Volume 1 : Issue 1 Kamran Hughes, BS, BA; Joshua Thompson, BS, BA; Joseph E. Trimble, PhD Article Ref. #: 1000SBRPOJ1106 Department of Psychology, Western Washington University, Bellingham, WA 98225, USA Article History Received: August 19th, 2016 Accepted: September 29th, 2016 The framing effect has far-reaching implications for our understanding of social psy- Published: September 30th, 2016 chology and intergroup behavior. In recent decades, the effect garnered considerable attention in the fields of psychology, political science, and communication studies. Whether the effect is demonstrated by repetitious news stories1 or in voting behavior,2 framing matters. It matters Citation for both theoretical and pragmatic reasons. We will make connections between framing and Hughes K, Thompson J, Trimble JE. Investigating the framing effect in so- politics as a way of illustrating the real world applicability of this effect. The practical relevance cial and behavioral science research: of the framing effect is why effectively researching it is so crucial. The purpose of this paper is Potential influences on behavior, cog- to propose ways of improving framing research practices. To begin, we will define the framing nition and emotion. Soc Behav Res effect and provide some germane examples in order to clarify the concept. Pract Open J.
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Mental Health Disorder Fact Sheet for the Classroom
    1 Children’s Mental Health Disorder Fact Sheet for the Classroom1 Disorder Symptoms or Behaviors About the Disorder Educational Implications Instructional Strategies and Classroom Accommodations Anxiety Frequent Absences All children feel anxious at times. Many feel stress, for example, when Students are easily frustrated and may Allow students to contract a flexible deadline for Refusal to join in social activities separated from parents; others fear the dark. Some though suffer enough have difficulty completing work. They worrisome assignments. Isolating behavior to interfere with their daily activities. Anxious students may lose friends may suffer from perfectionism and take Have the student check with the teacher or have the teacher Many physical complaints and be left out of social activities. Because they are quiet and compliant, much longer to complete work. Or they check with the student to make sure that assignments have Excessive worry about homework/grades the signs are often missed. They commonly experience academic failure may simply refuse to begin out of fear been written down correctly. Many teachers will choose to Frequent bouts of tears and low self-esteem. that they won’t be able to do anything initial an assignment notebook to indicate that information Fear of new situations right. Their fears of being embarrassed, is correct. Drug or alcohol abuse As many as 1 in 10 young people suffer from an AD. About 50% with humiliated, or failing may result in Consider modifying or adapting the curriculum to better AD also have a second AD or other behavioral disorder (e.g. school avoidance. Getting behind in their suit the student’s learning style-this may lessen his/her depression).
    [Show full text]
  • Social Neuroscience and Its Relationship to Social Psychology
    Social Cognition, Vol. 28, No. 6, 2010, pp. 675–685 CACIOPPO ET AL. SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE AND ITS RELATIONSHIP TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY John T. Cacioppo University of Chicago Gary G. Berntson Ohio State University Jean Decety University of Chicago Social species create emergent organizations beyond the individual. These emergent structures evolved hand in hand with neural, hormonal, cel- lular, and genetic mechanisms to support them because the consequent social behaviors helped these organisms survive, reproduce, and care for offspring suf!ciently long that they too reproduced. Social neuroscience seeks to specify the neural, hormonal, cellular, and genetic mechanisms underlying social behavior, and in so doing to understand the associations and in"uences between social and biological levels of organization. Suc- cess in the !eld, therefore, is not measured in terms of the contributions to social psychology per se, but rather in terms of the speci!cation of the biological mechanisms underlying social interactions and behavior—one of the major problems for the neurosciences to address in the 21st century. Social species, by definition, create emergent organizations beyond the individ- ual—structures ranging from dyads and families to groups and cultures. These emergent social structures evolved hand in hand with neural, hormonal, cellular, and genetic mechanisms to support them because the consequent social behaviors helped these organisms survive, reproduce and, in the case of some social spe- cies, care for offspring sufficiently long that they too reproduced thereby ensuring their genetic legacy. Social neuroscience is the interdisciplinary field devoted to the study of these neural, hormonal, cellular, and genetic mechanisms and, relat- edly, to the study of the associations and influences between social and biological Preparation of this manuscript was supported by NIA Grant No.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review and Reconceptualization of Social Aggression: Adaptive and Maladaptive Correlates
    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev DOI 10.1007/s10567-008-0037-9 A Review and Reconceptualization of Social Aggression: Adaptive and Maladaptive Correlates Nicole Heilbron Æ Mitchell J. Prinstein Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008 Abstract The emergence of a research literature explor- decades, a considerable empirical literature has demon- ing parallels between physical and nonphysical (i.e., social, strated associations between physical aggression and relational, indirect) forms of aggression has raised many various indices of social incompetence, including social- questions about the developmental effects of aggressive cognitive biases, social skill deficits, and peer rejection behavior on psychological functioning, peer relationships, (e.g., Dodge 1983; Lochman and Dodge 1998). Numerous and social status. Although both forms of aggression have longitudinal studies have identified childhood physical been linked to problematic outcomes in childhood and aggression as a risk factor for a variety of deleterious adolescence, more recent findings have highlighted the outcomes, including future delinquency, criminal activity, importance of considering the possible social rewards school dropout, and substance use during adolescence and conferred by socially aggressive behavior. This paper adulthood (e.g., Broidy et al. 2003; Nagin and Tremblay examines relevant theory and empirical research investi- 1999; Patterson et al. 1991). Given that treatment of anti- gating the adaptive and maladaptive correlates specific to social and aggressive behavior represents a primary cause nonphysical forms of aggression. Findings are explored at of referral to inpatient and outpatient clinics for adoles- the level of group (e.g., peer rejection), dyadic (e.g., cents, knowledge of the adverse correlates and friendship quality), and individual (e.g., depressive symp- consequences of aggression is critical for informing toms) variables.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and Social Behavior 1,2
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Culture and social behavior 1,2 Joseph Henrich Comparative research from diverse societies shows that with nearly everything in-between. In some populations, human social behavior varies immensely across a broad range contributions declined as people played. In others, they of domains, including cooperation, fairness, trust, punishment, did not. Then, when opportunities for participants to pay aggressiveness, morality and competitiveness. Efforts to to punish other players were added to the basic game explain this global variation have increasingly pointed to the setup, the diversity across groups increased even more. importance of packages of social norms, or institutions. This Contributions in the first round now ranged from roughly work suggests that institutions related to anonymous markets, 30% in Istanbul, Riyadh and Athens to nearly 80% in moralizing religions, monogamous marriage and complex Boston, Copenhagen and St. Gallen (Switzerland). Most kinship systems fundamentally shape human psychology and striking was that, unlike the usual experiments among behavior. To better tackle this, work on cultural evolution and Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic culture-gene coevolution delivers the tools and approaches to (WEIRD) populations [14] where opportunities to punish develop theories to explain these psychological and behavioral result in the sanctioning of non-cooperators and in high patterns, and to understand their relationship to culture and rates of cooperation, the addition of punishment oppor- human nature. tunities made things worse in several places. In these Addresses places, participants punished not only low contributors 1 University of British Columbia, Department of Psychology, Canada but also high contributors, which stifled any increase in 2 University of British Columbia, Department of Economics, Canada the overall contributions.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: TRAIT SOCIAL ANXIETY 1
    Running head: TRAIT SOCIAL ANXIETY 1 Trait Social Anxiety as a Conditional Adaptation: A Developmental and Evolutionary Framework Tara A. Karasewich and Valerie A. Kuhlmeier Department of Psychology, Queen’s University Accepted for publication, Developmental Review CORRESPONDING AUTHOR: Tara Karasewich Department of Psychology, Queen’s University Kingston, ON K7L 3N6 Canada E-mail: [email protected] © 2019. This manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ TRAIT SOCIAL ANXIETY 2 Abstract Individuals with trait social anxiety are disposed to be wary of others. Although feeling social anxiety is unpleasant, evolutionary psychologists consider it to be an adaptation. In current models, social anxiety is described as functioning to have helped our prehistoric ancestors avoid social threat by warning individuals when their interactions with other group members were likely to be negative and motivating them to act in ways to prevent conflict or limit its damage. Thus, trait social anxiety is thought to have evolved in our species because it allowed our ancestors to preserve their relationships and maintain their positions in social hierarchies. While we agree with this conclusion drawn by existing evolutionary models, we believe that there is an important element missing in these explanations: the role that individual development has played in the evolution of trait social anxiety. We propose a new model, which argues for trait social anxiety to be considered a conditional adaptation; that is, the trait should develop as a response to cues in the early childhood environment in order to prepare individuals to face social threat in adulthood.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: a Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential
    W&M ScholarWorks Arts & Sciences Book Chapters Arts and Sciences 2007 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential. Bruce D. Bartholow Cheryl L. Dickter College of William and Mary, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons Recommended Citation Bartholow, B. D., & Dickter, C. L. (2007). Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential.. E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Ed.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 376-400). New York, NY: Guilford Press. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters/46 This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Arts and Sciences at W&M ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Arts & Sciences Book Chapters by an authorized administrator of W&M ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. V PERSON PERCEPTION, STEREOTYPING, AND PREJUDICE Copyright © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. All rights reserved. © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. Copyright 17 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception A SELECTIVE REVIEW FOCUSED ON THE EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIAL Bruce D. Bartholow and Cheryl L. Dickter Although social psychologists have long been interested in understanding the cognitive processes underlying social phenomena (e.g., Markus & Zajonc, 1985), their methods for studying them traditionally have been rather limited. Early research in person perception, like most other areas of social-psychological inquiry, relied primarily on verbal reports. Researchers using this approach have devised clever experimental designs to ensure the validity of their conclusions concerning social behavior (see Reis & Judd, 2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Social Skills Versus Skilled Social Behavior: a Problematic Distinction in Autism Spectrum Disorders
    JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005 Social Skills versus Skilled Social Behavior: A Problematic Distinction in Autism Spectrum Disorders Raymond G. Romanczyk, Ph.D., BCBA, Sara White, Ph.D., BCBA, and Jennifer M. Gillis, MA., BCBA Abstract The primary core deficit in autism spectrum disorders is social development. While frequently acknowledged as a critical aspect of intervention for improved functioning, research on the specifics of social development and effective models of intervention is not commensurate with the acknowledged importance. We present a model of social competence that is derived from a behavioral molecular analysis approach, but which is presented in a broad ecological context. We distinguish social skills from skilled social behavior and present specific clinical examples that are derived from the model. KEYWORDS: Skills training, social competence, model development. Introduction From the earliest writings of Leo Kanner concerning autism, the primary focus was on social development. In 1943 he wrote "The outstanding pathognomonic fundamental disorder is the children's inability to relate themselves in the ordinary way to people and situations from the beginning of life." This emphasis on social interaction development clearly persists through current diagnostic definitions (DSM-IV TR), in which, even under the criteria for communication impairment, one sees emphasis on the social aspects of communication. This is in addition to the primary category of “Qualitative impairment in social interaction.” Even from the various perspectives of individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), family and caregivers, researchers, and service providers, the development of social skills for individuals with ASD is critical for positive outcome. Yet it remains under-emphasized in most educational settings, often a relative weakness in behavioral intervention programs, and poorly addressed for individuals with ASD (particularly adults) who participate in psychotherapy and ‘social skills groups’.
    [Show full text]