Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition 185

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition 185 Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition 185 CROSS-CULTURAL WORK IN MUSIC COGNITION: CHALLENGES, INSIGHTS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS NORI JACOBY GAVIN STEINGO Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt Princeton University am Main, Germany CATHERINE J. STEVENS ELIZABETH HELLMUTH MARGULIS Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia Princeton University LAUREL TRAINOR MARTIN CLAYTON McMaster University, Hamilton, Canada Durham University, Durham, United Kingdom SANDRA TREHUB ERIN HANNON University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada University of Nevada, Las Vegas MICHAEL VEAL HENKJAN HONING Yale University University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands MELANIE WALD-FUHRMANN JOHN IVERSEN Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt University of California, San Diego am Main, Germany TOBIAS ROBERT KLEIN MANY FOUNDATIONAL QUESTIONS IN THE Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany psychology of music require cross-cultural approaches, yet the vast majority of work in the field to date has SAMUEL A. MEHR been conducted with Western participants and Western Harvard University music. For cross-cultural research to thrive, it will require collaboration between people from different disciplinary LARA PEARSON backgrounds, as well as strategies for overcoming differ- Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt ences in assumptions, methods, and terminology. This am Main, Germany position paper surveys the current state of the field and offers a number of concrete recommendations focused ISABELLE PERETZ on issues involving ethics, empirical methods, and defi- University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada nitions of ‘‘music’’ and ‘‘culture.’’ MARC PERLMAN Received: June 10, 2019, accepted October 15, 2019. Brown University Key words: cross-cultural, music, psychology, ethno- musicology, methods RAINER POLAK Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany OST RESEARCH IN PSYCHOLOGY AND ANDREA RAVIGNANI neuroscience has been conducted on WEIRD Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium M participants—that is, individuals who hail from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and PATRICK E. SAVAGE Democratic societies (for an extensive review of this Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan issue in psychology see Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Music Perception, VOLUME 37, ISSUE 3, PP. 185–195, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312. © 2020 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS ’S REPRINTS AND PERMISSIONS WEB PAGE, HTTPS://WWW.UCPRESS.EDU/JOURNALS/REPRINTS-PERMISSIONS. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2020.37.3.185 186 Nori Jacoby et al. 2010). Music psychology shares this sampling bias: issues and recommendations for each. We hope that this participants are almost always recruited from WEIRD overview will prove useful to researchers planning societies, experimental materials are usually drawn empirical cross-cultural projects and will encourage from Western music, and studies tend to investigate interdisciplinary teams that bridge the sciences and constructs such as harmonic progressions that are dis- humanities. Although the recommendations described proportionately relevant to Western music. here are mainly focused on cross-cultural research, This situation gives rise to at least two fundamental many of them are also relevant to music research more problems. First, it biases the understanding of human broadly. All research on music could benefit from broad mechanisms for music perception and production, hypotheses that incorporate diverse ideas about how because WEIRD populations do not necessarily consti- music works and by exposing the limitations of a pro- tute a representative sample along multiple critical ject’s musical and methodological choices. dimensions ranging from basic visual and spatial per- ception to social cognition (Henrich et al., 2010). Sec- CONTEXT ond, important questions regarding the biological and Researchers are integrated into disciplinary subcultures cultural origins of music, its relation to language and with different assumptions and goals. These need to be other activities, and the diversity and commonality of rendered explicit to facilitate communication across dis- human music making require the kind of comparative ciplinary divides. Some issues raised by cross-cultural cross-cultural approaches already common in other music cognition research, and the possibilities and fields such as linguistics and anthropology. Addressing obstacles facing interdisciplinary engagement between these issues will require interdisciplinary collaboration music psychology and ethnomusicology, have been dis- among psychologists, neuroscientists, anthropologists, cussed over the last decade or so (e.g., Becker, 2009a, and (ethno)musicologists, as well as the incorporation 2009b; Clayton, Dueck, & Leante, 2013; Lawson, 2014; of methods from cross-cultural psychology into music Tolbert, 1992; Widdess, 2012), but much remains to be cognition research. resolved if we are to produce a sustained body of The goal of this paper is to consider some require- research in which both disciplines can be confident. ments as well as possible challenges and benefits occa- Our current interest in cross-cultural music cognition sioned by such collaborations. This document outlines might be said to recapitulate some aspects of compara- a list of recommendations that arose from an interdis- tive musicology of the late 19th and early 20th centuries, ciplinary conversation at the Max Planck Institute for a time when the approaches that would develop into Empirical Aesthetics in Frankfurt, Germany over the music psychology and ethnomusicology were deeply course of several days in October 2018. The meeting entwined (see review in Clayton, 2009). Comparative from which these recommendations emerged was not musicology emerged in the late 19th century and flour- designed to generate an exhaustive checklist or set of ished in the early 20th century. Inspired by Gestalt psy- guidelines for cross-cultural research in music, but chology, it was motivated by theories about the origins rather to serve as a departure point for further thought. and structure of music and explicitly aimed to uncover For example, a major limitation of the meeting was that universal trends and features of music (Hornbostel, it was comprised predominantly of white men and 1975; Nettl, 2015; Nettl & Bohlman, 1991; Sachs, women from elite Euro-American academic institu- 1962; Savage & Brown, 2013; Stumpf, 1911/2012). Com- tions. Given that representation was one of the key parative studies by researchers such as Carl Stumpf, issues raised, we debated the wisdom of publishing our Erich Moritz von Hornbostel, Charles Myers, Curt summary document before more workshops could be Sachs, and Alan Lomax raise complex ethical questions. held with a wider range of participants. We decided, On the one hand, such research attempted to assert the however, that there was merit in sharing our thoughts value of non-Western music that had previously been and stimulating discussion of these issues, while dismissed; on the other hand, their attempts to use acknowledging the necessity of further conversations Western scientific frameworks to do so have been crit- with more representative and diverse voices. icized as perpetuating ideologies of the imperial powers This paper begins with a brief description of the dis- (Clarke, 2014; Clayton, 2007, in press; Lomax, 1968; ciplinary context of cross-cultural and comparative Nettl & Bohlman, 1991; Savage, 2018). research on music, illuminating some of the sources of With the growing influence of North American cul- disciplinary tensions. It then explores four central tural anthropology, carefully detailed ethnographies of topics—1) music and musicality; 2) culture(s); 3) ethics; specific communities increasingly replaced comparative and 4) paradigms and methods—and outlines some key approaches. The Society for Ethnomusicology was Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition 187 founded in 1955 as an explicit break with the tradition music cognition with auditory cognition) is predicated of comparative musicology. In broad terms, ethnomu- on culturally based agreement negotiated within and sicology emphasized participant observation, and raised between groups, which changes over time. The term cultural relativism to the level of axiom (Nettl, 2015). encompasses a variety of concepts surrounding human The formation of the discipline in explicitly relativistic activities that may include structured sound (especially terms occurred in tandem with a wave of independence in terms of pitch height, pitch duration, timbre, and and anti-colonial movements across the globe. Begin- form), communicative meaning, rituals, and constitu- ning in the 1970s and continuing through the early tive body movements (singing, playing an instrument, 1990s, many disciplines in the social sciences and gesturing, clapping, dancing). humanities underwent a thorough self-critique by inves- Cross-culturally, conceptions of music vary greatly. tigating their epistemological and institutional origins in What is heard as music by an outside listener may not colonialism. This resulted in a tendency to emphasize function as such within the society that produces the ‘‘partial’’ (rather than ‘‘universal’’) knowledge, the par- sound; even different individuals within a society may ticular and the local (e.g.,
Recommended publications
  • ICMPC11 Schedule at a Glan
    Upadted 7/9/10 SCHEDULE AT A GLANCE 8/23-27/10 MONDAY 8/23 REGISTRATION REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby REGISTRATION - Kane Hall Lobby 8:00-9:00AM Kane 130 Kane 110 Gowen 301 Smith 120 KANE - Walker Ames Room Session Rooms Gowen 201 WELCOME/KEYNOTE ADDRESS: Welcome and Opening Singing: when it hurts, when it helps, Keynote 9-10:30AM and when it changes brains. Gottfried Schlaug BREAK: 10:30-11:00AM Break Break Break Break Break Break INVITED SYMPOSIUM: SESSION 1 Effects of Musical Experience on Development During MUSIC THERAPY 1 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 1 TONAL PERCEPTION 1 11-12:30 Infancy Laurel Trainor PA 021 Modeling Musical Structure from the Audience: Emergent PA027 The Effect of Structure and Rate Variation on Key-Finding SYM31:Beat Induction as a Fundamental Musical Skill PA 025 A Theory of Music and Sadness: A Role for Prolactin? 11:00 Rhythmic Models from Spontaneous Vocalizations in Samba Culture Morwaread Farbood, Gary Marcus, Panayotis Mavromatis, David Henkjan Honing David Huron Luiz Naveda, Fabien Gouyon, Marc Leman Poeppel SYM32: New Perspectives on Consonance and Dissonance PA 018 Improvisational Psychodynamic Music Therapy for PA110 Influences of Minority Status and Social Identity on the PA057 Common and Rare Musical Keys Are Absolutely Different: 11:30 Judy Plantinga, Sandra E. Trehub Depression: Randomized Controlled Trial Elaboration of Unfamiliar Music by Adolescents Implicit Absolute Pitch, Exposure
    [Show full text]
  • Classical Net Review
    The Internet's Premier Classical Music Source BOOK REVIEW The Psychology of Music Diana Deutsch, editor Academic Press, Third Edition, 2013, pp xvii + 765 ISBN-10: 012381460X ISBN-13: 978-0123814609 The psychology of music was first explored in detail in modern times in a book of that name by Carl E. Seashore… Psychology Of Music was published in 1919. Dover's paperback edition of almost 450 pages (ISBN- 10: 0486218511; ISBN-13: 978-0486218519) is still in print from half a century later (1967) and remains a good starting point for those wishing to understand the relationship between our minds and music, chiefly as a series of physical processes. From the last quarter of the twentieth century onwards much research and many theories have changed the models we have of the mind when listening to or playing music. Changes in music itself, of course, have dictated that the nature of human interaction with it has grown. Unsurprisingly, books covering the subject have proliferated too. These range from examinations of how memory affects our experience of music through various forms of mental disabilities, therapies and deviations from "standard" auditory reception, to attempts to explain music appreciation psychologically. Donald Hodges' and David Conrad Sebald's Music in the Human Experience: An Introduction to Music Psychology (ISBN-10: 0415881862; ISBN-13: 978- 0415881869) makes a good introduction to the subject; while Aniruddh Patel's Music, Language, and the Brain (ISBN-10: 0199755302; ISBN-13: 978-0199755301) is a good (and now classic/reference) overview. Oliver Sacks' Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain (ISBN-10: 1400033535; ISBN-13: 978-1400033539) examines specific areas from a clinical perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • Pad and Sad: Two Awareness-Weighted Rhythmic Similarity Distances
    PAD AND SAD: TWO AWARENESS-WEIGHTED RHYTHMIC SIMILARITY DISTANCES Daniel Gomez-Mar´ ´ın Sergi Jorda` Perfecto Herrera Universitat Pompeu Fabra Universitat Pompeu Fabra Universitat Pompeu Fabra [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT ties and shorter time-scales to determine similarity. In this paper we address the above-mentioned gap and propose Measuring rhythm similarity is relevant for the analysis two rhythm similarity distances that refine those currently and generation of music. Existing similarity metrics tend available (and probably rougher than desirable). The pro- to consider our perception of rhythms as being in time posed distances have been derived from music cognition without discriminating the importance of some regions knowledge and have been tuned using experiments involv- over others. In a previously reported experiment we ob- ing human listeners. We additionally show that they can served that measures of similarity may differ given the be adapted to work (at least) in a music-loop collection presence or absence of a pulse inducing sound and the im- organization context, where music creators want to orga- portance of those measures is not constant along the pat- nize their building blocks in rhythm-contrasting or rhythm tern. These results are now reinterpreted by refining the flowing ways where similarity would provide the criterion previously proposed metrics. We consider that the percep- for such concatenation of elements. tual contribution of each beat to the measured similarity Previous work has used rhythmic descriptors, computed is non-homogeneous but might indeed depend on the tem- from audio signals, to analyze song databases.
    [Show full text]
  • Honing Def.Indd
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The illiterate listener on music cognition, musicality and methodology Honing, H. DOI 10.26530/OAPEN_480090 Publication date 2011 Document Version Final published version Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Honing, H. (2011). The illiterate listener: on music cognition, musicality and methodology. Vossiuspers UvA. https://doi.org/10.26530/OAPEN_480090 General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:01 Oct 2021 henkjan honing The Illiterate Lisener On Music Cognition, Musicality and Methodology The Illiterate Listener The Illiterate Listener On Music Cognition, Musicality and Methodology Henkjan Honing Vossiuspers UvA is an imprint of Amsterdam University Press. This edition is established under the auspices of the Universiteit van Amsterdam. Cover design: Geert de Koning, Ten Post Lay-out: JAPES, Amsterdam English translation by Jonathan Reeder ISBN e-ISBN © Henkjan Honing/Vossiuspers UvA, Amsterdam, All rights reserved.
    [Show full text]
  • Music and Language Comprehension in the Brain
    Music and Language Comprehension in the Brain © Richard Kunert 2017 Cover design: Jorrit Kiel ISBN 978-90-76203-79-9 Printed and bound by Ipskamp Drukkers b.v. The research reported in this thesis was supported by a PhD grant by the Max Planck society (Max-Planck-Gesellschaft) awarded to Richard Kunert and a Spinoza Prize by the Dutch science organization (Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek) awarded to Peter Hagoort. Music and Language Comprehension in the Brain Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. dr. J.H.J.M. van Krieken, volgens besluit van het college van decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 10 februari 2017 om 12:30 uur precies door Richard Kunert geboren op 3 december 1985 te Halberstadt (Duitsland) Promotor: Prof. Peter Hagoort Copromotor: Dr. Roel M. Willems Manuscriptcommissie: Prof. James McQueen (Radboud Universiteit) Prof. Sonja A. Kotz (Universiteit Maastricht) Prof. Henkjan Honing (Universiteit van Amsterdam) Table of Contents Foreword 7 Chapter 1 Introduction 9 Chapter 2 Language influences music harmony 31 perception: effects of shared syntactic integration resources beyond attention Chapter 3 Music and Language Syntax Interact in 97 Broca’s Area: An fMRI Study Chapter 4 When do Music Harmony and Language 121 Syntax Processing Interact? An MEG Study Chapter 5 Structural processing of music and 139 language: imaging domain-specific neural resources Chapter 6 An independent psychometric evaluation 171 of the PROMS measure of music perception skills Chapter 7 General discussion and conclusions 193 References 203 Samenvatting 219 Zusammenfassung 221 Acknowledgements 223 Curriculum Vitae 227 Publications 229 MPI Series in 231 Psycholinguistics Foreword Foreword Many a days during my PhD I asked myself why on earth I am doing a PhD on music and language.
    [Show full text]
  • Henkjan Honing Interview Switching on the Radio to Listen to Silky-Voiced
    Henkjan Honing interview Switching on the radio to listen to silky-voiced DJs introducing auto-tuned top- 40 music by perfectly preened pop stars with perfume lines or watching tonally challenged innocents with stars in their eyes being led to the slaughter in the cacophonous contemporary TV coliseum that is X Factor, one could be mistaken for thinking that a large percentage of human beings are far from musical. However, according to Henkjan Honing, professor of Cognitive and Computational Musicology at the University of Amsterdam (UvA), we are all musical creatures and this sets humans apart from the other creatures roaming this planet.* Music Cognition is a relatively new field of study, having risen in prominence in the last 20 years. It uses an interdisciplinary approach, combining fields ranging from computer science and psychology to chemistry and medicine, in its quest to understand the cognitive processes that underpin musical behaviour, such as perception, comprehension, memory, attention and performance. Honing gave a highly entertaining presentation in the Glass House of Science and Scholarship on 3 September 2012, an event which saw UvA professors deliver lectures to the general public in the centre of Amsterdam. Spectators were treated to videos of apes playing percussion, parakeets dancing in time to the Backstreet Boys and babies wired up with electrodes so that they resembled tiny Russian cosmonauts (although Honing was quick to assure us they were not harmed or in any discomfort). In this interview, Prof. Honing gives more insight into his innovative research, which has attracted worldwide media attention and explains the science behind man’s innate musicality and how it can be harnessed for the common good.
    [Show full text]
  • Border Crossings: a Commentary on Henkjan Honing's "On the Growing Role of Observation, Formalization and Experimental Method in Musicology"
    Empirical Musicology Review Vol. 1, No. 1, 2006 Border Crossings: A Commentary on Henkjan Honing's "On the Growing Role of Observation, Formalization and Experimental Method in Musicology" NICHOLAS COOK Royal Holloway, University of London ABSTRACT: In the early twentieth century systematic musicology, which was based on the comparative method, played a prominent role in the discipline: however it was appropriated by the Nazis and fell out of favour after the war. It was replaced by ethnomusicology and structuralist music theory, both of which emphasized the individual context (cultural or structural) and eschewed comparison between contexts. Both also developed an epistemology based on the generation of meaning through the act of "experiencing and understanding music" (Titon 1997: 87): this epistemology, characteristic of cultural musicology and theory (CMT) in general, is quite distinct from that of the cognitive sciences of music (CSM). The otherwise confusing variety of musicological practices subsumed under the category "systematic musicology", as set out in Honing's article on which this is a commentary, can be usefully seen in terms of two distinct dimensions, those of method and of epistemology. It follows from this that empirical methods are as consistent with, and as potentially valuable to, CMT as they are to CSM, and that EMR has the potential to reach both constituencies. Submitted 2005 November 14; accepted 2005 November 30. KEYWORDS: systematic, musicology, method, epistemology ONE of the good things about a new journal like Empirical Musicology Review is that it stimulates efforts to define, or redefine, the field. In his article "On the growing role of observation, formalization and experimental method in musicology" Henkjan Honing situates "empirical" musicology within the context of "computational" and "cognitive" musicology: these are the three main headings into which his article falls, and he links them to observational, formalized, and experimental approaches to the study of music.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cognitive Approach to Medieval Mode
    Empirical Musicology Review Vol. 5, No. 4, 2010 Hooked on Music Information Retrieval W. BAS DE HAAS [1] Utrecht University FRANS WIERING Utrecht University ABSTRACT: This article provides a reply to „Lure(d) into listening: The potential of cognition-based music information retrieval,‟ in which Henkjan Honing discusses the potential impact of his proposed Listen, Lure & Locate project on Music Information Retrieval (MIR). Honing presents some critical remarks on data-oriented approaches in MIR, which we endorse. To place these remarks in context, we first give a brief overview of the state of the art of MIR research. Then we present a series of arguments that show why purely data-oriented approaches are unlikely to take MIR research and applications to a more advanced level. Next, we propose our view on MIR research, in which the modelling of musical knowledge has a central role. Finally, we elaborate on the ideas in Honing‟s paper from a MIR perspective in this paper and propose some additions to the Listen, Lure & Locate project. Submitted 2010 November 12; accepted 2010 November 17. KEYWORDS: music information retrieval, music cognition, musical data, hook, tagging INTRODUCTION FROM its shady beginnings in the 1960s, Music Information Retrieval (MIR) has evolved into a flourishing “multidisciplinary research endeavor that strives to develop innovative content-based searching schemes, novel interfaces, and evolving networked delivery mechanisms in an effort to make the world‟s vast store of music accessible to all” (Downie, 2004, p. 12). This store is indeed vast, especially in the digital domain. Reportedly, iTunes has over 13,000,000 songs for sale [2].
    [Show full text]
  • “Always Already Technological”: New Views of Music and the Human In
    Loughridge, Deirdre. 2021. “Always “Always Already Technological”: Already Technological”: New Views of Music and the Human in Musicology New Views of Music and the Human in and the Cognitive Sciences.” Music Research Annual 2: 1–22. Musicology and the Cognitive Sciences ISSN 2563-7290 deirdre loughridge ABSTRACT: The relationship between nature and culture has been a traditional battleground for the humanities and the sciences. Now, however, new compatibilities and tensions are emerging between the fields of musicology and music psychology, neuroscience, and cognition, spurred by the incorporation of “technology” into conceptions of “the human.” This essay aims to surface and compare changing assumptions about technology in relation to music and the human across these fields. It surveys musicological attention to technology and how this attention has segued into posthumanist paradigms, and it charts the rise of biotechnological conceptions of human nature in music cognition and neuroscience. It finds that while posthumanist and biotechnological frameworks share the premise of an “always already technological” human, and with that an interest in how biology and culture work together, the fields have drawn different implications from this new vision. Musicology and music sciences may both benefit by taking stock of the shifts in conceptualizing the human in each discipline, reflecting on what discrepancies between them reveal about the benefits and limitations of each perspective, and considering anew the possibilities for dialogue and collaboration. KEYWORDS: music cognition, music and neuroscience, music and technology, musicology, music science, posthumanism, psychology of music, neurohumanities, vocal music DEIRDRE LOUGHRIDGE is an Associate Professor in the Department of Music at Northeastern University.
    [Show full text]
  • The Benefits of Musical Creation: Improving Education and Encouraging Innovation Martin Guerpin, Jonathan Goldman
    The Benefits of Musical Creation: Improving Education and Encouraging Innovation Martin Guerpin, Jonathan Goldman To cite this version: Martin Guerpin, Jonathan Goldman. The Benefits of Musical Creation: Improving Education and Encouraging Innovation. [Research Report] Réseau canadien pour les musiques nouvelles - Canadian New Music Network 2014. halshs-01560789 HAL Id: halshs-01560789 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01560789 Submitted on 12 Jul 2017 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution| 4.0 International License Led by Tim Brady, the Digital Content Initiative (DCI) recently commissioned a research document in order to give academic and scientific support to their arguments as to the value of “specialized” music. This document includes an analysis and bibliography of a wide range of studies that have been published on the importance and benefits of music and creativity. CNMN members are welcome to freely use this document, and these arguments, when helping to support the cause of creative new music in Canada. The Benefits of Musical Creation: Improving Education and Encouraging Innovation Martin Guerpin (Université Paris-Sorbonne/ Université de Montréal/ OICRM) et Jonathan Goldman (Université de Montréal/ OICRM) December 2014 Discussion A number of recent studies (see Bibliography below) have demonstrated the value and benefits which can be generated by initiatives supporting musical creation.
    [Show full text]
  • Semiótica, Semiótica De La Música Y Semiótica Cognitivo- Enactiva De La Música Notas Para Un Manual De Usuario
    Semiótica, semiótica de la música y semiótica cognitivo- enactiva de la música Notas para un manual de usuario. Rubén López Cano [email protected], www.lopezcano.net Escola Superior de Música de Catalunya Rubén López Cano 2007 Los contenidos de este texto están bajo una licencia Creative Commons. Consúltela antes de usarlo. The content on this text is under a Creative Commons license. Consult it before using this article. Cómo citar este artículo: How to cite this article: López Cano, Rubén. 2007. “Semiótica, semiótica de la música y semiótica cognitivo-enactiva de la música. Notas para un manual de usuario”. Texto didáctico (actualizado junio 2007). www.lopezcano.net (Consultado o descargado [día, mes y año]) (Accessed [Day Month Year of access]) Semiótica de la música Rubén López Cano Índice: • Advertencia • Semiótica de la Música • El Signo • ¿Qué es significado en música? • Fundamentos • Saussure y Peirce • Semiótica cognitiva de la música • ¿Semiología o semiótica? • Persuasiones y disuasiones: oferta y problemas de la semiótica de la música • Las ciencias cognitivas • La musicología cognitiva • La cognición enactiva • Como introducirse en el estudio de esto… o Semiótica de la música Algunos autores importantes en el ámbito de la semiótica de la música: Primer encontronazo con la semiótica Introducción a la semiótica general Semiótica de Peirce Semióticas particulares: Eco y Greimas Historia de la semiótica musical Introducción a la semiótica de la música Bibliografia fundamental de algunos maestros de la semiótica musical
    [Show full text]
  • For Infants, Musical Exposure Is Simple and Complex
    THIS STORY HAS BEEN FORMATTED FOR EASY PRINTING Can't get it out of my head A father's yearlong quest to grasp the infant musical mind By Jeremy Eichler, Globe Staff | July 13, 2008 I've never felt so paralyzed standing before my CD collection as the day I brought my newborn son home from the hospital and decided to play him his very first music. So much was at stake. Should it be modern or Baroque? Orchestral or opera? Would Mozart make him smarter? Would Schoenberg instill in him revolutionary tendencies? Would Wagner make him loathe his Jewish roots? I settled on Bach's "Art of Fugue" in an arrangement for string quartet. Why not begin at the summit, and what's more, I imagined, all that searching counterpoint would be like honey for the infant brain. He responded with aplomb, conveying his wise, wordless mastery of the material by slipping into an eyes-closed, meditative state. OK, he fell asleep. But my yearlong quest to understand the infant musical mind had begun. As it turns out, my timing was good, as the cognitive and neuroscience research on music has been exploding these days, driven by techno logical breakthroughs in brain imaging and a newly widespread openness toward music as a legitimate field of scientific study. It's hard to miss the reverberations. Keith Lockhart has been outfitted with sensors on the podium of Symphony Hall; Oliver Sacks's "Musicophilia" has brought strange tales of musical obsession to the bestseller list; the journal Nature has been running a nine-part essay series on the science of music; and a conference this weekend at Tufts University is convening more than 100 researchers from 13 countries to discuss the subject of "Music, Language, and the Mind." The art form that Claude Lévi-Strauss once dubbed "the supreme mystery of the science of man" is, one note at a time, becoming less A child being studied at the Auditory Development Lab at McMaster University, where a study indicated that the way adults bounce babies affects whether the kids prefer marches or waltzes.
    [Show full text]