Oxford Handbooks Online

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Oxford Handbooks Online The prevalence of synesthesia: The Consistency Revolution Oxford Handbooks Online The prevalence of synesthesia: The Consistency Revolution Donielle Johnson, Carrie Allison, and Simon Baron-Cohen Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia Edited by Julia Simner and Edward Hubbard Print Publication Date: Dec 2013 Subject: Psychology, Cognitive Neuroscience, History and Systems in Psychology Online Publication Date: Dec 2013 DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199603329.013.0001 Abstract and Keywords We begin this chapter with a review of the history of synaesthesia and a comparison of what we consider to be either genuine or inauthentic manifestations of the phenomenon. Next, we describe the creation and development of synaesthetic consistency tests and explore reasons why assessing consistency became the most widely used method of confirming the genuineness of synaesthesia. We then consider methodologies that demonstrate synaesthesia's authenticity by capitalizing on properties other than consistency. Finally, we discuss how together, consistency tests and other methodologies are helping researchers determine prevalence and elucidate the mechanisms of synaesthesia. Keywords: synaesthesia, consistency, Test of Genuineness, prevalence A Brief History of Synesthesia Research Traditionally, the term synesthesia describes a condition in which the stimulation of one sensory modality automatically evokes a perception in an unstimulated modality (e.g., the sound of a bell leads the synesthete to experience the color pink; Baron-Cohen, Wyke, and Binnie 1987; Bor, Billington, and Baron-Cohen 2007; Marks 1975; Sagiv 2005). While this definition describes a cross-sensory association, synesthetic experiences can also be intrasensory (e.g., the letter “g” triggers a blue photism when read). The stimulus (bell or “g”) that triggers the synesthetic perception is referred to as an inducer, while the resulting experience or percept (blue) is called the concurrent (Grossenbacher 1997; Grossenbacher and Lovelace 2001). Not all inducers are sensory, however. Page 1 of 26 PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy). Subscriber: University of Cambridge; date: 12 July 2016 The prevalence of synesthesia: The Consistency Revolution Grossenbacher and Lovelace (2001) used the term synesthetic conception to refer to synesthetic experiences in which inducers are concepts. For example, in “time-space” synesthesia months take on locations in space, and this can occur whether the months are read, heard, or even thought about (Grossenbacher and Lovelace 2001). Throughout this chapter, when we use the term synesthesia we are referring to genuine, developmental synesthesia. Developmental synesthetes generally report having synesthesia for as long as they can remember and cannot provide an explanation or learning account of their experiences (Baron-Cohen et al. 2007). Genuine synesthesia (p. 4) does not arise voluntarily, nor is it learned through training, acquired through drug use, or induced (solely) by any neurological pathology.1 Learned or acquired associations (e.g., in those familiar with the number-color coding on cross-stitch needles) can be difficult to distinguish from genuine synesthesia. Over- rehearsal can lead to a high level of performance similar to synesthetes on tests designed to assess synesthesia (i.e., the synesthetic Stroop test; discussed in more detail later). Non-synesthetes have also been shown to experience learned or trained associations as automatically as true synesthetes do (Elias et al. 2003; Meier and Rothen 2009). This in turn has led some to suggest that synesthesia itself is merely learned. However, a learning account of synesthesia cannot explain why siblings with synesthesia reared in essentially identical environments (or at least exposed to the same alphabet teaching materials or children’s books) report different colors for the same inducer, or experience different variants of synesthesia altogether (Barnett et al. 2008). Metaphors, too, can seem synesthetic. It is not uncommon, for example, for non- synesthete artists and musicians to ascribe colors or other characteristics to pieces. A composer might refer to a musical piece as heavy and blue because of its tone or the mood it evokes, not because it literally triggers a heavy, blue percept for him or her. Synesthetic metaphors are not exclusively used by artists. Many people describe wine or cheese as “sharp” or “robust.” However, the use of metaphor and imagery should be distinguished from true synesthesia, in which percepts are induced automatically and involuntarily (Baron-Cohen and Harrison 1999; Simpson and McKellar 1955). Having established what we consider as synesthesia in this article, we will now review the recent history of research into this phenomenon. Despite its popularity in the late nineteenth century, by the mid 1930s there was a considerable reduction in the psychological literature on synesthesia (Marks 1975). The problem for researchers then and now is that synesthesia is an internal, subjective experience. Although some early studies incorporated objective verification (Calkins 1893, and see later), most relied on descriptions and reports given by synesthetes themselves. In the belief that it would restore objectivity to the field of psychology, the Page 2 of 26 PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy). Subscriber: University of Cambridge; date: 12 July 2016 The prevalence of synesthesia: The Consistency Revolution school of behaviorism, which emerged in the 1920s and 1930s, rejected the study of subjective, mental phenomena, or required rigorous independent evidence of their existence. One casualty of this was synesthesia research. With few or no objective tests to validate self-reports from synesthetes and thereby distinguish them from people with a vivid imagination or a faculty for generating striking pseudo-synesthetic metaphors, the condition was “[no longer] considered amenable to scientific investigation” (Harrison and Baron-Cohen 1997, 4). When behaviorism was supplanted by cognitivism in the latter half of (p. 5) the twentieth century, however, new methods were introduced to assess the genuineness of synesthesia. This ushered in a synesthesia “renaissance” characterized by rigorous empirical investigation which continues to this day. Since the earliest days of synesthesia research, we have moved beyond merely proving that synesthesia exists. Much of this progress can be attributed to advancements stemming from a relatively simple but powerful test developed (or re-discovered, see later) in the 1980s. This test allows researchers to quickly determine whether a person is a genuine synesthete, and ultimately opens the door for a more thorough examination of the condition. In this chapter we will describe the creation and development of this test, now known as the Test of Genuineness (TOG) and discuss reasons why its central tenet, assessing the consistency of synesthetes’ reports, became the first “gold standard” method for authenticating reported synesthesia (Rich, Bradshaw, and Mattingley 2005; Simner et al. 2006; Ward and Mattingley 2006). We will also consider recent examinations of synesthesia’s other features and show that together, consistency tests, behavioral tasks, and neuroimaging methods are helping researchers establish prevalence rates, shed light on neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying the phenomenon, and use synesthesia research to inform our understanding of typical cognition. Synesthetic Consistency and the First Test of Genuineness Early investigators meticulously recorded the experiences of synesthetes, primarily providing detailed case descriptions and reproducing drawings of reported percepts when possible. A comparison of early studies reveals differences of opinion regarding definitions (Phillips 1897), prevalence rates (Calkins 1895; Phillips 1897), heritability (Flournoy 1893), and what might be considered typical manifestations of the same form of synesthesia (e.g., what constitutes “normal” number-form visualizations for synesthetes; Galton 1883; Phillips 1897; see later). However, the constancy of synesthetic Page 3 of 26 PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy). Subscriber: University of Cambridge; date: 12 July 2016 The prevalence of synesthesia: The Consistency Revolution percepts is one point of agreement: for any particular synesthete, the synesthetic experience is typically the same for any given trigger (e.g., if the letter “A” is red for a synesthete, it will tend to always be red for that synesthete, over days, months, years, and even decades).2 In one early study, for example, Dresslar (1903) aimed to assess the stability of a young woman’s synesthesia over a period of 8 years. Whether she chose color words from a dictionary
Recommended publications
  • Synesthetes: a Handbook
    Synesthetes: a handbook by Sean A. Day i © 2016 Sean A. Day All pictures and diagrams used in this publication are either in public domain or are the property of Sean A. Day ii Dedications To the following: Susanne Michaela Wiesner Midori Ming-Mei Cameo Myrdene Anderson and subscribers to the Synesthesia List, past and present iii Table of Contents Chapter 1: Introduction – What is synesthesia? ................................................... 1 Definition......................................................................................................... 1 The Synesthesia ListSM .................................................................................... 3 What causes synesthesia? ................................................................................ 4 What are the characteristics of synesthesia? .................................................... 6 On synesthesia being “abnormal” and ineffable ............................................ 11 Chapter 2: What is the full range of possibilities of types of synesthesia? ........ 13 How many different types of synesthesia are there? ..................................... 13 Can synesthesia be two-way? ........................................................................ 22 What is the ratio of synesthetes to non-synesthetes? ..................................... 22 What is the age of onset for congenital synesthesia? ..................................... 23 Chapter 3: From graphemes ............................................................................... 25
    [Show full text]
  • Chromesthesia As Phenomenon: Emotional Colors
    Writing Programs Academic Resource Center Fall 2014 Chromesthesia as Phenomenon: Emotional Colors Jessica Makhlin Loyola Marymount University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lmu.edu/arc_wp Repository Citation Makhlin, Jessica, "Chromesthesia as Phenomenon: Emotional Colors" (2014). Writing Programs. 12. https://digitalcommons.lmu.edu/arc_wp/12 This Essay is brought to you for free and open access by the Academic Resource Center at Digital Commons @ Loyola Marymount University and Loyola Law School. It has been accepted for inclusion in Writing Programs by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons@Loyola Marymount University and Loyola Law School. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Chromesthesia as Phenomenon: Emotional Colors by Jessica Makhlin An essay written as part of the Writing Programs Academic Resource Center Loyola Marymount University Spring 2015 1 Jessica Makhlin Chromesthesia as Phenomenon: Emotional Colors Imagine listening to a piece of music and seeing colors with every pitch, change in timbre, or different chord progressions. Individuals with chromesthesia, also known as synesthetes, commonly experience these “colorful senses.” Chromesthesia is defined as “the eliciting of visual images (colors) by aural stimuli; most common form of synesthesia.”1 Synesthesia is considered the wider plane of these “enhanced senses.” It is the condition where one sense is perceived at the same time as another sense.2 This is why chromesthesia is narrowed down to be a type of synesthesia: because it is the condition where hearing is simultaneously perceived with sights/feelings (colors). This phenomenon incites emotions from colors in addition to emotions that music produces. Chromesthesia evokes strong emotional connections to music because the listener associates different pitches and tones to certain colors, which in turn, produces specific feelings.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuronal Dynamics of Grapheme-Color Synesthesia A
    Neuronal Dynamics of Grapheme-Color Synesthesia A Thesis Presented to The Interdivisional Committee for Biology and Psychology Reed College In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Arts Christian Joseph Graulty May 2015 Approved for the Committee (Biology & Psychology) Enriqueta Canseco-Gonzalez Preface This is an ad hoc Biology-Psychology thesis, and consequently the introduction incorporates concepts from both disciplines. It also provides a considerable amount of information on the phenomenon of synesthesia in general. For the reader who would like to focus specifically on the experimental section of this document, I include a “Background Summary” section that should allow anyone to understand the study without needing to read the full introduction. Rather, if you start at section 1.4, findings from previous studies and the overall aim of this research should be fairly straightforward. I do not have synesthesia myself, but I have always been interested in it. Sensory systems are the only portals through which our conscious selves can gain information about the external world. But more and more, neuroscience research shows that our senses are unreliable narrators, merely secondary sources providing us with pre- processed results as opposed to completely raw data. This is a very good thing. It makes our sensory systems more efficient for survival- fast processing is what saves you from being run over or eaten every day. But the minor cost of this efficient processing is that we are doomed to a life of visual illusions and existential crises in which we wonder whether we’re all in The Matrix, or everything is just a dream.
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Neurological Signs
    FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page i A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS SECOND EDITION FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page iii A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS SECOND EDITION A.J. LARNER MA, MD, MRCP(UK), DHMSA Consultant Neurologist Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Liverpool Honorary Lecturer in Neuroscience, University of Liverpool Society of Apothecaries’ Honorary Lecturer in the History of Medicine, University of Liverpool Liverpool, U.K. FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page iv A.J. Larner, MA, MD, MRCP(UK), DHMSA Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery Liverpool, UK Library of Congress Control Number: 2005927413 ISBN-10: 0-387-26214-8 ISBN-13: 978-0387-26214-7 Printed on acid-free paper. © 2006, 2001 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, Inc., 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dis- similar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to propri- etary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omis- sions that may be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Training, Hypnosis, and Drugs: Artificial Synaesthesia, Or Artificial Paradises?
    HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE published: 14 October 2013 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00660 Training, hypnosis, and drugs: artificial synaesthesia, or artificial paradises? Ophelia Deroy 1* and Charles Spence 2 1 Centre for the Study of the Senses, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London, UK 2 Department of Experimental Psychology, Crossmodal Research Laboratory, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Edited by: The last few years have seen the publication of a number of studies by researchers Roi C. Kadosh, University of Oxford, claiming to have induced “synaesthesia,” “pseudo-synaesthesia,” or “synaesthesia-like” UK phenomena in non-synaesthetic participants. Although the intention of these studies Reviewed by: has been to try and shed light on the way in which synaesthesia might have been Beat Meier, University of Bern, Switzerland acquired in developmental synaesthestes, we argue that they may only have documented David Luke, University of a phenomenon that has elsewhere been accounted for in terms of the acquisition of Greenwich, UK sensory associations and is not evidently linked to synaesthesia. As synaesthesia remains *Correspondence: largely defined in terms of the involuntary elicitation of conscious concurrents, we suggest Ophelia Deroy, Centre for the Study that the theoretical rapprochement with synaesthesia (in any of its guises) is unnecessary, of the Senses, School of Advanced Study, University of London, Senate and potentially distracting. It might therefore, be less confusing if researchers were to House, R 276, Malet Street, WC1E avoid referring to synaesthesia when characterizing cases that lack robust evidence of a 7HU London, UK conscious manifestation. Even in the case of those other conditions for which conscious e-mail: [email protected] experiences are better evidenced, when training has been occurred during hypnotic suggestion, or when it has been combined with drugs, we argue that not every conscious manifestation should necessarily be counted as synaesthetic.
    [Show full text]
  • Sleep Disorders
    University of Zagreb school of Medicine Zagreb, croatia volUMe 4, nUMber 3–4, pp. 249–361 JUly–deceMber 2017 Udc 616.8 Udc 616.89 Udc 159.9 Udc 612 issn 1849-5427 (online) issn 2459-5233 (print) Volume 4, Number 3–4, pp. 249–361 July–December 2017 249–361 July–December pp. Number 3–4, 4, Volume Gyrus Gyrus sleep disorders Rhythmic variations What color are your Menstrual cycle and Pediatric hydrocephalus: of mood letters? – an insight cognitive functions causes, symptoms and Sleep Disorders Sleep into synesthesia treatment modalities p. 304 p. 319 p. 343 p. 355 sleep disorders impressum IMPRESSUM founded 2013 zagreb, croatia UDC 616.8 UDC 159.9 ISSN 1849-5427 (ONlINe) volume 4, number 3 – 4, pp. 249 – 361 UDC 616.89 UDC 612 ISSN 2459-5233 (PrINt) july – december 2017 EDITOR-IN-CHIEF/ GLAVNA UREDNICA Barbara Tomić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine DEPUTY EDITOR-IN-CHIEF/ ZAMJENIK GLAVNE UREDNICE Ivan Mlinarić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine ASSOCIATE EDITORS/ PRIDRUŽENI UREDNICI Mislav Čaić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Marta Dugonjić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Nina Fajs University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Bea Hohšteter University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Ida Ivek University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Vinka Kugelman University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Niko Njirić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Karlo Toljan University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Luka Turkalj University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Ivan Vukelić University of Zagreb, School of Medicine Ivana Car University
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Synaesthesia in Synaesthetes and Its
    MUSICAL SYNAESTHESIA IN SYNAESTHETES AND ITS MANIFESTATION AS A WIDER PHENOMENON c. MARK BEAUMONT. Submitted as part of a PhD assessment by the Department of Music of the University of Sheffield. SEPTEMBER 2003. 1 Acknowledgements While claiming full responsibility for the work contained in this volume, and consequently request that the work be acknowledged as mine when used for future academic research, and permission sought in all other cases, its production would not have been possible without much support, formal and informal, technical and academic and cooperation. While the sources of such assistance are too many to list on this page and while this thesis is no exception to the assertion that every piece of academic writing has its story-of-the-making to tell, I feel it especially appropriate to thank Eric Clarke, my supervisor in the Music Department for his guidance. Second on the list is my gratitude to the many people who have been able to advise me with regards to the various areas that this interdisciplinary work has included. All ideas that were not my own ultimately, of course, came from available sources, but to be able to almost rely on people for inspiration has been a countless asset throughout these years. Finally, every thesis relies on outcomes which in tum generally depend on empirical research. This requires people who used to be called 'subjects' but who we now refer to as 'participants'. I believe that this change of language is a good thing since it encourages experimenters not to take them for granted and to ensure that their experience in the experimentation room is a possible one.
    [Show full text]
  • Music-Colour Synaesthesia: Concept, Context and Qualia
    This is a repository copy of Music-colour synaesthesia: Concept, context and qualia. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/129726/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Curwen, C. (2018) Music-colour synaesthesia: Concept, context and qualia. Consciousness and Cognition, 61. pp. 94-106. ISSN 1053-8100 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.04.005 © 2018 Elsevier. This is an author produced version of a paper subsequently published in Consciousness and Cognition. Uploaded in accordance with the publisher's self-archiving policy. Article available under the terms of the CC-BY-NC-ND licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) Reuse This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs (CC BY-NC-ND) licence. This licence only allows you to download this work and share it with others as long as you credit the authors, but you can’t change the article in any way or use it commercially. More information and the full terms of the licence here: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Music-Colour Synaesthesia: Concept, Context and Qualia Caroline Curwen Department of Music, The University of Sheffield, Jessop Building, 34 Leavygreave Road, Sheffield S3 7RD Corresponding Author: Caroline Curwen Mailing address: 44 Queens Drive, Heaton Mersey, Stockport, SK4 3JW Mobile phone: 07967 206785 E-mail: [email protected] 0 1 Introduction Synaesthesia is a relatively rare condition that manifests itself in approximately four percent of the population.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural Medicine and Neuroplasticity
    XI International Conference of Lithuanian Neuroscience Association BEHAVIOURAL MEDICINE AND NEUROPLASTICITY 29 November 2019 Vytautas Magnus University Kaunas Lithuania 2019 pixabay image 1 ORGANIZERS SCIENTIFIC AND ORGANIZING COMMITTEE Prof. Osvaldas Rukšėnas (Vilnius University, Lithuania) Prof. Saulius Šatkauskas (Vytautas Magnus University, Kaunas, Lithuania) Prof. Aušra Saudargienė (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences; Vytautas Magnus University, Kaunas, Lithuania) Dr. Rima Naginienė (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania) Prof. Vilmantė Borutaitė (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania) Prof. Neringa Paužienė (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania) Dr. Gytis Svirskis (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania) Prof. Aleksandr Bulatov (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania) Assoc. Prof. Ramunė Grikšienė (Vilnius University, Lithuania) Dr. Inga Griškova-Bulanova (Vilnius University, Lithuania) Dr. Aleksandras Pleskačiauskas (Vilnius University, Lithuania) ISBN 978-609-467-426-6 (Online) © Lithuanian Neuroscience Association. All rights reserved https://doi.org/10.7220/9786094674266 Abstracts were reviewed by Scientific Committee 2 SPONSORS 3 PROGRAM Venue: Vytautas Magnus University Small Conference Hall (2nd floor), S. Daukanto st. 28 Kaunas, Lithuania 8.30-9.30 Registration. Coffee / Tea 9.30–9.40 Opening and welcome Prof. Osvaldas Rukšėnas, President of the Lithuanian Neuroscience Association Representatives from Vytautas
    [Show full text]
  • The Visually-Evoked Auditory Response
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Fassnidge, C. (2018). The Visually-Evoked Auditory Response. (Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City, Universtiy of London) This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/19689/ Link to published version: Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] The Visually-Evoked Auditory Response Christopher James Fassnidge A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY CITY, UNIVERSITY OF LONDON FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY March 2018 Table of Contents TABLE OF CONTENTS ............................................................................................................. 2 LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Synaesthesia in the Poetry and Poetics of the Early Twentieth Century
    Words of Shape and Shade: Synaesthesia in the Poetry and Poetics of the Early Twentieth Century Fiona Elizabeth Burrows 10329989 BA (Hons 1), The University of Western Australia, 2006 This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of The University of Western Australia School of Social Sciences (English and Cultural Studies) 2012 i ii Words of Shape and Shade: Synaesthesia in the Poetry and Poetics of the Early Twentieth Century I fell in love - that is the only expression I can think of - at once, and am still at the mercy of words… Out of them came the gusts and grunts and hiccups and heehaws of the common fun of the earth; and though what the words meant was, in its own way, often deliciously funny enough, so much funnier seemed to me, at that almost forgotten time, the shape and shade and size and noise of the words as they hummed, strummed, jigged and galloped along. - Dylan Thomas, "Notes on the Art of Poetry." The Poems of Dylan Thomas, Ed. Daniel Jones. (New York: New Directions Publishing, 2003). E.E Cummings, Noise Number 1, 1919, Oil on Canvas. iii iv ABSTRACT Synaesthesia is a neurological condition defined by the Oxford English Dictionary as “the production of a sense impression relating to one sense or part of the body by stimulation of another sense or part of the body”. This thesis examines the link between poetic and synaesthetic concepts, establishing the relevance of synaesthetic metaphor, sensory imagery and sound symbolism to the British and American poetry – the major English language poetics – of the early twentieth century.
    [Show full text]
  • The New High 2013
    The New High 2013 Designer Drugs and New Trends Donald W. Reid 2013 25I-NBOMe (25I) slide 4 Ketamine slide 24 Kratom slide 41 Pump-It Powder slide 55 Methoxetamine slide 66 Benzo Fury slide 76 Smoking Alcohol slide 89 Waxy Marijuana slide 99 Butane Extraction Process slide 111 Zannie Air Freshener slide 124 Molly (Bath Salts) slide 143 Electronic Cigarettes with THC slide 154 Alcohol Enema slide 162 Liquid Meth slide 170 Cheeba Chews slide 188 Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Pills slide 194 Donald W. Reid 2013 FAIR USE NOTICE Due to this material being collected from various open sources, it may contain copyrighted material the use of which has not been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. I believe this constitutes a ‘fair use’ of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law. In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, this presentation is distributed without profit for the public good, primarily intended for those in Law Enforcement, First Responders, Firefighters, Medical Personnel, Customs Personnel, Teachers and Parents for comment and nonprofit educational purposes. For more information go to: http://www.copyright.gov/title17/92chap1.html#107. Donald W. Reid 2013 25I-NBOMe (25I) Donald W. Reid 2013 Street names 2C-I-NBOMEe; 25I-NBOMe; 25I; N- Bomb; Smiles Donald W. Reid 2013 Overview Donald W. Reid 2013 25I-NBOMe (25I) is a new research chemical considered to be a hallucinogen with psychedelic, stimulant, euphoriant effects. Drug users perceive this chemical as a “legal version” of LSD. 25I is very potent and is active at extremely low (μg - microgram level) doses, most frequently observed as blotter paper doses.
    [Show full text]