Argument Maps Improve Critical Thinking

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Argument Maps Improve Critical Thinking Teaching Philosophy, 27:2, June 2004 95 Argument Maps Improve Critical Thinking CHARLES TWARDY Monash University I’ve been skeptical about claims for various approaches to teaching critical thinking, including those for argument maps coming from nearby University of Melbourne. Indeed, confident in our skepticism, we at Monash Philosophy accepted a challenge to compare our meth- ods with theirs on pre- and post-test gains on the California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST) developed by Peter Facione (1990, 1992). The Monash students did a bit better than theirs on the pre-test, raising our hopes. But when Melbourne University’s post-test results showed far higher performance gains, I thought their method worth a closer look. In short, I think computer-based argument mapping is the key to Melbourne’s success, and that it should feature centrally in any critical thinking course. In fact, it is useful quite beyond critical thinking courses. I will describe the method, my experiences with it, and the results. Introduction Being soundly trounced on improvement scores only convinced us that something over there was working. I thought the approach was fine, and argument maps were certainly helpful, but suspected that most of the gains were due to founder effect: the Melbourne teacher (Tim van Gelder) developed the Reason! approach to critical thinking, the Reason!Able software,1 and the course materials used at Melbourne. In addition, he had been the sole teacher since they adopted this ap- proach, so it was very likely that a lot of the result was because of his founder’s enthusiasm, a well-known effect in education. Tim also wanted to see how much was founder effect. So we replaced Tim with a relatively inexperienced postdoc from an outside university. Me. Tim got more time to develop materials and do research, and Monash got a “spy” to learn more about the Reason! method and why it worked so well. © Teaching Philosophy, 2004. All rights reserved. 0145-5788 pp. 95–116 96 CHARLES TWARDY The quick answer is that with my novice teaching skills thrown for the first time into an unfamiliar curriculum, student improvement on the CCTST was still 90 percent of Tim’s previous gains, and about three times the gross gain typical of other endeavors. The students improved noticeably despite what I must candidly regard as a poorly taught first-half of the subject. I’ll report on my experiences teaching the Reason! method, in- cluding common student difficulties and misconceptions, and benefits and drawbacks of the course materials and the Reason!Able software. I’ll start with some results, to show that there really is a difference, and move on to discuss the method in detail. However, as a quick introduction, I should say that the Reason! method consists mainly of intensive practice on computer-supported argument mapping in a highly structured setting. Numbers and Results This section highlights some of the results presented in Donohue et al. (2002). With a first-round entrance cutoff at the 93rd percentile of entrance score for students in Arts, Melbourne University is roughly equivalent to an elite private university in the United States. (There are no private secular universities in Australia.) It takes only the top 5–7 percent. Monash University, by comparison, has a first-round cutoff of about 88 for Arts. I lectured in a first-year introduction to critical thinking with about 135 students. I ran one of that class’s nine tutorials (“discussion sec- tions” in the U.S.). Mine had twelve students; Tim had two tutorials and the remainder were taken by philosophy graduate or honors students. I also ran a tutorial in a third-year History and Philosophy of Science class that used the Reason!Able software and argument maps to guide discussions. All tutorials used a computer-equipped classroom with an interactive whiteboard. In the first-year introduction, we measured the average gain (im- provement) in performance on the CCTST. At the beginning of the se- mester, we randomly gave half the students one version of the CCTST, and half another version, and at the end of the semester, gave them the other version. Raw gains would apply only to this test, but we want a common measure we can use across tests. On common standardized effect size measures the average gain in terms of the variability in the scores: divide the average gain by that variability. The usual measure of variability is the standard deviation. The stan- dard deviation is calculated so that a central slice of the bell-shaped curve one standard deviation to either side of the mean contains about 68 percent of the scores. Two standard deviations contain about 95 percent, and three standard deviations contain about 99 percent. So a ARGUMENT MAPS IMPROVE CRITICAL THINKING 97 tall, narrow bell will have a small standard deviation, while a wide flat bell will have a large standard deviation, but no matter the width of the bell, one standard deviation will capture 68 percent of the scores. If we divide the gain by the standard deviation in raw test scores, we get a standardized effect size called Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988). It measures the number of standard deviations’ improvement, and so gives a measure that takes inherent variability into account.2 The result was that my first-year class had a gain of about 0.72 standard deviations, down slightly from Tim’s average of about 0.8. On the other hand, an effect size of 0.7 compares favorably with the 0.3 typical of other endeavors, and the 0.5 in a critical-thinking sub- ject at McMaster University. (van Gelder, 2001; Donohue et al. 2002; Hitchcock, 2003) More results are shown in Figure 1. Because the confidence intervals do not overlap the line at 0.34, we know that the Reason! results are clearly above the expected gain for one whole year in university, and so represent a real improvement.3 The other subjects are not clearly adding anything to this baseline “maturation” effect. Indeed, our Monash CT subject is up with the rest, despite being only six weeks of lecture time in the elapsed twelve weeks. 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 Average gain for fi rst year of university 0.2 Estimated gain for fi rst semester of university 0 Fullerton Monash Monash Fullerton Melb McMaster Reason Reason Reason Phil CT Phil CTs HPS LEMUR 2002 2001 2000 1990 2001 2001 1990 2001 2001 Figure 1. Effect sizes in standard deviations (Cohen’s d) on the CCTST for various subjects. 98 CHARLES TWARDY To put the numbers in further perspective, consider general educa- tional testing results. A full three years of undergraduate education has an effect size of about 0.55 on the CCTST, with about 0.34 occurring in the first year. (Pascarella and Terenzini, forthcoming). Or consider the SAT college entrance exam, which has had a stan- dard deviation of about 115 in recent years. (Educational Statistics, 1997) Therefore an effect size of 0.8 would correspond to an 90-point increase on SAT score. In contrast, major (and expensive) test-prep companies claim to provide a 120–140-point average increase but ac- cording to a College Board study4 they actually provide an average increase of only 29 points on the verbal and 40 points on the math. (Powers and Rock, 1998) And there is no reason to believe SAT coach- ing transfers to anything other than the SAT (because it often consists largely of test-taking strategy specifically for the SAT, plus some crash review), while there is good reason to believe that Reason!-based criti- cal thinking does transfer (perhaps even to verbal SAT scores, because they rely in part on logical thinking and analysis similar to what is found on the CCTST). But anyone can get large effects by teaching to the test. However, a CCTST question is typically something like: When Mark is late for dinner, both Susan and Andrew eat out. Which of the following must be true? 1. Mark is late, Susan eats out, Andrew eats out 2. Mark is not late, Susan eats in, Andrew eats in In contrast, as we will see below, an argument mapping question is typi- cally a passage of text and a request to produce and evaluate a map. For comparison, interactive methods of physics education such as those of Eric Mazur (Mazur, 1997) get a stronger effect. In a survey of 62 introductory physics courses (6,500 students) using the standard Force Concepts Inventory pre/post test, Richard Hake (Hake, 1998, 1999) found that interactive instruction yielded a gain of 2.18(!!) standard deviations, versus 0.88 for traditional methods.5 On the other hand, they are teaching not just a skill but domain knowledge, and in class they frequently discuss conceptual questions like those which would appear on the FCI.6 Limitations of the Empirical Study The previous section aims to show that the Reason! method is much more effective than other methods at teaching critical thinking. There are two main avenues of criticism: results on the CCTST may not be the right measure, and there may be problems with this kind of study design and analysis. ARGUMENT MAPS IMPROVE CRITICAL THINKING 99 The CCTST is not perfect, but there is good reason to think it is relevant. Nevertheless, it is good to know that in the years where other tests were also given, independent assessments of written essays simi- larly favored the Reason! approach, as did the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal (Donohue et al., 2002).
Recommended publications
  • The Impact of Guided Practice in Argument Analysis And
    University of South Carolina Scholar Commons Theses and Dissertations Summer 2020 The Impact of Guided Practice in Argument Analysis and Composition via Computer-Assisted Argument Mapping Software on Students’ Ability to Analyze and Compose Evidence-Based Arguments Donna Lorain Grant Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/etd Part of the Curriculum and Instruction Commons Recommended Citation Grant, D. L.(2020). The Impact of Guided Practice in Argument Analysis and Composition via Computer- Assisted Argument Mapping Software on Students’ Ability to Analyze and Compose Evidence-Based Arguments. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/etd/6079 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you by Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE IMPACT OF GUIDED PRACTICE IN ARGUMENT ANALYSIS AND COMPOSITION VIA COMPUTER -ASSISTED ARGUMENT MAPPING SOFTWARE ON STUDENTS’ ABILITY TO ANALYZE AND COMPOSE EVIDENCE -BASED ARGUMENTS by Donna Lorain Grant Bachelor of Arts University of South Carolina—Upstate, 2000 Master of Education Converse College, 2007 Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Education in Curriculum and Instruction College of Education University of South Carolina 2020 Accepted by: Rhonda Jeffries , Major Professor Yasha Becton, Committee Member Leigh D’Amico, Committee Member Kamania Wynter-Hoyte, Committee Member Cheryl L. Addy, Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School © Copyright by Donna Lorain Grant, 2020 All Rights Reserved. ii DEDICATION To my Lord and Savior, Jesus Christ who made me for a purpose and graced me with the ability to fulfill it To my father, Donald B.
    [Show full text]
  • The Potential of Argument Mapping As a Tool for Teaching Critical Thinking in Secondary School
    Linköping University | Department of Computer and Information Science Master thesis, 30 ECTS | Cognitive Science 2018 | LIU-IDA/KOGVET-A--18/002--SE The Potential of Argument Mapping as a Tool for Teaching Critical Thinking in Secondary School Potentialen av argument mapping som ett verktyg för att undervisa kritiskt tänkande i högstadiet Tim Lidåker Supervisor : Annika Silvervarg Examiner : Arne Jönsson Linköpings universitet SE–581 83 Linköping +46 13 28 10 00 , www.liu.se Copyright The publishers will keep this document online on the Internet – or its possible replacement – for a period of 25 years starting from the date of publication barring exceptional circum- stances. The online availability of the document implies permanent permission for anyone to read, to download, or to print out single copies for his/hers own use and to use it unchanged for non-commercial research and educational purpose. Subsequent transfers of copyright cannot revoke this permission. All other uses of the document are conditional upon the con- sent of the copyright owner. The publisher has taken technical and administrative measures to assure authenticity, security and accessibility. According to intellectual property law the author has the right to be mentioned when his/her work is accessed as described above and to be protected against infringement. For additional information about the Linköping Uni- versity Electronic Press and its procedures for publication and for assurance of document integrity, please refer to its www home page: http://www.ep.liu.se/. Upphovsrätt Detta dokument hålls tillgängligt på Internet – eller dess framtida ersättare – under 25 år från publiceringsdatum under förutsättning att inga extraordinära omständigheter uppstår.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping the Structure of Debate
    Mapping the Structure of Debate JEFFREY YOSHIMI University a/California, Merced Abstract:Although debate is a richly struc­ Resume: Bien que Ie debat soit un type de tured and prevalent form of discourse, it has discours repandu et structurellement riche, il received little scholarly attention. Logicians a refYu peu d' attention savante. Les logiciens have focused on the structure of individual ont mis au point la microstructure arguments-how they divide into premises d'arguments particuliers: ils ont distingue les and conclusions, which in turn divide into premisses des conclusions et ensuite ont various constituents. In contrast, I focus on identifie divers elements qui constituent les the structure of sets of arguments, showing premisses et les conclusions. Par contraste how arguments are themselves constituents avec cette approche, je mets au point la in high-level dialectical structures. I represent macrostructure d'ensemble d'arguments et debates and positions by graphs whose ver­ demontre comment les arguments sont eux­ tices correspond to arguments and whose memes des elements composant des edges correspond to two inter-argument re­ structures dialectiques. l' emploie des lations: "dispute" and "support," respec­ graphiques pour representer les debats, les tively. On this basis I develop a theory of positions dans ceux-ci, les desaccords et the structure of debate. I'appui. Je developpe une theorie de debat a partir de ces representations. Keywords: debate, argumentation analysis, argument diagrams, argument visualization, dialectics, discourse analysis. 1. Introduction From 1992 to 1997, I took part in a project mapping out the artificial intelligence debate (Hom, Yoshimi, Deering, & McBride, 1998).1 It was a daunting effort.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Argumentation Schemes to Find Motives and Intentions of a Rational
    Argument & Computation 10 (2019) 233–275 233 DOI 10.3233/AAC-190480 IOS Press Using argumentation schemes to find motives and intentions of a rational agent Douglas Walton † Centre for Research on Reasoning, Argumentation and Rhetoric, University of Windsor, Windsor, Canada Abstract. Because motives and intentions are internal, and not directly observable by another agent, it has always been a problem to find a pathway of reasoning linking them to externally observable evidence. This paper proposes an argumentation- based method that one can use to support or attack hypotheses about the motives or intentions of an intelligent autonomous agent based on verifiable evidence. The method is based on a dialectical argumentation approach along with a commitment-based theory of mind. It is implemented using the Carneades Argumentation System, which already has 106 programmed schemes available to it and has an argument search tool. The method uses schemes, notably ones for abductive reasoning, argument from action to intention, argument from action to motive, and some new ones. Keywords: Multiagent systems, evidential reasoning in law, finding intentions, artificial intelligence 1. Introduction Research on intention recognition carried out in computer science for over thirty years has addressed the problem of identifying the intentions of an intelligent autonomous agent based on evidence from the agent’s environment, including its known actions. As shown in Section 2, some researchers in this area have devised computational systems that use abductive reasoning to support evidence-based conjectures about an automated agent’s intentions. A comparable problem in the study of intelligent autonomous agents arises for goals and motives.
    [Show full text]
  • Concept Mapping, Mind Mapping and Argument Mapping: What Are the Differences and Do They Matter?
    Concept Mapping, Mind Mapping and Argument Mapping: What are the Differences and Do They Matter? W. Martin Davies The University of Melbourne, Australia [email protected] Abstract: In recent years, academics and educators have begun to use software mapping tools for a number of education-related purposes. Typically, the tools are used to help impart critical and analytical skills to students, to enable students to see relationships between concepts, and also as a method of assessment. The common feature of all these tools is the use of diagrammatic relationships of various kinds in preference to written or verbal descriptions. Pictures and structured diagrams are thought to be more comprehensible than just words, and a clearer way to illustrate understanding of complex topics. Variants of these tools are available under different names: “concept mapping”, “mind mapping” and “argument mapping”. Sometimes these terms are used synonymously. However, as this paper will demonstrate, there are clear differences in each of these mapping tools. This paper offers an outline of the various types of tool available and their advantages and disadvantages. It argues that the choice of mapping tool largely depends on the purpose or aim for which the tool is used and that the tools may well be converging to offer educators as yet unrealised and potentially complementary functions. Keywords: Concept mapping, mind mapping, computer-aided argument mapping, critical thinking, argument, inference-making, knowledge mapping. 1. INTRODUCTION The era of computer-aided mapping tools is well and truly here. In the past five to ten years, a variety of software packages have been developed that enable the visual display of information, concepts and relations between ideas.
    [Show full text]
  • Step by Step Argument Map in Learning Environments: an Example of the Subject of Lenses1
    Online Science Education Journal, 2018; 3(2): 15-25. Online Fen Eğitimi Dergisi, 2018; 3(2): 15-25. Step by Step Argument Map in Learning Environments: An Example of the Subject of Lenses1 Büşra Nur ÇAKAN AKKAŞ, Graduate School of Natural and Applied Science, Kastamonu University, Turkey, [email protected] Elif SÖNMEZ, Department of Primary Education, Kastamonu University, Turkey, [email protected] Esra KABATAŞ MEMİŞ, Department of Mathematics and Science Education, Kastamonu University, Turkey, [email protected] To refer to this article Çakan Akkaş, B. N., Sönmez E., & Kabataş Memiş, E. (2018). Step by step argument map in learning environments: An example of the subject of lenses. Online Science Education Journal, 3(2): 15-25. ABSTRACT Argument is defined as a whole of claim that have presumptive relationships with each other. Argument map, on the other hand, is used in visually presenting arguments that have a presumptive structure with the help of graphical techniques. Argument maps help individuals to visualize and evaluate reasoning processes. In learning environments, argument mapping helps to establish connections between the data and think at a higher level. The purpose of the study is to enable preservice science teachers to create individual and interactive argument maps using the RationaleTM program within the scope of the subject of lenses. Argument mapping process was realized on the basis of “RationaleTM”, which is an online computer program. The aforementioned program allows us to examine argument maps that are taught in the subtopic of “Lenses” in the subject of “Optic” at higher education level. Also, discussions were made concerning how to use argument mapping in learning environments and examples of relevant argument maps were presented within the scope of the study.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Guide to Argument Mapping
    A Brief Guide to Argument Mapping Shamik Dasgupta January 15, 2018 1. Introduction We all have opinions and views. Sometimes we support our views with arguments. By an argument I mean a reason to believe the view in question. Argument maps are visual representations of these arguments. Suppose you believe that it’s wrong to eat meat, and you support your view with the following speech: “Look, it’s wrong to eat meat. Why? Because eating meat causes animal pain, and it’s wrong to cause animal pain.” Congratulations, you just gave an argument! We can represent the argument visually as follows: Map 1: The claim at the top, “It’s wrong to eat meat”, is called the contention. It’s the claim that the argument tries to support. The other claims, 1A-a and 1A-b, are called premises. They’re the reasons offered in support of the contention; they’re the reason to believe the contention. The green line represents this idea that 1A-a and 1A-b support the contention. The line can be read as “because” or “therefore”, depending on whether you’re reading the argument map top-down or bottom up: Top-down: It’s wrong to eat meat because eating meat causes animal pain and it’s wrong to cause animal pain. Bottom-up: Eating meat causes animal pain and it’s wrong to cause animal pain. Therefore, it’s wrong to eat meat. That’s the basic idea: every argument map consists of a contention, some premises, and lines of support. Constructing argument maps is an extremely valuable skill.
    [Show full text]
  • Argument Visualization for Eparticipation: Towards a Research Agenda and Prototype Tool
    Argument Visualization for eParticipation: Towards a Research Agenda and Prototype Tool Neil Benn1 Ann Macintosh1 1Institute of Communications Studies, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK {N.J.L.Benn, A.Macintosh}@leeds.ac.uk Abstract. This paper describes research that aims to develop an argument visualization tool and associated method for supporting eParticipation and online deliberation. Based on the state-of-the- art in the field of computer-supported argument visualization, the tool will support the work of relevant eParticipation actors by enabling them to navigate through arguments contained in relevant consultation and policy documents. This tool will form the core of our investigation into the mediating role that large, Web-based argument maps can play in eParticipation scenarios. In particular, we intend to investigate the method and practice of how various eParticipation actors use the tool in the policy-making process. To this end, this paper sets out a clear research agenda for research at the intersection of eParticipation and computer-supported argument visualization. Keywords: Argument visualization, technologies for eParticipation, online deliberation 1 Introduction This paper describes research and development on an argument visualization tool (AVT) for supporting eParticipation and online deliberation. The AVT is part of a larger suite of tools being developed within the EU-funded IMPACT project. The project began January 1, 2010 and will run for three years. The aims of the IMPACT project include addressing the four overarching problems outlined in [1], namely: 1. How can the various actors determine the relationships between contributions to policy development, whether taken from expert papers, consultations or public forum discourse, and appreciate how these contributions are taken through to decisions? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Argument Mapping at Work Some Questions Answered
    Argument Mapping at Work Some questions answered Inside: • What is Argument Mapping? • How does Argument Mapping differ from other kinds of mapping? • Why would I map? • When would I use Argument Mapping? • How would I use Argument Mapping? • Sample maps © Critical Thinking Skills BV Govert Flinckstraat 59, 1072 EC Amsterdam The Netherlands. Phone + 31 085 8771268 [email protected] www.ReasoningLab.com , www.RationaleOnline.com What is Argument Mapping? Argument mapping is a way of laying out visually reasoning and evidence for and against a statement or claim. A good map clarifies and organizes thinking by showing the logical relationships between thoughts that are expressed simply and precisely. Argument maps are driven by the question, ‘ Why should I believe that? ’. The statement in the top box is the contention under consideration. The top level of boxes underneath the contention are the reasons for (green) and against (red) it. The lower levels show further reasoning or evidence supporting or opposing claims immediately above. You can produce two kinds of argument maps in Rationale TM : 1. Reasoning maps, which lay out arguments in a quick, intuitive way 2. Analytic maps, which enable a more careful and rigorous analysis of an argument. You can see some samples of each type at the end of this booklet. How does Argument Mapping differ from other kinds of mapping? Different kinds of map are defined by the nature of the relationships they depict – what the boxes and lines mean. What kind of map something is depends on: • what goes in the boxes; and • what the connecting lines indicate.
    [Show full text]
  • Concept Mapping, Mind Mapping and Argument Mapping: What Are the Differences and Do They Matter?
    High Educ (2011) 62:279–301 DOI 10.1007/s10734-010-9387-6 Concept mapping, mind mapping and argument mapping: what are the differences and do they matter? Martin Davies Published online: 27 November 2010 Ó Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010 Abstract In recent years, academics and educators have begun to use software map- ping tools for a number of education-related purposes. Typically, the tools are used to help impart critical and analytical skills to students, to enable students to see rela- tionships between concepts, and also as a method of assessment. The common feature of all these tools is the use of diagrammatic relationships of various kinds in preference to written or verbal descriptions. Pictures and structured diagrams are thought to be more comprehensible than just words, and a clearer way to illustrate understanding of complex topics. Variants of these tools are available under different names: ‘‘concept mapping’’, ‘‘mind mapping’’ and ‘‘argument mapping’’. Sometimes these terms are used synonymously. However, as this paper will demonstrate, there are clear differences in each of these mapping tools. This paper offers an outline of the various types of tool available and their advantages and disadvantages. It argues that the choice of mapping tool largely depends on the purpose or aim for which the tool is used and that the tools may well be converging to offer educators as yet unrealised and potentially comple- mentary functions. Keywords Concept mapping Á Mind mapping Á Computer-aided argument mapping Á Critical thinking Á Argument Á Inference-making Á Knowledge mapping Introduction In the past 5–10 years, a variety of software packages have been developed that enable the visual display of information, concepts and relations between ideas.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Mapping to Enhance Learning and Critical Thinking Skills
    Background ritical thinking dates back more than 2,000 years at the birth of Western civiliza- C tion. Socrates’ dictum “The unexamined life is not worth living” subsumes the ultimate value of critical thinking in human life. Plato’s Dia- logues epitomizes not only a method Visual Mapping to but also a way of living that is still relevant to our time.1 Critical think- Enhance Learning and ing skills involve the processing of in- formation through analysis, synthesis, interpretation, explanation, evaluation, Critical Thinking Skills generalization, abstraction, application, comparison and contrast.2 Critical thinking skills are, like common sense, not very common. Studies indicate that 70% of high school graduates are defi- cient and only 28% of four-year college graduates possess excellent skills. Yet, it Héctor C. Santiago, OD, PhD, FAAO is considered to be the most important asset sought by human resource profes- sionals.3 Across the globe, many university edu- cators use the lecture format assisted with PowerPoint slides as the main delivery method. More than a genera- Abstract tion of students has suffered “death by PowerPoint,”a term describing the use Visual mapping allows the learner to explicitly explore, analyze, synthesize and of slides cluttered with text, often with share ideas.This paper reviews mapping tools suited for brainstorming and pic- irrelevant embellishments, leading to turing the thinking process (mind mapping), exploring the structure of knowl- student boredom and little meaning- 4,5 edge (concept mapping), developing premises, counter arguments and conclusions ful learning . Although these presen- around a contention (argument maps), exploring the learner’s own thinking pro- tations can be improved, there is an cess (®Thinking Maps), seeking the inter-relation among variables (general sys- inherent limitation to these traditional methods.
    [Show full text]
  • Use Style: Paper Title
    Argument analysis of the debate on the energy transition of the German road transport sector A visual analysis approach Simon Hoffmann [email protected] Instituto Superior Técnico, Universidade de Lisboa, Portugal May 2018 Abstract— The object of investigation of this work is the performance of an argument analysis on technologies for A. Battery electric vehicles decarbonizing the German road transport sector which include Battery electric vehicles (BEVs) are vehicles that operate battery electric vehicles (BEVs), fuel cell electric vehicles on electric energy provided by an on-board battery which (FCEVs) as well as biofuels and synthetic fuels for the feeds an electric motor. Due to the high efficiency of the operation of conventional internal combustion engine vehicles electric motor of up to 95% and the possibility to recuperate (ICEVs). The relevant arguments are brought forth in favour a part of the mechanical energy during the breaking process, or against the application of a certain technology are identified BEVs have the highest well-to-wheel efficiency of all and mapped to display their intercorrelations and the resulting considered technologies (around 70%) [2]. As of today, implications. The argument map is considered the main BEVs represent the most advanced alternative drive outcome of this process aiming at visualising how the different technology with more than 3 million vehicles in operation identified arguments relate to each other and to which extent worldwide and an additional 200 million electric two technologies hold competitive advantages to one another. Finally, an uncertainty analysis is performed seeking to wheelers registered [3]. highlight knowledge gaps within the debate that impact the future of the German road transport sector.
    [Show full text]