Brain, Psyche, and Self: a Dialectic Between Analytical Psychology and Neuroscience

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Brain, Psyche, and Self: a Dialectic Between Analytical Psychology and Neuroscience BRAIN, PSYCHE, AND SELF: A DIALECTIC BETWEEN ANALYTICAL PSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROSCIENCE A dissertation submitted by KESSTAN C. BLANDIN to PACIFICA GRADUATE INSTITUTE in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in DEPTH PSYCHOLOGY This dissertation has been accepted for the faculty of Pacifica Graduate Institute by: •ennis P. Slattery, Chair Jennifer L.~Selig, Reader (/tyjz Susan E. Mehrtens, PhD External Reader UMI Number: 3519792 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. DiygrMution UMI 3519792 Published by ProQuest LLC 2012. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 ii JANUARY 25, 2011 Copyright by KESSTAN C. BLANDIN 2011 iii ABSTRACT Brain, Psyche, and Self: A Dialectic Between Analytical Psychology and Neuroscience by Kesstan Blandin Although much of C. G. Jung's work is not compatible with neuroscientific methods or perspectives, his ideas on the structure of the psyche and self overlap with attempts to understand the phenomenon of a self in consciousness through mapping correlates with brain functions and processes. They are therefore appropriate to engage in dialogue with neuroscience. Through these dialogues we can further understand the construction of the self and identity in light of current findings in neuroscience and the theories of C. G. Jung, particularly the collective unconscious and archetypes. In this exploration we discover how the boundaries of the self in the imagination of the psyche are revealed as the horizon of the self in the brain emerges. This dissertation employs a dialectic methodology with a dual-aspect monism and complex systems theory perspective. Dual-aspect monism understands brain and mind as different aspects of the same phenomenon, whereas complex systems theory holds that the emergence of the psyche is of unique integrity and relative autonomy. Research on implicit consciousness and the right brain hemisphere indicates that the subjective experience of the collective unconscious is autonomous and thus outside the boundaries of the ego. Temperamental predispositions manifested through neurobiological profiles are analyzed through Jung's theory of typology, which is found to be the first subjective manifestations of physiological predisposition. A discussion of iv the role of experience and the external world is provided for balance and clarity in light of the self s construction through the interchange of brain, psyche, and experience. The theory of archetypes is analyzed through a current dialogue within analytical psychology in light of an emergent perspective in neuroscience. Further exploration considers the role of memory as the bridge between neuronal functions of the brain and imaginative functions of the self. Primary conclusions are that identity is the mythic skin of the self, whereas archetypes are emergent symbols of the potential becoming of the self. TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter 1. Introduction 1 Statement of Research 2 The Dialogue 8 Fear of Reduction 16 Pathology and Souls in Distress 18 Essays on Definitions 24 Subject, self7Self 24 Identity, Ego, Persona 26 Provisional, Habitual, Conditioned self 27 Chapter 2. The Modern Self: Introduction to the Literature Review 29 Metaphysical Self. 31 Historical Self 35 Physiological Self 40 Fictive Self 44 Summary: The Clearing of the Research 50 Chapter 3. Methodology 52 An Archimedean Point 52 Dialectics and Interiority 56 Dual-aspect Monism 57 Delimits 58 SECTION I. THE BRAIN 60 Chapter 4. The Ground 63 The Collective Unconscious 64 The Evolution of the Brain: Phylogenetic 67 The Evolution of the Brain: Ontogenetic 70 The Limbic System 71 Prefrontal Cortex 77 Case Study: Borderline Personality Disorder 79 Chapter 5. The Other Within 91 Core Consciousness 92 Implicit Consciousness 95 Cerebral Hemispheres 100 Conclusions 109 SECTION II. FATE 113 Chapter 6. Temperament and Typology 116 Temperament 116 Typology 128 Chapter 7. A Confluence of Events 139 Biological Predisposition 140 Temperament and Parental Conditioning 145 The Other Environment: Social Adaptation 147 Parents and Culture 154 Public and Private Selves 159 vii Conclusions 164 SECTION III. IMAGINATION 171 Chapter 8. Imagine the Archetype 173 A Continuum of Existence 184 Archetypes are Emergent 189 Archetypes are Numinous and "Not-me" 198 Archetypes are Subjective, Distinctly Human, and Transformative 200 Chapter 9. Memory's Cleave 209 Memory in the Brain 209 Imagination and Memory 219 Misattribution 221 Suggestibility 222 Bias 225 Memory's Cleave 227 Chapter 10. Psyche's Remainder 230 Self Memory System 230 Nodal Points 233 A Congregation 236 Archetypes Revisited 240 Mythic Skin 245 Chapter 11. Concluding Thoughts 248 Critique of Jungian Theory 248 Brain and Psyche: Structures of the Self. 250 Vlll The Joy of Discovery 253 References 254 The style used throughout this dissertation is in accordance with the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (6th Edition, 2001) and Pacifica Graduate Institute's Depth Dissertation Handbook (2010-2011). Chapter 1 Introduction Identity has been a life-long fascination of mine, primarily because I wanted to be someone else. I remember the day that a favorite fantasy came into being: I was 7, alone and lying flat on my back against cool earth in the middle of a rhubarb patch. It was late spring in Vermont: a saturated, fragrant, and lush world of green, warm wind, humming insects. In my fantasy, I suffered a terrible accident that left me with complete and irreversible amnesia. I remembered no thing and no one. A tabula rasa. I would quickly move, as only a 7-year-old can, through the obligatory letting go of sad family members for whom I was as lost as my memories, and begin the glorious task of rebuilding myself from the inside out. Without memories, I had no evidence of known qualities or characteristics. I began from scratch. This took quite some time, and as I repeated the fantasy through the years, I would try on different qualities from people I admired like a Potato Head doll. When my personality was rebuilt, I went about constructing new memories from my new life to gird my personality; sometimes pre-amnesia memories would reappear like ghosts. I would decide whether to keep each one or not, and how I would treat it, as though it were a guest. I relished this fantasy and told no one about it. Although the construction of identity from the world is plainly obvious—family conditioning, cultural and historical influences—it is plainly not complete either. Always, a navel of mystery remains; for example, why do people respond so differently to similar life circumstances? Even my siblings, who were raised with the same familial conditioning factors, in the same part of the country at the same time in history, incorporated those conditioning influences differently than I did. Each self, it appears, does the same thing uniquely. 2 How did you come to be? Have you ever wondered how much of who you are has been imagined? Put another way, have you ever considered if who you think you are is somehow constructed or made up? And if this is so, who or what constructed you? What aspects of identity can be reconstructed, recreated, or changed, and which cannot? It is possible, and the current research posits, that our identity is a product of imagination as much as of physiological processes and conditioned experience. This dissertation searches into the nature of identity as an imaginative and physiological construction—a historical fiction. The intention is to deconstruct the literalness of identity—the certainty of who we think we are—by examining its construction in early experience employing an interdisciplinary analysis of neuroscience and Jungian psychology. Statement of Research: Importance and Contribution Western culture values self-knowledge, beginning with the Greeks and the examined life of Socrates and the Oracle at Delphi's famous dictum, Know Thyself, and this charge being taken up by centuries of philosophers. Still, Western culture, and particularly American popular culture, has been accused of promoting a high degree of self-involvement and even narcissism. Is research into the construction of identity contributing to anything other than turning the gerbil wheel of self-absorption? Andre Gide, in a famous reference, denounces self-knowledge: "Whoever observes himself arrests his own development. A caterpillar who seeks to know himself would never become a butterfly" (quoted in Nersoyan, 1969, p. 58). An ironic statement, indeed, for a novelist and philosopher, one who must spend considerable time in reflective thought on his own interiority and that of others if he is to connect with readers, a necessity of a 3 successful writer, which Gide certainly was. Still, it is a comment worthy of response. Why know yourself? The construction of subjectivity, a question that inquires into how we know at all and therefore how we know the world itself, has been central to psychology and philosophy for more than the last century. However, the 20th century has seen an increased and compelling interest in identity in many fields, perhaps in large part due to the dangerous and precarious state of the world and the essential role played by human beings in creating this state. Scholar Nick Mansfield's description of the subjective experience of the modern self reflects this precariousness: Things and events are now understood on the level of the pulsing, breathing, feeling individual self. Yet at the same time, this self is reported to feel less confident, more isolated, fragile and vulnerable than ever.
Recommended publications
  • Do Parents Matter? Judith Rich Harris and Child Development by Malcolm Gladwell
    August 17, 1998 The New Yorker ANNALS OF BEHAVIOR Do Parents Matter? Judith Rich Harris and child development by Malcolm Gladwell 1. The idea that will make Judith Rich Harris famous came to her, unbidden, on the afternoon of January 20, 1994. At the time, Harris was a textbook writer, with no doctorate or academic affiliation, working from her home in suburban New Jersey. Because of a lupus-like illness, she doesn’t have the strength to leave the house, and she’d spent that morning in bed. By early afternoon, though, she was at her desk, glancing through a paper by a prominent psychologist about juvenile delinquency, and for some reason a couple of unremarkable sentences struck her as odd: “Delinquency must be a social behavior that allows access to some desirable resource. I suggest that the resource is mature status, with its consequent power and privilege.” It is an observation consistent with our ideas about what it means to grow up. Teen-agers rebel against being teen-agers, against the restrictions imposed on them by adults. They smoke because only adults are supposed to smoke. They steal cars because they are too young to have cars. But Harris was suddenly convinced that the paper had it backward. “Adolescents aren’t trying to be like adults--they are trying to contrast themselves with adults,” she explains. “And it was as if a light had gone on in the sky. It was one of the most exciting things that have ever happened to me. In a minute or two, I had the germ of the theory, and in ten minutes I had enough of it to see that it was important.” If adolescents didn’t want to be like adults, it was because they wanted to be like other adolescents.
    [Show full text]
  • Barnes Et Al
    DEMONSTRATING THE VALIDITY OF TWIN RESEARCH IN CRIMINOLOGY∗ J. C. BARNES,1 JOHN PAUL WRIGHT,1,6 BRIAN B. BOUTWELL,2 JOSEPH A. SCHWARTZ,3 ERIC J. CONNOLLY,4 JOSEPH L. NEDELEC,1 and KEVIN M. BEAVER5,6 1School of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati 2School of Social Work, Saint Louis University 3School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Nebraska at Omaha 4Criminal Justice Department, Pennsylvania State University, Abington 5College of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Florida State University 6Center for Social and Humanities Research, King Abdulaziz University, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia KEYWORDS: assumptions, behavior genetics, biosocial, empirical, quantitative, twins In a recent article published in Criminology, Burt and Simons (2014) claimed that the statistical violations of the classical twin design render heritability studies useless. Claiming quantitative genetics is “fatally flawed” and describing the results generated from these models as “preposterous,” Burt and Simons took the unprecedented step to call for abandoning heritability studies and their constituent findings. We show that their call for an “end to heritability studies” was premature, misleading, and entirely without merit. Specifically, we trace the history of behavioral genetics and show that 1) the Burt and Simons critique dates back 40 years and has been subject to a broad array of empirical investigations, 2) the violation of assumptions in twin models does not in- validate their results, and 3) Burt and Simons created a distorted and highly misleading portrait of behavioral genetics and those who use quantitative genetic approaches. “The flaws of twin studies are not fatal, but rather seem no worse (and may be better) than the flaws of the typical causal study that relies on observational data.” (Felson, 2012: ii) Behavioral genetic research has existed for more than 100 years (Maxson, 2007).
    [Show full text]
  • CHILDREN DON't DO THINGS HALF WAY a Conversation with Judith Rich Harris [June 1999]
    Summer Reading from the Archive CHILDREN DON'T DO THINGS HALF WAY A Conversation with Judith Rich Harris [June 1999] I'm prone to making statements like this one: how the parents rear the child has no long-term effects on the child's personality, intelligence, or mental health. I guess you could call that an extreme statement. But I prefer to think of myself as a defender of the null hypothesis. Introduction It was in the 1990s that I received a phone call from Steven Pinker who wanted to make the world aware of the work of Judith Rich Harris, an unheralded psychologist who was advocating a revolutionary idea which she discussed in her 1999 Edge interview, "Children don't do things half way: children don’t compromise," in which she said, "how the parents rear the child has no long-term effects on the child's personality, intelligence, or mental health." 1 From the very early days of Edge, Harris was the gift that kept giving. Beginning in 1998, with her response to "What Questions Are You Asking Yourself?" through "The Last Question" in 2016, she exemplified the role of the Third Culture intellectual: "those scientists and other thinkers in the empirical world who, through their work and expository writing, are taking the place of the traditional intellectual in rendering visible the deeper meanings of our lives, redefining who and what we are." Her subsequent Edge essays over the years focused on subjects as varied as natural selection, parenting styles, the effect of genes on human behavior, twin studies, the survival of friendship, beauty as truth, among others are evidence of a keen intellect and a fearless thinker determined to advance science-based thinking as well as her own controversial ideas.
    [Show full text]
  • Do Parents Make a Difference in Children's Character Development?
    DO PARENTS MAKE A DIFFERENCE IN CHILDREN’S CHARACTER DEVELOPMENT? WHAT THE RESEARCH SHOWS Tom Lickona Center for the 4th and 5th Rs SUNY Cortland Fourth Annual Fall Character Education Seminar November 20, 1998 In its August 24, 1998 issue, Newsweek magazine devoted its cover story to a controversial new book, The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do by Judith Rich Harris. Ms. Harris does not claim to be a psychologist or expert in child development; she is a textbook writer. Nevertheless, she puts forth a startling thesis: Parents do not have much effect on how kids turn out. How kids turn out, Harris says, is mostly the result of genes and peers. Of course, we all know that heredity and peers play a part in kids' development. But is it true, as Harris says, that parents have little influence? If it's true that parents have little influence on their children, that's very bad news for schools, for schools would logically have even less influence. I'd like to argue that Harris is wrong and common sense is right: Parents do matter. Studies of childrearing reveal many things effective parents do that make a major impact on their children and especially on their character development. We should know this body of evidence for at least two reasons. First, so we can counter Harris's claim and communicate to parents what the research really shows, thereby bolstering their confidence that they do make a difference. A second reason is that good schools are like good families.
    [Show full text]
  • Concepts and Implications of Altruism Bias and Pathological Altruism
    Concepts and implications of altruism bias and pathological altruism Barbara A. Oakley1 Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Oakland University, Rochester, MI 48309 Edited by John C. Avise, University of California, Irvine, CA, and approved April 9, 2013 (received for review February 14, 2013) The profound benefits of altruism in modern society are self-evident. Pathological altruism can be conceived as behavior in which However, the potential hurtful aspects of altruism have gone largely attempts to promote the welfare of another, or others, results unrecognized in scientific inquiry. This is despite the fact that instead in harm that an external observer would conclude was virtually all forms of altruism are associated with tradeoffs—some reasonably foreseeable. More precisely, this paper defines path- of enormous importance and sensitivity—and notwithstanding that ological altruism as an observable behavior or personal tendency examples of pathologies of altruism abound. Presented here are the in which the explicit or implicit subjective motivation is in- mechanistic bases and potential ramifications of pathological altru- tentionally to promote the welfare of another, but instead of ism, that is, altruism in which attempts to promote the welfare of overall beneficial outcomes the altruism instead has unreasonable others instead result in unanticipated harm. A basic conceptual ap- (from the relative perspective of an outside observer) negative proach toward the quantification of altruism bias is presented. consequences to the other or even to the self. This definition does Guardian systems and their over arching importance in the evolution not suggest that there are absolutes but instead suggests that, of cooperation are also discussed.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology Times, Fall 2007
    FALL 2007 D E P A R T M E N T O F P SYCHO L OGY | UNIVERSITY O F Ill I N O I S A T U R B A N A - C H A MP A I G N FOCUS ON RESEA R CH The Legacy of Early Experience: Prospective and Psychological Retrospective Evidence for Enduring Effects Models of Language By Glenn I. Roisman Comprehension By Kara Federmeier One of the more Ironically, debates about the developmental controversial claims significance of early experience have, until Human brains are storehouses for vast in developmental recently, taken place in the relative absence amounts of information about things, psychology is that of much data from long-term, longitudinal people, places, and events. Remarkably, in early experiences studies, tracking cohorts of children from only hundreds of milliseconds the brain with caregivers leave infancy to maturity, that have used state- of an experienced language user can an enduring mark on of-the-art methods for studying the quality retrieve information from this storehouse in human development. of interpersonal relationships in the early response to a complex, often ambiguous, Although this is a very old idea, formalized in life course. Fortunately for me, it was as a perceptual signal—that is, a visual or theories that date back to the beginnings of graduate research assistant on one of the auditory word. Research conducted in the psychological science, no strong consensus few such investigations—Alan Sroufe, Byron University of Illinois Cognition and Brain has been built within the field or in society Egeland, and Andy at large about this issue.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Succeed in Childhood
    How to Succeed in Childhood by Judith Rich Harris very day, tell your children bad for kids. In those days, spanking was that you love them. Hug considered not just the parents’ right but them at least once every 24 their duty. hours. Never hit them. If they Partly as a result of the major retoolings Edo something wrong, don’t say, “You’re in the advice industry, child-rearing styles bad!” Say, “What you did was bad.” No, have changed drastically over the course of wait—even that might be too harsh. Say, this century. Although abusive parents instead, “What you did made me unhap- have always existed, run-of-the-mill par- py.” ents—the large majority of the popula- The people who are in the business of tion—administer more hugs and fewer giving out this sort of advice are very angry spankings than they used to. at me, and with good reason. I’m the Now ask yourself this: Are children turn- author of The Nurture Assumption—the ing out better? Are they happier and better book that allegedly claims that “parents adjusted than they were in the earlier part don’t matter.” Though that’s not what the of the century? Less aggressive? Less anx- book actually says, the advice givers are ious? Nicer? nonetheless justified in their anger. I don’t pull punches, and I’m not impressed by ² their air of benevolent omniscience. Their advice is based not on scientific evidence It was Sigmund Freud who gave us the but on prevailing cultural myths.
    [Show full text]
  • TORT LIABILITY in the AGE of the HELICOPTER PARENT Elizabeth G. Porter∗ Discussions of Parental Liability by Courts and Legal
    3 PORTER 533 – 587 (DO NOT DELETE) 2/1/2013 12:26 PM TORT LIABILITY IN THE AGE OF THE HELICOPTER PARENT Elizabeth G. Porter∗ ABSTRACT Discussions of parental liability by courts and legal scholars are often tinged with fear: fear that government interference will chill parental autonomy; fear that parents will be held liable for their children’s every misdeed; and, recently, fear that a new generation of so-called “helicopter parents” who hover over their children’s every move will establish unrealistically high legal standards for parenting. However, in the context of common law suits against parents, these fears are misguided. To the contrary, courts have consistently shielded wealthier parents—those most likely to be defendants in civil suits—from exposure to liability for conduct related to their parenting practices. This Article critically examines the common law of parental (non-) liability, both historically and in light of current cultural trends. Parental liability takes two forms: liability for parents’ harm to their children, and liability of parents for harms caused to others by their children. Individually these subjects have received remarkably little scholarly attention; together they have received none. Yet both types of parental liability are central to ongoing cultural debates about parenting, as well as to current controversies about the role of courts in establishing legal duty. A thorough reconsideration of parental liability is particularly timely in light of the new Restatement (Third) of Torts, which speaks directly to issues that are central to both forms of parental liability. This Article concludes that courts should hold parents to a standard of reasonable care.
    [Show full text]
  • Richard Lynn Maps World
    03/10/12 Gene Expression: A World of Dif f erence: Richard Ly nn Maps World Intelligence Front page Wednesday, February 01, 2006 Open Thread A World of Difference: Richard Lynn Maps World Contact Authors GNXP forum Intelligence posted by Jason Malloy @ 2/01/2006 07:12:00 PM Shortcuts: Discover GNXP Archives A review of Richard Lynn's Race Differences in Intelligence: Interview s An Evolutionary Analysis Blogroll (Click here for summary chart. Warning: 20 page review.) Principles of Population Genetics Genetics of Populations The generally listed "peak" age for scientific creativity and Molecular Evolution productivity is around the surprisingly young age range of 30-40, Quantitative Genetics but the same exact age doesn't apply to all scientific disciplines. Evolutionary Quantitative Genetics The peak in fields that demand greater doses of pure reasoning, Evolutionary Genetics such as mathematics, theoretical physics, and molecular Evolution biology, appears to be somewhere in the twenties. So, for Molecular Markers, Natural History, and Evolution instance, James Watson discovered the double helix at 25 and The Genetics of Human Populations Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits then dropped off the radar as anything but a nerd celebrity. In Epistasis and Evolutionary Process contrast are fields such as evolutionary biology, where years of Evolutionary Human Genetics collecting and assimilating large amounts of data can be required Biometry for original analysis. So, for example, Charles Darwin was 50 Mathematical Models in Biology years old when he published his landmark The Origin of Species, Speciation not to mention 62 for The Descent of Man and 63 for The Evolutionary Genetics: Case Studies and Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceived Sibling Compatibiltiy and the Effects of Personality
    PERCEIVED SIBLING COMPATIBILTIY AND THE EFFECTS OF PERSONALITY by CHERYL WARREN NEALE (Under the Direction of Charles F. Halverson) ABSTRACT This exploratory study examined perceived sibling compatibility and the effects of personality factors on 134 sibling pairs of older adolescents. The sibling pairs completed an Internet-based survey with a demographic questionnaire and two measures. The Lifespan Sibling Relationship Scale (LSRS; Riggio, 2000) measured perceived sibling compatibility, and the Inventory of Children’s Individual Differences (ICID; Halverson & Havill, et al., 2003) measured personality structure. Results showed that high compatibility sibling pairs were more likely to share in the same levels of certain personality factors and that Agreeableness was most important when predicting perceived sibling compatibility. INDEX WORDS: Sibling relationships, Siblings, Compatibility, Personality PERCEIVED SIBLING COMPATIBILITY AND THE EFFECTS OF PERSONALITY by CHERYL WARREN NEALE B.S., James Madison University, 2000 A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of The University of Georgia in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree MASTER OF SCIENCE ATHENS, GEORGIA 2003 © 2003 Cheryl Warren Neale All Rights Reserved PERCEIVED SIBLING COMPATIBILITY AND THE EFFECTS OF PERSONALITY by CHERYL WARREN NEALE Major Professor: Charles F. Halverson Committee: Valerie Havill Zolinda Stoneman Electronic Version Approved: Maureen Grasso Dean of the Graduate School The University of Georgia December 2003 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would first like to thank my professors for all of their assistance and support throughout this entire paper and process; Charles F. Halverson, Valerie Havill, Zolinda Stoneman, and Lynda Walters, I could not have done this without all of you. I think of all of you as mentors and I respect and admire you and your work a great deal.
    [Show full text]
  • The General Psychologist
    A Publication of the Society WILLIAM JAMES BOOK AWARD for General Psychology Division One The Blank Slate of the American Psychological Association by Steven Pinker, Harvard University INSIDE THIS ISSUE Steven Pinker is the Johnstone Family Professor in the Department of Psychology at de Waal: The Most Bipolar Ape Harvard University. For his book, The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Na- ..........................9 ture, the Society for General Psychology awarded Dr. Pinker the William James Book Award for 2003. The following essay, based on his invited address at the 2005 APA con- Koltko-Rivera: Worldviews vention, draws on material in the book. .........................11 Sternberg: Still Hope of Unity .........................15 uman nature is a topic of perennial interest, because every- Bitterman: Conditioning one has a theory of human nature. All of us have to anticipate ........................16 Hhow people will react to their surroundings, and that means that we all need theories, implicit or explicit, about what makes Mentoring people tick. .........................17 So much depends on our theory of human nature. In our private lives we use it to win friends and influence people, to manage our A Word from Our President relationships, to bring up our children, to control our own behavior. .........................18 Its assumptions about learning guide our policies in education; its Steven Pinker 2006 Convention Program assumptions about motivation guide our policies in law and poli- .........................19 tics. And because the theory of human nature delineates what we can achieve easily, what we can achieve only with effort and sacrifice, and what we cannot achieve at all, it’s tied to Announcements our values: what we think we can reasonably strive for as individuals and as a society.
    [Show full text]
  • Summer Reading 2019 GT English
    Mesquite ISD High School G/T English Summer Reading 2019 Your task this summer is to read a nonfiction book of your choice. ● Select a book that you have not read before. ● Choose a book from the attached list. ● Pick a topic that interests you. ● Do a little research about the book first. ○ Read reviews on library sites, Good Reads, and/or Amazon.com. ○ Several authors on the list have TED Talks. ● Make sure your parent(s) approve(s) the book you select. ● If there is another nonfiction book that you would like to read (no biographies, memoirs, or religious/spiritual texts), check with your teacher first. As you read, keep a dialectical journal to record your thinking. Divide your paper in two columns. ​ ​ In the first column, you will write quotations from the book that stand out to you. In the second column, write your response to the quotation. Set up your paper like this: Your name The title of the book The author’s name Quotations (with page numbers) Responses (Commentary, not Paraphrase) A significant quotation and where it is located • What is the author’s claim? in the book. • Do I agree/disagree with the point being made? • What is the author’s evidence? Why is this credible, relevant, and sufficient? • What larger issue or issues are raised in the book? Is this a modern phenomenon or one humans have always faced? • What hadn’t I thought of before? • What is missing? • What is most important? • How is this useful? • What should I do with this information? Requirements: ● Include between 10 and 20 quotations from your book.
    [Show full text]