Emotional Display Rules As Work Unit Norms: a Multilevel Analysis of Emotional Labor Among Nurses

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emotional Display Rules As Work Unit Norms: a Multilevel Analysis of Emotional Labor Among Nurses Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 16, No. 2, 170–186 1076-8998/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021725 Emotional Display Rules as Work Unit Norms: A Multilevel Analysis of Emotional Labor Among Nurses James M. Diefendorff Alicia A. Grandey and Rebecca J. Erickson Pennsylvania State University University of Akron Jason J. Dahling The College of New Jersey Emotional labor theory has conceptualized emotional display rules as shared norms governing the expression of emotions at work. Using a sample of registered nurses working in different units of a hospital system, we provided the first empirical evidence that display rules can be represented as shared, unit-level beliefs. Additionally, controlling for the influence of dispositional affectivity, individual-level display rule perceptions, and emotion regulation, we found that unit-level display rules are associated with individual-level job satisfaction. We also showed that unit-level display rules relate to burnout indirectly through individual-level display rule perceptions and emotion regulation strategies. Finally, unit-level display rules also interacted with individual-level dispo- sitional affectivity to predict employee use of emotion regulation strategies. We discuss how future research on emotional labor and display rules, particularly in the health care setting, can build on these findings. Keywords: emotional display rules, emotional labor, emotions, health care, job burnout Stimulated by Ekman and Friesen’s (1975) notion positive emotions and suppressing negative emo- of social–cultural emotional norms and Hochschild’s tions, also known as integrative display rules (1983) ideas of emotional labor, organizational re- (Wharton & Erickson, 1993), are common in “people searchers adopted the term display rules to describe work” jobs (e.g., health care, education, service; the expressive expectations placed on employees as Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002) and are viewed as part of the occupational or organizational context in-role job requirements by employees and their su- (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Rafaeli & Sutton, pervisors (Diefendorff, Richard, & Croyle, 2006). 1989; Van Maanen & Kunda, 1989). According to Display rules are typically conceptualized as “a set the emotional labor literature, display rules shape of shared, albeit often latent rules” (Hochschild, employee emotional displays in ways that facilitate 1983, p. 268) that can vary based on occupational the attainment of organizational objectives (e.g., cus- norms or sociocultural differences (Ashforth & Hum- tomer satisfaction, team morale). This literature has phrey, 1993; Ekman, 1973). However, empirical re- provided evidence that display rules for expressing search has yet to examine whether employees actu- ally share display rule beliefs and what effect these shared beliefs might have on emotional labor pro- This article was published Online First January 17, 2011. cesses. The present study extends the literature on James M. Diefendorff, Department of Psychology, Uni- emotional display rules in three ways. First, we de- versity of Akron; Rebecca J. Erickson, Department of So- velop the idea that display rules are, in part, shared ciology, University of Akron; Alicia A. Grandey, Depart- ment of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University; and norms derived from unit- or group-level characteris- Jason J. Dahling, Department of Psychology, The College tics, as implied by previous authors (Bartel & Saave- of New Jersey. dra, 2000; Kelly & Barsade, 2001). We focus on This research was supported by a grant (1 D1D HP employees in one occupational group (i.e., nursing) 00004-010) from the Health Resources and Services Ad- ministration. We thank Erin Richard and Christina Saluan and one organization (i.e., a hospital system) and for their constructive comments on earlier drafts of this assess whether display rules exhibit shared properties article. at the work unit level of analysis. Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- Second, we test key theoretical points in the emo- dressed to James M. Diefendorff, Department of Psychol- ogy, The University of Akron, Akron, OH 44325-4301. tional labor literature by incorporating both unit-level E-mail: [email protected] display rules and individual-level display rule per- 170 EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES 171 ceptions in our research design. Specifically, we than in other contexts (Brunton, 2005; Erickson & compare theoretical proposals that display rules re- Grove, 2008b; Henderson, 2001). Given the high late to employee well-being indirectly through the turnover rates and high demand for health care work- emotion regulation performed by employees ers in the United States, understanding factors that (Grandey, 2000) and directly, as a result of feeling affect the well-being of nurses is of practical signif- controlled by work requirements (Gosserand & Die- icance. In what follows, we develop and test the fendorff, 2005). In testing the effects of unit-level theoretical relationships depicted in Figure 1. display rules in emotional labor processes, we con- sider whether shared beliefs have unique effects be- Display Rules as a Unit-Level Concept yond individual-level display rule perceptions or whether unit-level effects operate through these em- Although typically studied as individual-level per- ployee perceptions. ceptions, display rules have been argued to vary at both Third, although previous theory implies that how the occupational and organizational levels. Before one typically feels should interact with display rules Hochschild’s work, Ekman and Friesen (1975) pointed to predict emotion regulation (Diefendorff & Gosser- out that display rules varied as a function of occupa- and, 2003), empirical work has primarily examined tional requirements. For example, nurses are socialized the main effects of display rules and employee affect to express compassion, caring, and empathy (Hinds, on emotion regulation (e.g., Brotheridge & Grandey, Quargnenti, Hickey, & Mangum, 1994) at the same 2002; Diefendorff, Croyle, & Gosserand, 2005; Gos- time that they are encouraged to develop a level of serand & Diefendorff, 2005; Grandey, 2003; Totter- professional detachment (Carmack, 1997; Savett, 2000; dell & Holman, 2003; for exceptions, see Beal, Trou- Stephany, 1989; Stowers, 1983). At the organizational gakos, Weiss, & Green, 2006; Bono & Vey, 2007; level, display rules are thought to be shaped and per- Goldberg & Grandey, 2007). We examine whether petuated by informal company-specific culture (e.g., unit-level display rules and individual affectivity in- language, symbols and stories, informal social conse- teract to predict emotion regulation strategies and quences for display rule violation), as well as formal provide the first test of such interactive effects in a policies and practices (e.g., selection, recruitment, so- field study. cialization, reward systems; Arvey, Renz, & Watson, Previous research has frequently focused on cus- 1998; Martin, Knopff, & Beckman, 1998; Van Maanen tomer service employees (e.g., sales; Tsai, 2001) or & Kunda, 1989). Thus, there is evidence that emotional employees from a heterogeneous set of occupations display rules operate at levels of analysis higher than (e.g., Diefendorff & Richard, 2003). We focus on the that of individual perceptions. emotionally charged context of hospital nurses, Furthermore, we suggest that display rules will which may make display rules even more meaningful emerge in, and vary by, work groups as well (Kelly & Unit-Level Integrative Display Rules Employee Dispositional Emotional Well-being Affectivity Labor Strategies - Job satisfaction - Positive - Surface Acting - Burnout - Negative -Deep Acting Figure 1. Theoretical model. Gray shading indicates unit-level construct; all others are at the individual level. The effects of individual-level display rule perceptions on each of these theoretical links are examined. 172 DIEFENDORFF, ERICKSON, GRANDEY, AND DAHLING Barsade, 2001). Consistent with this view, Bartel and ing is common (Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987; see also Saavedra (2000) theorized that social norms guide Smith, 1992; Smith & Kleinman, 1989). Moreover, team members’ expressions of their moods, and by overlooking these unit-level norms, previous re- Kelly and Barsade (2001) suggested that work groups search has not fully modeled how display rules in- may come to perceive local “emotion norms” that fluence employee behavior (e.g., emotion regulation) differ from or even supersede (Barker, 1993) the and outcomes (e.g., attitudes, stress), as unit-level formal display rules of the organization or occupa- emotional display rules may represent a unique tion. One qualitative study demonstrated that differ- source of influence on emotion management and ent units of The Body Shop developed distinct group- well-being. Furthermore, we propose that unit-level level norms that firmly controlled emotional behavior display rules may combine with individual level dis- (Martin et al., 1998). positional affect to predict emotion regulation. Such local emotional display norms are likely to be shaped by top-down, contextual factors as well as bottom-up, emergent properties of the social environ- Main Effects of Unit-Level Display Rules ment (Kozlowski & Klein, 2000). In addition to the on Emotional Regulation and Well-Being distal top-down influences of occupation and organi- zation, more proximal top-down influences on work- Generally, the presence of display rules is linked to unit
Recommended publications
  • Cultural Distance, Perception of Emotional Display Rules, And
    CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Science Nicole L. Gullekson August 2007 2 This thesis titled CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT by NICOLE L. GULLEKSON has been approved for the Department of Psychology and the College of Arts and Sciences by ________________________________________________ Jeffrey B. Vancouver Associate Professor of Psychology ________________________________________________ Benjamin M. Ogles Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 Abstract GULLEKSON, NICOLE L., M.S., August 2007, Experimental Psychology CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT (109 pp.) With increased globalization, more individuals temporarily leave home to work and study in foreign countries. These sojourners are confronted with societal norms different from their home cultures. The present study investigated the extent to which international student sojourners perceive differences in emotional display norms between their home and host cultures, as well as the influence of such perception on adjustment. Although accurate perception of the host culture’s emotional display rules was not related to adjustment, a “guest” effect existed. Specifically, international student participants reported that one should
    [Show full text]
  • Daily Emotional Labor, Negative Affect State, and Emotional Exhaustion: Cross-Level Moderators of Affective Commitment
    sustainability Article Daily Emotional Labor, Negative Affect State, and Emotional Exhaustion: Cross-Level Moderators of Affective Commitment Hyewon Kong 1 and Joo-Eon Jeon 2,* 1 College of Liberal Arts and Interdisciplinary Studies, Kyonggi University, Suwon 16227, Korea; [email protected] 2 Department of Business Administration, Anyang University, Anyang 14028, Korea * Correspondence: [email protected] Received: 9 May 2018; Accepted: 8 June 2018; Published: 12 June 2018 Abstract: Employees’ emotional-labor strategies, experienced affects, and emotional exhaustion in the workplace may vary over time within individuals, even within the same day. However, previous studies on these relationships have not highlighted their dynamic properties of these relationships. In addition, although the effects of surface and deep acting on emotional exhaustion have been investigated in emotional-labor research, empirical studies on these relationships still report mixed results. Thus, we suggest that moderators may affect the relationship between emotional labor and emotional exhaustion. Also, this study examines the relationship between emotional labor and emotional exhaustion within individuals by repeated measurements, and verifies the mediating effect of a negative affect state. Finally, our study confirms the moderating effects that affective commitment has on the relationship between emotional labor and emotional exhaustion. Data was collected from tellers who had a high degree of interaction with clients at banks based in South Korea. A total of 56 tellers participated in the survey and responded for five working days. A total of 616 data entries were collected from the 56 respondents. We used a hierarchical linear model (HLM) to examine our hypothesis. The results showed that surface-acting emotional labor increases emotional exhaustion; furthermore, the relationship between surface acting emotional labor and emotional exhaustion is mediated by a negative affect state within individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Dispositional Affect As a Moderator in the Relationship Between Role Conflict and Exposure to Bullying Behaviors
    fpsyg-10-00044 January 22, 2019 Time: 17:17 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 24 January 2019 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00044 Dispositional Affect as a Moderator in the Relationship Between Role Conflict and Exposure to Bullying Behaviors Iselin Reknes1*, Ståle Valvatne Einarsen1, Johannes Gjerstad1,2 and Morten Birkeland Nielsen1,2 1 Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway, 2 National Institute of Occupational Health, Oslo, Norway Stressors in the work environment and individual dispositions among targets have been established separately as antecedents and risk factors of workplace bullying. However, few studies have examined these stressors in conjunction in order to determine personal dispositions among targets as possible moderators in the work stressor–bullying relationship. The aim of the present study was to examine multiple types of dispositional affect among targets as potential moderators in the relationship between role conflict and exposure to bullying behaviors, employing two independent cross-sectional Edited by: samples. The first sample comprised 462 employees from a Norwegian sea transport Gabriela Topa, organization, where trait anger and trait anxiety were included moderators. The second Universidad Nacional de Educación sample was a nationwide probability sample of the Norwegian working population and a Distancia (UNED), Spain comprised 1,608 employees randomly drawn from The Norwegian Central Employee Reviewed by: Miriam Lacey, Register, where positive and negative affect were included moderators. The results Pepperdine University, United States showed that trait anger, trait anxiety, and negative affect strengthened the positive Javier Bardon, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Spain relationship between role conflict and reports of bullying behaviors. Positive affect did *Correspondence: not moderate this relationship.
    [Show full text]
  • Toward a Model of Emotion-Speci®C In¯Uences on Judgement and Choice
    COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2000, 14 (4), 473±493 Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-speci®c in¯uences on judgement and choice Jennifer S. Lerner Carnegie Mellon University and University of California, Los Angeles, USA Dacher Keltner University of California, Berkeley, USA Most theories of affective in¯uences on judgement and choice take a valence- based approach, contrasting the effects of positive versus negative feeling states. These approaches have not speci®ed if and when distinct emotions of the same valence have different effects on judgement. In this article, we propose a model of emotion-speci®c in¯uences on judgement and choice. We posit that each emotion is de®ned by a tendency to perceive new events and objects in ways that are consistent with the original cognitive-appraisal dimensions of the emotion. To pit the valence and appraisal-tendency approaches against one another, we present a study that addresses whether two emotions of the same valence but differing appraisals–anger and fear– relate in different ways to risk perception. Consistent with the appraisal- tendency hypothesis, fearful people made pessimistic judgements of future events whereas angry people made optimistic judgements. In the Discussion we expand the proposed model and review evidence supporting two social moderators of appraisal-tendency processes. INTRODUCTION Once an exclusively cognitive enterprise, research on judgement and choice increasingly addresses the powerful in¯uence of affect (for reviews see Bodenhausen, 1993; Bower, 1991; Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Forgas, 1995; Loewenstein, 1996; Schwarz & Clore, 1996; Zajonc, 1998). Requests for reprints should be sent to Jennifer Lerner at the Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213-3890 , USA; e-mail: [email protected].
    [Show full text]
  • Trait Positivity Predicts Increased Rejection of Unfair Financial Offers
    ‘‘I’m Worth More than That’’: Trait Positivity Predicts Increased Rejection of Unfair Financial Offers Barnaby D. Dunn1*, Dasha Makarova2, David Evans1, Luke Clark2 1 Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, UK Medical Research Council, Cambridge, United Kingdom, 2 Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom Abstract Humans react strongly to unfairness, sometimes rejecting inequitable proposals even if this sacrifices personal financial gain. Here we explored whether emotional dispositions - trait tendencies to experience positive or negative feelings – shape the rejection of unfair financial offers. Participants played an Ultimatum Game, where the division of a sum of money is proposed and the player can accept or reject this offer. Individuals high in trait positivity and low in trait negativity rejected more unfair offers. These relationships could not be explained by existing accounts which argue that rejection behaviour results from a failure to regulate negative emotions, or serves to arbitrate social relationships and identity. Instead, the relationship between dispositional affect and rejection behaviour may be underpinned by perceived self worth, with those of a positive disposition believing that they are ‘‘worth more than that’’ and those of a negative disposition resigning themselves to ‘‘taking the crumbs from under the table’’. Citation: Dunn BD, Makarova D, Evans D, Clark L (2010) ‘‘I’m Worth More than That’’: Trait Positivity Predicts Increased Rejection of Unfair Financial Offers. PLoS ONE 5(12): e15095. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015095 Editor: James A. R. Marshall, University of Sheffield, United Kingdom Received August 5, 2010; Accepted October 20, 2010; Published December 8, 2010 Copyright: ß 2010 Dunn et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Dispositional Positive and Negative Affect and Self-Employment Transitions: the Mediating Role of Job Satisfaction
    Dispositional positive and negative affect and self-employment transitions: The mediating role of job satisfaction Boris Nikolaev Affiliation: Hankamer School of Business, Baylor University Address: One Bear Place #98011, Waco, TX 76798 Phone: 813.401.9756 E-mail: [email protected] Nadav Shir Stockholm School of Economics P.O. Box 6501 SE-11383 Stockholm, Sweden [email protected] and Hanken School of Economics Arkadiankatu 22, 001 01 Helsinki, Finland [email protected] Johan Wiklund Whitman School of Management Syracuse University, 721 University Avenue/Suite 116 Syracuse NY 13244-2450, United States [email protected] Forthcoming in Entrepreneurship Theory & Practice * This paper uses unit record data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey. The HILDA Project was initiated and is funded by the Australian Government Department of Social Services (DSS) and is managed by the Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (Melbourne Institute). The findings and views reported in this paper, however, are those of the author and should not be attributed to either DSS or the Melbourne Institute. We would like to thank the associate editor and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful and developmental comments. 1 Abstract: Affect is increasingly studied within entrepreneurship. We develop a partial mediation model in which positive and negative dispositional affect influences entry into entrepreneurship, suggesting that those experiencing greater negative affect experience less job satisfaction and are more likely to enter entrepreneurship. Using a novel methodological approach to capture affective disposition, we test our model on a large panel dataset from Australia, finding support for our hypotheses.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Individual Differences in Sadness Coherence: Associations with Dispositional Affect and Age Deborah J
    Emotion Individual Differences in Sadness Coherence: Associations With Dispositional Affect and Age Deborah J. Wu, Ryan C. Svoboda, Katherine K. Bae, and Claudia M. Haase Online First Publication, March 19, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000731 CITATION Wu, D. J., Svoboda, R. C., Bae, K. K., & Haase, C. M. (2020, March 19). Individual Differences in Sadness Coherence: Associations With Dispositional Affect and Age. Emotion. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000731 Emotion © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000 ISSN: 1528-3542 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000731 Individual Differences in Sadness Coherence: Associations With Dispositional Affect and Age Deborah J. Wu Ryan C. Svoboda University of Massachusetts Amherst Northwestern University Katherine K. Bae Claudia M. Haase University of Michigan, Ann Arbor Northwestern University The current laboratory-based study examined individual differences in sadness coherence (i.e., coherence between objectively coded sad facial expressions and heart rate in response to a sad film clip) and associations with dispositional affect (i.e., positive and negative affect, extraversion, neuroticism) and age in a sample of younger and older adults. Results showed that (a) greater sadness coherence was associated with lower dispositional negative affect (i.e., greater positive to negative affect ratio; lower neuroticism) and (b) older adults had greater sadness coherence than younger adults. Findings remained stable when controlling for covariates.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Contagion and Its Influence on Group Behavior Author(S): Sigal G
    The Ripple Effect: Emotional Contagion and Its Influence on Group Behavior Author(s): Sigal G. Barsade Source: Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 47, No. 4 (Dec., 2002), pp. 644-675 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of the Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3094912 . Accessed: 12/02/2015 02:13 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Sage Publications, Inc. and Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Administrative Science Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 128.103.149.52 on Thu, 12 Feb 2015 02:13:10 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The Ripple Effect: Group emotional contagion, the transfer of moods Emotional among people in a group, and its influence on work Contagion was examined and Its Influence on group dynamics in a laboratory study of managerial decision making using multiple, convergent Group Behavior measures of mood, individual attitudes, behavior, and group-level dynamics. Using a 2 x 2 experimental design, Sigal G. Barsade with a trained confederate enacting mood conditions, the effect of emotional was found YaleUniversity predicted contagion among group members, using both outside coders' rat- ings of participants' mood and participants' self-reported mood.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Levels of Happiness by Robert Biswas-Diener, Portland State University and Noba, & Christopher W
    Optimal Levels of Happiness By Robert Biswas-Diener, Portland State University and Noba, & Christopher W. Wiese, Purdue University Citation: Biswas-Diener, R., & Wiese, C. W. (2018). Optimal levels of happiness. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers. DOI:nobascholar.com Abstract: In the post-material era, people are increasingly focusing on the pursuit of happiness. This popular interest has been met by a growing number of authors and interventionists who promise routes to happiness. In large part, these avenues to the good life are anchored in the results of scientific studies. Here, we offer the caution that there may be an optimal level of happiness. While it is true that happiness is widely beneficial, there is also a growing body of research suggesting that it is not always beneficial for all people. In this chapter, we define happiness and review the literature suggesting that more nuance discussion is needed. We focus on three ways that happiness might be sub-optimal, including mood dysregulation (being happy regardless of circumstance), complacency (curtailing effort), and affect imbalance. We conclude by discussing three contexts that interventionists and policy makers ought to consider when promoting happiness. These include attention to happiness in specific life domains, distinguishing between short and long-term happiness, and a deeper appreciation of how cultural factors affect the pursuit and experience of happiness. Keywords: Optimal levels; happiness; well-being; interventions Everyone makes attributions. We believe our neighbor is a jerk, our spouse is generous, and that our pet dogs are loyal.
    [Show full text]
  • Variations of Emotional Display Rules Within and Across Cultures: a Comparison Between Canada, USA, and Japan
    Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science © 2009 Canadian Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 41, No. 1, 1–10 0008-400X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014387 Variations of Emotional Display Rules Within and Across Cultures: A Comparison Between Canada, USA, and Japan Saba Safdar Wolfgang Friedlmeier University of Guelph Grand Valley State University David Matsumoto Seung Hee Yoo San Francisco State University Yale University Catherine T. Kwantes Hisako Kakai University of Windsor Aoyama Gakuin University Eri Shigemasu Yamanashi Gakuin University This study investigates emotional display rules for seven basic emotions. The main goal was to compare emotional display rules of Canadians, US Americans, and Japanese across as well as within cultures regarding the specific emotion, the type of interaction partner, and gender. A total of 835 university students participated in the study. The results indicate that Japanese display rules permit the expression of powerful (anger, contempt, and disgust) significantly less than those of the two North American samples. Japanese also think that they should express positive emotions (happiness, surprise) significantly less than the Canadian sample. Furthermore, Japanese varied the display rules for different interaction partners more than the two North American samples did only for powerful emotions. Gender differences were similar across all three cultural groups. Men expressed powerful emotions more than women and women expressed powerless emotions (sadness, fear) and happiness more than men. Depending on the type of emotion and interaction partner some shared display rules occurred across culture and gender. The implications of these findings are discussed in relation to cultural dimensions and other cultural characteristics. Keywords: emotional display rules, norms of emotions, cross-cultural comparison, ingroup/outgroup The notion of cultural display rules was first introduced by of culture and emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Dispositional Affect and Cognition on Venture Investment Portfolio Concentration
    Journal of Business Venturing 28 (2013) 397–412 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Business Venturing The influence of dispositional affect and cognition on venture investment portfolio concentration ☆ Chien Sheng Richard Chan a,⁎, Haemin Dennis Park b,c a Peking University HSBC Business School, University Town, Nanshan District, Shenzhen, 518055 China b Bennett S. LeBow College of Business, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA 19104, United States c Institute for Entrepreneurship and Innovation, Henry W. Bloch School of Management, University of Missouri — Kansas City, Kansas City, MO 64110, United States article info abstract Article history: We explore how an investor's dispositional affect and cognitive style influence venture investment Received 7 December 2010 portfolio concentration. Based on a field study using a sample of 128 judges from a business plan Received in revised form 16 February 2012 competition, we find that high positive affectivity investors construct more concentrated invest- Accepted 24 February 2012 ment portfolios than their low positive affectivity counterparts, whereas high negative affectivity Available online 15 June 2012 investors construct more diversified investment portfolios than their low negative affectivity counterparts. Further, investors who rely on analytical decision making display a weaker rela- Field Editor: M. Frese tionship between negative affectivity and investment diversification whereas investors who rely on emotion-based decision making display a stronger relationship between positive affectivity and Keywords: investment concentration. Venture investment decisions © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Dispositional affect Cognition 1. Executive summary Venture investment decision making is an important topic in entrepreneurship because it constitutes a process whereby resources and opportunities are matched.
    [Show full text]
  • THE EFFECTS of MOOD and DISPOSITIONAL AFFECTIVITY on SELF- REPORTED JOB SATISFACTION by CRISTINA DIANE KIRKENDALL B.S., Eastern
    THE EFFECTS OF MOOD AND DISPOSITIONAL AFFECTIVITY ON SELF- REPORTED JOB SATISFACTION A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science By CRISTINA DIANE KIRKENDALL B.S., Eastern Michigan University, 2003 2010 Wright State University WRIGHT STATE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES 11-12-2009 I HEREBY RECOMMEND THAT THE THESIS PREPARED UNDER MY SUPERVISION BY Cristina Kirkendall ENTITLED The Effects of Mood and Dispositional Affectivity on Self-reported Job Satisfaction BE ACCEPTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF Master of Science. ________________________________ Committee on Final Examination Nathan Bowling, Ph.D. Thesis Director _________________________________ ________________________________ Gary Burns, Ph.D. Scott Watamaniuk, Ph.D. Graduate Program Director _________________________________ Martin Gooden, Ph.D. ________________________________ John Flach, Ph.D. Chair, Department of Psychology _________________________________ Nathan Bowling, Ph.D. _________________________________ Joseph F. Thomas, Jr., Ph.D. Dean, School of Graduate Studies ABSTRACT Kirkendall, Cristina Diane. M.S., Department of Psychology, Wright State University, 2010. The Effects of Mood and Dispositional Affectivity on Self-Reported Job Satisfaction. Job satisfaction has several antecedents, including situational factors (e.g., pay, job characteristics), personality factors (e.g., positive and negative affectivity), and social interactions at work. Job satisfaction is most often measured with self-report surveys which may not effectively capture unconscious attitudes or context effects such as mood. Mood at time of survey completion has been shown to have an effect on self-reported satisfaction measures. This study uses animal-related video clips as a mood induction and examines the effect of induced mood and personality factors on self-report measures of job satisfaction.
    [Show full text]