Taste Processing: Insights from Animal Models
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
12 Recipes That Will Change the Way You Cook Make Bold, Fresh Food the Milk Street Way
12 Recipes that Will Change the Way You Cook Make bold, fresh food the Milk Street way CHRISTOPHER KIMBALL’S ◆ THE NEW HOME COOKING SPECIAL EDITION ◆ ◆ Special Edition Christopher Kimball’s MILK STREET Magazine The New Home Cooking ◆ RECIPE INDEX No-Sear Lamb or Beef and Chickpea Stew Page 2 Fluffy Olive Oil Scrambled Eggs Page 4 Chinese Chili and Scallion Noodles Page 5 Chinese White-Cooked Chicken Page 6 Cacio e Pepe, Gricia and Carbonara Page 8 Pinchos Morunos Page 12 Roasted Cauliflower With Tahini Page 13 Charred Brussels Sprouts Page 14 Red Lentil Soup with Spinach Page 15 Tahini Swirl Brownies Page 16 Israeli Hummus Page 17 Stovetop Chocolate Cake Page 21 Front Cover Photo: Joyelle West; Styling: Christine Tobin Back Cover Photo: Noam Moskowitz Christopher Kimball’s Milk Street in downtown Boston—at 177 Milk Street—is home how we cook by searching the world for bold, simple recipes and techniques that to our editorial offices and cooking school. It is also where we recordChristopher are adapted and tested for home cooks everywhere. For more information, go to Kimball’s Milk Street television and radio shows. Milk Street is devoted to changing 177MilkStreet.com. 12 Recipes That Will Change the Way You Cook [ EDITOR’S NOTE] C hristopher K imball One for Life, One for Love, President and Founder One for Death Christopher Kimball Media Director and Co-Founder Melissa Baldino Editorial Director - J.M. Hirsch recently dined at Sichuan peppercorns and the Food Editor - Matthew Card Art Director - Jennifer Baldino Cox la Grenouille in spicy white pepper so popular Managing Editor - Jenn Ladd Books & Special Editions Editor - Michelle Locke New York—the last of in Asian cooking. -
Impaired Latent Inhibition in GDNF-Deficient Mice Exposed To
ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 10 October 2017 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00177 Impaired Latent Inhibition in GDNF-Deficient Mice Exposed to Chronic Stress Mona Buhusi*, Colten K. Brown and Catalin V. Buhusi Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT, United States Increased reactivity to stress is maladaptive and linked to abnormal behaviors and psychopathology. Chronic unpredictable stress (CUS) alters catecholaminergic neurotransmission and remodels neuronal circuits involved in learning, attention and decision making. Glial-derived neurotrophic factor (GDNF) is essential for the physiology and survival of dopaminergic neurons in substantia nigra and of noradrenergic neurons in the locus coeruleus. Up-regulation of GDNF expression during stress is linked to resilience; on the other hand, the inability to up-regulate GDNF in response to stress, as a result of either genetic or epigenetic modifications, induces behavioral alterations. For example, GDNF-deficient mice exposed to chronic stress exhibit alterations of executive function, such as increased temporal discounting. Here we investigated the effects of CUS on latent inhibition (LI), a measure of selective attention and learning, in GDNF-heterozygous (HET) mice and their wild-type (WT) littermate controls. No differences in LI were found between GDNF HET and WT mice under baseline experimental conditions. However, following CUS, GDNF-deficient mice failed to express LI. Moreover, stressed GDNF-HET mice, but not their WT controls, showed decreased neuronal activation (number of c-Fos positive neurons) in the nucleus Edited by: accumbens shell and increased activation in the nucleus accumbens core, both key João J. Cerqueira, regions in the expression of LI. -
'An Aquired Taste'
The ninth annual short story competition The Mogford Prize for Food & Drink Writing 2021 ‘An Aquired Taste’ by Emma Teichmann Short List Runner Up ‘An Acquired Taste’ It usually falls under cosmetic surgery, in which case the Public Health Service won’t cover it and you have to fork out for private care, but as I’d lost mine in a road accident they said the procedure would be free – or, rather, at the expense of the tax payer. It seemed like a no-brainer. This was almost a year after the collision. When I was thrown from my motorbike, having clipped the side of a dairy truck as I came round a corner just a little too fast and wide, my immediate worries were the shattered pelvis, ruptured spleen and brain bleed, my skull partially caving in when my helmet cracked like an egg against the concrete. It was, I was told, a miracle I survived, though it didn’t feel that way as I lay in my hospital bed. I was nil by mouth the first week as I went in and out of theatre. My jaw and nose were also broken, and even when I was finally able to – slowly, cautiously – imbibe a liquid meal, it was only a sludge-green smoothie. It didn’t look like it should taste of anything, so I wasn’t surprised when it didn’t. ‘An Aquired Taste’ 1 As I got stronger and began to feel a semblance of my At first, I tried to make light of it. Dining with friends, I normal self, I would stop a passing nurse to ask, “What’s for raised my glass of water and said, “Hey, now I can turn water dinner?” When she said, “Still smoothies, I’m afraid” I’d into wine.” They laughed a little too loudly at my feeble groan, “Not again! Let me have a proper meal,” singing after attempt at a joke. -
Using Psilocybin to Investigate the Relationship Between Attention, Working Memory, and the Serotonin 1A and 2A Receptors
Using Psilocybin to Investigate the Relationship between Attention, Working Memory, and the Serotonin 1A and 2A Receptors Olivia L. Carter1,2, David C. Burr3, John D. Pettigrew1, Guy M. Wallis1, Felix Hasler2, and Franz X. Vollenweider2 Abstract & Increasing evidence suggests a link between attention, reduced attentional tracking ability, but had no significant working memory, serotonin (5-HT), and prefrontal cortex ac- effect on spatial working memory, suggesting a functional dis- tivity. In an attempt to tease out the relationship between these sociation between the two tasks. Pretreatment with ketanserin elements, this study tested the effects of the hallucinogenic did not attenuate the effect of psilocybin on attentional per- mixed 5-HT1A/2A receptor agonist psilocybin alone and after formance, suggesting a primary involvement of the 5-HT1A pretreatment with the 5-HT2A antagonist ketanserin. Eight receptor in the observed deficit. Based on physiological and healthy human volunteers were tested on a multiple-object pharmacological data, we speculate that this impaired atten- tracking task and spatial working memory task under the tional performance may reflect a reduced ability to suppress or four conditions: placebo, psilocybin (215 Ag/kg), ketanserin ignore distracting stimuli rather than reduced attentional capac- (50 mg), and psilocybin and ketanserin. Psilocybin significantly ity. The clinical relevance of these results is also discussed. & INTRODUCTION ever, more recent work has shown that it is possible In a world where objects and events occurring around us to simultaneously track multiple objects distributed have varying degrees of relevance and importance, the throughout space (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988), indepen- ability to selectively direct and maintain attention on a dent of eye movements (Culham et al., 1998). -
The Visual Search Analogue of Latent Inhibition: Implications for Theories of Irrelevant Stimulus Processing in Normal and Schizophrenic Groups
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 224-243 The visual search analogue of latent inhibition: Implications for theories of irrelevant stimulus processing in normal and schizophrenic groups R. E. LUBOW Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, Israel and OREN KAPLAN College of Management, Rishon Lezion, Israel Latent inhibition (LI) is a robust phenomenon that is demonstrated when a previously inconsequen- tial stimulus is less effective in a new learning situation than a novel stimulus. Despite LI’s simplicity, there is considerable disagreement as to its theoretical basis. Attentional theories claim that unat- tended stimulus preexposures reduce stimulus associability. Alternatively, it has been asserted that as- sociability is unaffected and that LI is a result of competition/retrieval processes. The present article reviews a series of visual search studies, some with normal subjects, both undifferentiated and divided into low and high schizotypals, and others with pathologies that entail dysfunctional attention, such as schizophrenia, Parkinson’s disease, and anxiety. The visual search conditions were designed to model those of traditional LI experiments, while tapping attentional processes independently of the learning scores that index LI. A variety of evidence from these and other studies is used to support the involve- ment of attentional and retrieval processes in LI. A model of the mechanism of action of these pro- cesses in LI is presented, together with its application to schizophrenia. There are some conditions that make it more difficult to tensive documentation indicates that such stimulus pre- acquire or to express a new association to a familiar stim- exposure may also enhance performance on a learning ulus than one to a novel stimulus, particularly when the fa- task, as in perceptual-learning effects (for reviews, see miliar stimulus has been previously unattended. -
A Modeling Study of a Decremental Cholinergic Pathway and Its Influence on Attention and Learning
Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 29, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Research Learning to ignore: A modeling study of a decremental cholinergic pathway and its influence on attention and learning Nicolas Oros,1,4 Andrea A. Chiba,3 Douglas A. Nitz,3 and Jeffrey L. Krichmar1,2 1Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, USA; 2Department of Computer Science, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, USA; 3Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA Learning to ignore irrelevant stimuli is essential to achieving efficient and fluid attention, and serves as the complement to increasing attention to relevant stimuli. The different cholinergic (ACh) subsystems within the basal forebrain regulate at- tention in distinct but complementary ways. ACh projections from the substantia innominata/nucleus basalis region (SI/ nBM) to the neocortex are necessary to increase attention to relevant stimuli and have been well studied. Lesser known are ACh projections from the medial septum/vertical limb of the diagonal band (MS/VDB) to the hippocampus and the cin- gulate that are necessary to reduce attention to irrelevant stimuli. We developed a neural simulation to provide insight into how ACh can decrement attention using this distinct pathway from the MS/VDB. We tested the model in behavioral par- adigms that require decremental attention. The model exhibits behavioral effects such as associative learning, latent inhibi- tion, and persisting behavior. Lesioning the MS/VDB disrupts latent inhibition, and drastically increases perseverative behavior. Taken together, the model demonstrates that the ACh decremental pathway is necessary for appropriate learning and attention under dynamic circumstances and suggests a canonical neural architecture for decrementing attention. -
Latent Inhibition in an Insect: the Role of Aminergic Signaling
View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by CONICET Digital Brief Communication Latent inhibition in an insect: The role of aminergic signaling Vanesa M. Ferna´ndez,1,2 Martin Giurfa,3,4 Jean-Marc Devaud,3,4,5 and Walter M. Farina1,2,5,6 1Departamento de Biodiversidad y Biologı´a Experimental, Grupo de Estudio de Insectos Sociales, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad Universitaria 1428, Buenos Aires, Argentina; 2IFIBYNE, CONICET, Grupo de Estudio de Insectos Sociales, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad Universitaria 1428, Buenos Aires, Argentina; 3Universite´ de Toulouse, UPS, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, F-31062 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; 4CNRS, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, F-31062 Toulouse Cedex 9, France Latent inhibition (LI) is a decrement in learning performance that results from the nonreinforced pre-exposure of the to-be- conditioned stimulus, in both vertebrates and invertebrates. In vertebrates, LI development involves dopamine and seroto- nin; in invertebrates there is yet no information. We studied differential olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension response in the honeybee Apis mellifera, and we compared LI in individuals treated with antagonists of biogenic amines (dop- amine, octopamine, and serotonin). An antagonist of octopamine receptors and two antagonists of serotonin receptors showed LI disruption. We thus provide evidence that serotonin would participate in the regulation of LI in honeybees. [Supplemental material is available for this article.] Pavlovian conditioning consists of learning an association be- Are the neural mechanisms underlying LI conserved across tween a neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus: CS) and a species? To answer this question, comparative studies dissecting biologically relevant stimulus (the unconditioned stimulus: US) the role of brain areas and neurotransmitters in a broad spectrum (Pavlov 1927). -
A Peck of Pickled Peppers | News | Presspubs.Com
3/17/2018 A peck of pickled peppers | News | presspubs.com http://www.presspubs.com/st_croix/news/article_f1b5a869-133f-50b5-b458-0eaf4d6ded69.html A peck of pickled peppers by Debra Neutkens Staff Writer Sep 9, 2010 Hugo Feed Mill manager Steve Marier grows 68 potted exotic pepper plants in his driveway. - Photo by Debra Neutkens HUGO — His friends call Steve Marier “Dr. Pepper.” The Feed Mill manager hesitates to call it an obsession, but Marier does admit to a fondness that borders on addiction for exotic peppers. Sixty-eight potted pepper plants line his driveway near City Hall. They originate from all over the world: Japan, Vietnam, India, Brazil, Columbia, Peru and Mexico. With the exception of one sweet variety, all are hot. Really hot. http://www.presspubs.com/st_croix/newsarticle_f1b5a869-133f-50b5-b458-0eaf4d6ded69.html 1/4 3/17/2018 A peck of pickled peppers | News | presspubs.com Marier explained that chili peppers are rated on a hotness scale called Scoville units, named after the American chemist who invented the test. The hottest pepper in the world has over one million Scoville units. Called Bhut Jolokia, Marier has three pots of it. A toothpick dipped in a powdered piece left one of his friends gasping. The pepper is so hot in fact, it’s used in India in non-lethal hand grenades and pepper spray for self defense. The potted pepper hobby started about 15 years ago with some Caribbean seeds and a half-dozen pots. Many of the foreign varieties take 100 or more days to mature so Marier must start them indoors, moving pots outside as weather permits. -
1 TRY IT… YOU'll LIKE IT! Training Your Taste Buds to Enjoy Real
TRY IT… YOU’LL LIKE IT! Training Your Taste Buds to Enjoy Real Food If you currently find vegetables and many other healthy types of foods distasteful – chances are your taste buds are accustomed to the unnatural faire of the standard, American diet. Because of their extreme nature, these highly-refined foods with their artificial flavors and excessive amounts of sugar, refined salts and bad fats literally have burned out your taste buds. If this is the case for you as it is for most individuals, don’t despair. While you may never develop a taste for brussel sprouts you can develop a taste for healthier foods and actually come to prefer them with time. It is extremely important to understand that taste is an acquired phenomenon and it can be un-acquired as well. Given the right amount of time, your taste buds will acclimate to the taste of new foods and once you start eating more natural foods on a regular basis; you will not feel deprived for having given up the unhealthy refined junk foods. Actually, you’ll discover there are new foods that you’ll come to love even more and you’ll feel good about eating them! I’ve seen this happen with almost all of my patients. Patients who give up poor-quality, albeit tasty food, do typically tell me they enjoy the new and healthy foods they have tried. They don’t feel deprived or miss the junk foods they used to die for… and possibly from, literally! In fact, most individuals don’t even have an acquired taste for those foods anymore because when their taste buds are not bombarded and numbed by the many chemicals, sugars and salts that are in the majority of highly processed, pseudo-type foods; their taste buds changed. -
Prepulse Inhibition During Withdrawal from an Escalating Dosage Schedule of Amphetamine
View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by RERO DOC Digital Library Psychopharmacology (2003) 169:340–353 DOI 10.1007/s00213-002-1254-4 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Holger Russig · Carol A. Murphy · Joram Feldon Prepulse inhibition during withdrawal from an escalating dosage schedule of amphetamine Received: 6 March 2002 / Accepted: 21 August 2002 / Published online: 12 November 2002 Springer-Verlag 2002 Abstract Rationale: Psychomotor stimulants can induce Keywords Startle · Schizophrenia · Latent inhibition · psychotic states in humans that closely resemble those Sensitization · Rat observed in patients with idiopathic schizophrenia. At- tentional and sensorimotor gating impairments are ob- served in schizophrenic patients using the latent inhibition Introduction (LI) and prepulse inhibition (PPI) behavioral assays, respectively. Our previous studies demonstrated that after Administration of amphetamine (AMPH) can induce 4 days of withdrawal from a period of amphetamine symptoms of psychosis in humans. This outcome is most (AMPH) administration, animals exhibited disrupted LI frequently associated with a chronic high-dose escalating but normal PPI. Objective: The aim of the present study pattern of stimulant abuse (Davis and Schlemmer 1980; was to test PPI in AMPH-withdrawn rats under experi- Angrist 1994). Given that stimulant-induced psychosis in mental conditions similar to those used to best demon- humans closely resembles the psychosis observed in strate locomotor sensitization following AMPH patients with idiopathic schizophrenia (Ellinwood 1967; withdrawal. Methods: We examined the effects on PPI Griffith et al. 1972; Snyder 1973), it has been suggested of (1) pairing drug injections with PPI test-associated that similar neural adaptations could be responsible for cues, (2) administration of a low-dose dopamine agonist the development of these two phenomena. -
Whiskey & Spirits
01_117699 ffirs.qxp 10/2/07 12:41 PM Page i Whiskey & Spirits FOR DUMmIES‰ 01_117699 ffirs.qxp 10/2/07 12:41 PM Page ii 01_117699 ffirs.qxp 10/2/07 12:41 PM Page iii Whiskey & Spirits FOR DUMmIES‰ by Perry Luntz 01_117699 ffirs.qxp 10/2/07 12:41 PM Page iv Whiskey & Spirits For Dummies® Published by Wiley Publishing, Inc. 111 River St. Hoboken, NJ 07030-5774 Copyright © 2008 by Wiley Publishing, Inc., Indianapolis, Indiana Published by Wiley Publishing, Inc., Indianapolis, Indiana Published simultaneously in Canada No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Sections 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Legal Department, Wiley Publishing, Inc., 10475 Crosspoint Blvd., Indianapolis, IN 46256, 317-572-3447, fax 317-572-4355, or online at http:// www.wiley.com/go/permissions. Trademarks: Wiley, the Wiley Publishing logo, For Dummies, the Dummies Man logo, A Reference for the Rest of Us!, The Dummies Way, Dummies Daily, The Fun and Easy Way, Dummies.com and related trade dress are trademarks or registered trademarks of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and/or its affiliates in the United States and other countries, and may not be used without written permission. -
Ep #171: Why Alcohol Is an Acquired Taste
Ep #171: Why Alcohol Is an Acquired Taste Full Episode Transcript With Your Host Rachel Hart Take a Break from Drinking with Rachel Hart Ep #171: Why Alcohol Is an Acquired Taste You are listening to the Take A Break podcast with Rachel Hart, episode 171. Welcome to the Take A Break podcast with Rachel Hart. If you're an alcoholic or an addict, this is not the show for you. But if you are someone who has a highly functioning life, doing very well, but just drinking a bit too much and wants to take a break, then welcome to the show. Let's get started. Well hello everyone. I want to tell you a little bit about something that occurred to me as I was walking the other morning. I think this is one of the best things about going for a morning walk is that my brain just gets to think. I’m not looking at my phone. I’m not doing anything. I’m just taking a morning stroll and it gets to kind of connect pieces of a puzzle and put things together that it wouldn’t otherwise. And on this walk, I was thinking about why alcohol is an acquired taste. We’ve never really talked about it before on the podcast, but I do talk about it a lot in the Take A Break program. And I think understanding why it is that you acquire a taste for alcohol, why you don’t just immediately like it, I think that’s really, really fascinating because so often, I talk to you guys about how alcohol creates a reward in the brain.