<<

Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 363–367

Impaling in true (Laniidae): A behavioral and ontogenetic perspective

Reuven Yosef a,∗, Berry Pinshow b

a International Birding and Research Centre in Eilat, P.O. Box 774, 88000 Eilat, Israel b Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology, Jacob Blaustein Institute for Desert Research, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Sede Boqer Campus, 84993 Midreshet Ben-Gurion, Israel Received 11 September 2004; received in revised form 14 February 2005; accepted 14 February 2005

Abstract

The impaling of prey is a behavioral trait restricted to the true shrikes (Laniidae). Here, we suggest the ontogeny of this behavior. We believe impaling originated from wedging behavior that occurs among several other groups of , including corvids. Accidental impaling during wedging was likely the behavioral precursor of purposeful impaling. Fidelity to impaling sites lead to the creation of caches, which were eventually used by females for male evaluation. Caching allowed males to increase their fitness by using the caches as a display to attract potential mates. Further, caching is used by shrikes to demarcate territories, store food for inclement weather or periods of stress in the breeding cycle, divide labor between the breeding pair, and for “aging” while deterrent chemicals in prey decompose. © 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Laniidae; True shrikes; Impaling; Caching; Ontogeny; Evolution; Territoriality; Diet; Feeding behavior; Courtship

The role of impaling behavior in the biology of the Busbee, 1976). Other taxa have behaviors similar to true shrikes (Laniidae) has been extensively studied impaling. For example, some Australian Butcherbirds (e.g., Brewster, 1894; Watson, 1910; Donovan, 1929; (Craticidae spp.; Pizzey and Knight, 1997) and African Miller, 1937; St. Paul and Gwinner, 1967; Beven and Boubou Shrikes ( ferrugineus; Sonnenschein England, 1969; Wemmer, 1969; Yosef and Pinshow, and Reyer, 1984) are known to wedge prey, apparently 1988; Esely and Bollinger, 2003) and other to save handling time, however, these never im- (e.g., Young et al., 2004). However, the evolution- pale prey on thorns. By contrast, in the Laniidae, verte- ary development of this unique tool is unstudied al- brate prey is impaled on sharp objects, decapitated and, though its ontogeny in young shrikes is well known in most cases, the brain consumed before other body (Miller, 1931; Wemmer, 1969; Smith, 1972, 1973; parts (Yosef, unpublished data). Impaling is defined as the skewering of prey on a ∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +972 3671290; fax: +972 86335319. sharp projection; “wedging” is the placing of the item E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Yosef). in the fork of a substrate (branch, barbed wire, etc.)

0376-6357/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.023 364 R. Yosef, B. Pinshow / Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 363–367 that allows the predator to manipulate and feed on a food (Yosef and Pinshow, 1989; Sarkozi and Brooks, prey item. In some cases, while the predator attempts 2003). During the period of prenuptial display, male to wedge the prey it may be accidentally impaled on shrikes were observed to impale inedible objects such a sharp projection, but intentional and systematic im- as rags, snail and eggshells, fæcal sacs or even bread paling, as it occurs in the true shrikes has not been crusts, ostensibly to boost the visual effect of the cache documented in any non-Laniidaen species. Both (Durango, 1956; Lorenz and St. Paul, 1968; Yosef and wedging and impaling are considered tools that facili- Pinshow, 1988; Leonard, 1992). In this light, it was tate dismemberment of prey at lower energetic costs to suggested that male quality was evaluated, not only by the predator (Schon, 1994; Cade, 1995). the visual impression of the conspicuous cache, but also Most studies of impaling have concentrated on by the composition of the impaled prey items; this even the Loggerhead ( ludovicianus), Red-backed (L. though caches were continuously subject to kleptopara- collurio), Woodchat (L. senator) and Great Grey sitism (Yosef, 1989). In addition, fitness was positively (L. excubitor). Field observations confirm that the abil- correlated to initial cache size. Not only was repro- ity to impale prey develops in the young of these ductive success higher in males with larger caches, but species in the first 4–5 weeks after fledging (Miller, also polygynous breeding occurred in them more often 1931; Cade, 1967; Wemmer, 1969; Smith, 1973; Bus- (Yosef and Pinshow, 1988; Yosef et al., 1991). bee, 1976; Yosef, unpublished data). Although Lorenz Esely and Bollinger (2003) found a positive correla- and St. Paul (1968) concluded that impaling was innate tion between impaled prey abundance and the number in young shrikes, they suggested that it could only be of fledglings produced per successful nest. The facts mastered by practice and experience. Busbee (1976) that shrikes persist in maintaining conspicuous caches also considered experience an important component in even when their young have hatched and that they im- the development of vertebrate predatory behavior and pale inedible objects such as egg shells and fæcal sacs concluded that a definite ontogeny must occur before under the continual threat of kleptoparasites, which impaling proficiency could be achieved. These ideas may even endanger their young, have prompted several are consistent with the findings of Ashmole and To- authors to suggest that this exhibitionism has a role in var (1968) and Recher and Recher (1969) showing that reducing the energetic costs of territorial defense dur- capture rate, capture efficiency, prey size and handling ing the most strenuous part of the birds annual life cycle time improved with age in avian predators indicating (Mizzel, 1993). In fact, Mizzel (1993) and Sarkozi and that there is a learning component in avian predatory Brooks (2003) hypothesized that impaling plays a role behavior. in advertisement of territory ownership and the territo- Shrikes are similar to diurnal raptors (Falconi- rial boundaries defended. formes) in their predatory behavior, but in contrast they Other explanations that have been proffered to ex- do not posses the strong talons that the larger raptors plain impaling behavior. For example, many studies on use to dismember prey (Schon, 1994; Cade, 1995; Van- foraging have suggested that caching or hoarding, i.e., der Wall, 1990). It has been suggested that shrikes have the creation of a larder for later use, is the major func- overcome this limitation by evolving impaling behav- tion of impaling (Karasawa, 1976, 1982; Carlson, 1985; ior that allows them to increase the size of exploitable Vander Wall, 1990; Severinghaus and Liang, 1995). food items (Whyte, 1887; Olsson, 1985; Yao, 1985). Burton (1999) proposed that the impaling of old nest Wemmer (1969) and Valera et al. (2001) found that material by shrikes for later use results in inedible ob- the unpredictable and transitory nature of food surplus jects remaining impaled after the construction of a new seemed crucial to initiate impaling behavior. Moreover, nest is accomplished. Burton suggested that this behav- the composition of caches of impaled food was found to ior could have been the progenitor of the impaling of be a function of the distribution and accessibility of the large prey items for dismemberment. Applegate (1977) prey (Whyte, 1887; Durango, 1956; Kobayashi, 1980; theorized that impaling might serve to clearly divide Yosef and Grubb, 1993; Tryjanowski et al., 2003). sex-specific tasks between the parents, i.e., males hunt Caches made prior to pair bonding and the start of and impale while the females feed themselves and their the breeding season may be indicative to females of young from the caches. Yosef and Whitman (1992) re- male quality and the potential parental ability to supply ported an interesting exaptation of impaling behavior. R. Yosef, B. Pinshow / Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 363–367 365

Fig. 1. Ontogeny of impaling behavior in true shrikes, Laniidae.

They found that aposematically colored and chemically ing sites has carried over into their adult behavior. If defended lubber grasshoppers (Romalea guttata) were viewed in the perspective of evolutionary time, and tak- impaled by loggerhead shrikes, aged until the deterrent ing into account that no other avian species has ever chemicals decomposed, and eventually consumed. gone beyond the stage of dabbing or wedging (Smith, Consolidating at this juncture (Fig. 1), we posit that 1973), we consider the fidelity to favorite impaling sites impaling is an innate behavioral characteristic that is to be the basis for the evolution of this behavioral trait refined by learning and practice. Fledgling shrikes are which evolved from simple dabbing by fledglings, to known to play with inedible objects while still being wedging while gaining experience, and eventually to fed by their parents and to indulge in “dragging” behav- proficient impaling in adults that are now able to ex- ior that later develops into what was termed “dabbing” ploit prey that would otherwise be inaccessible. (Smith, 1972; aka “Tupfbewegungen”; Lorenz and St. Based on our observations in the Sede Boqer area, Paul, 1968). Once the fledglings are able to catch prey Israel (1986–1989), and at the MacArthur Agro- for themselves, they develop individual variations in ecology Research Centre, in Florida (1989–1993), we their prey handling. consider sexual selection by females to have been a The sole use of impaling by fledglings is to assist selective force behind the various exaptations of impal- in the dismemberment of prey. The young also return ing that have been reported. The pattern of change in to impale where they successfully impaled previously, impaling behavior might have gone like this: in the past usually at places where they engaged in dabbing (Yosef, when females arrived at the breeding grounds, they unpublished data). We think that this fidelity to impal- evaluated the male and his territory quality by visually 366 R. Yosef, B. Pinshow / Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 363–367 identifying the locations of the cache sites and their fected as impaling is unique in nature and requires fur- contents. Responding to what the females were using as ther study to understand the ecological processes driv- an index of quality, the males probably not only resorted ing its development and diversification in the Laniidae. to killing prey beyond their own needs and impaling This is paper number 479 of the Mitrani Department the excess in their caches, but also to adding small prey of Desert Ecology. items that could otherwise be consumed with relatively little handling cost. In order to enhance the visual impression of the cache, the males also impaled ined- ible, but often colorful, objects. This is probably when References aposematically colored, chemically defended insects Applegate, R.D., 1977. Possible ecological role of food caches of the were impaled—initially as visual cues, but that after . Auk 94, 391–392. a time detoxified and became edible (Yosef and Whit- Ashmole, N.P., Tovar, H., 1968. Prolonged parental care in royal man, 1992; Yosef et al., 1996; Fuisz and Yosef, 2001). terns and other birds. Auk 85, 90–100. The culmination of this process is that males with the Beven, G., England, M.D., 1969. The impaling of prey in shrikes. biggest caches mate with the earliest arriving females, Br. Birds 62, 192. Brewster, W., 1894. Note on the habits of the . Auk resulting in higher than average number of nesting at- 9, 329–330. tempts and a subsequently large number of fledged Burton, K.M., 1999. Use of barbed wire by Loggerhead Shrikes (La- young (Yosef and Pinshow, 1989; Esely and Bollinger, nius ludovicianus) to manipulate nest materials. Am. Midl. Nat. 2003), or in extreme cases resulting in polygyny with 142, 198–199. a male having two females breeding in his territory in Busbee, E.L., 1976. The ontogeny of cricket killing and mouse killing in Loggerhead Shrike Lanius ludovicianus. Condor 78, 357–365. parallel (Yosef, 1992a; Yosef et al., 1991; Lorek, 1991; Cade, T.J., 1967. Ecological and behavioral aspects of predation by Olsson, 2001; Probst, 2001). the Northern Shrike. Living Bird 6, 43–86. During the courtship feeding stage, which is rela- Cade, T.J., 1995. Shrikes as predators. Proc. Western Found. Vert. tively short in comparison to other , the food Zool. 6, 1–5. offered to the female by the male is invariably fresh. Carlson, A., 1985. Central place food foraging: a field experiment with Red-backed Shrikes (Lanius collurio). Behav. Ecol. Socio- Later, when the female is mostly incubating or brood- biol. 16, 317–322. ing the nestlings, the male can supplement the prey Donovan, H.E., 1929. Larder of Red-backed Shrike. Br. Birds 23, brought to the nest with items from his cache and re- 96. plenish the cache when time is available. We have noted Durango, S., 1956. Territory in the Red-backed Shrike Lanius collu- that male Great Grey Shrikes do not feed hatchlings rio. Ibis 98, 476–484. Esely, J.D., Bollinger, E.K., 2003. Patterns of impaling in a migratory until they are at least 5 days old—the nest is the exclu- population of the Loggerhead Shrike. Prairie Nat. 35, 1–8. sive domain of the female and she does all the feeding Fuisz, T.I., Yosef, R., 2001. Effect of habitat quality on the hunting (Yosef, 1992b). Incubating or brooding females have behaviour of breeding Great Grey Shrikes (Lanius excubitor). been observed flying to and eating directly from the Acta Zool. Acad. Sci. Hungarica 47, 311–320. cache. The amount of prey in the caches drops signif- Karasawa, K., 1976. Observations on impalings made by shrikes. Tori 25, 94–100. icantly during the fledgling and post-fledging period, Karasawa, K., 1982. Observations of predatory behavior of Bull- when the family consumes all the prey and the parents headed Shrikes Lanius bucephalus in an early nesting period. impale only for dismembering the larger prey items. Tori 31, 57–58. The pieces are then transported to the family (Yosef, Kobayashi, K., 1980. The insecta impaled by the Bull-headed Shrike 1992b). In most of the cases, where we observed the de- Lanius bucephalus bucephalus and the weekly variation of the number of impaled prey. Gensei 37, 1–6. capitation of an impaled vertebrate, the male consumed Leonard, D.L., 1992. Monthly variation in a Loggerhead Shrike the brain and the head, and the body was delivered to cache in central Florida. Florida Field Nat. 20, 104–107. the female and the young. Lorek, G., 1991. Poligynia u Srokosza (Lanius excubitor). Przegl. To conclude, through evolutionary time the Lani- Zool. 35, 349–353. idae have modified the behavioral adaptation of im- Lorenz, K., St. Paul, U.V., 1968. Die Entwicklung des Spiessens und Klemmens bei den drei Wurgerarten Lanius collurio, L. senator paling from a tool to dismember into a wide range of und L. excubitor. J. Ornithol. 109, 137–156. exaptations. The behavioral adaptation that begins as Miller, A.H., 1931. Systematic revision and natural history of the dragging, then dabbing and wedging, and is then per- American shrikes (Lanius). Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 38, 11–242. R. Yosef, B. Pinshow / Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 363–367 367

Miller, A.H., 1937. A comparison of behavior of certain North Amer- Valera, F., Kristin, A., Hoi, H., 2001. Why does the Lesser Grey ican and European shrikes. Condor 39, 119–122. Shrike (Lanius minor) seldom store food? Determinants of im- Mizzel, K.L., 1993. The impaling behavior of the Loggerhead Shrike paling in an uncommon storing species. Behaviour 138, 1421– (Lanius ludovicianus) in eastern Texas. M.Sc. Thesis. Stephen F. 1436. Austin State University, Nacogdoches, Texas. Vander Wall, S.B., 1990. Food Hoarding in Animals. The University Olsson, V., 1985. The winter habits of the , Lanius of Chicago Press, Chicago. excubitor. IV. Handling Prey, Var Fagelvarld 4, 269–283. Watson, J.R., 1910. The impaling instinct in shrikes. Auk 27, 459. Olsson, V., 2001. Polygyny in the Red-backed Shrike Lanius collurio. Wemmer, C., 1969. Impaling behaviour of the Loggerhead Shrike Ornis Svecica 11, 97–99. Lanius ludovicianus. Z. f. Tierpsychol. 26, 208–224. Pizzey, G., Knight, F., 1997. The Field Guide to the Birds of . Whyte, J., 1887. Object of the shrike in impaling its prey. Auk 4, 77. AngusandRobertson, Sydney, p. 542. Yao,C.T., 1985. Preliminary study of the impaling behavior of Lanius Probst, R., 2001. Succesful bigamy in the Great Grey Shrike Lanius schach. Ann. Taichung Bird Club, 8–15. e. excubitor. Ornis Fennica 78, 93–96. Yosef, R., 1989. The breeding biology of the Great Grey Shrike and Recher, H.F., Recher, J.A., 1969. Comparative foraging efficiency of the ecological implications of its impaling prey. M.Sc. Thesis. adult and immature Little Blue Herons (Florida caerula). Anim. Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva.n. Behav. 17, 320–322. Yosef, R., 1992a. Behavior of polygynous and monogamous Logger- Sarkozi, D.L., Brooks, D.M., 2003. Eastern red bat (Lasiurus bo- head Shrikes, and a comparison with Northern Shrikes. Wilson realis) impaled by a Loggerhead Shrike (Lanius ludovicianus). Bull. 104, 747–749. Southwest Nat. 48, 301–303. Yosef, R., 1992b. From nest building to fledging of young in Great Schon, M., 1994. Morphologische Grundlagen einiger Greifvogel- Grey Shrikes at Sede Boqer, Israel. J. Ornithol. 133, 279–285. Eigenschaften des Raubwurgers (Lanius excubitor). Okol. Vogel Yosef, R., Grubb Jr., T.C., 1993. Effect of vegetation height on 16, 1–10. hunting behavior and diet of Loggerhead Shrikes. Condor 95, Severinghaus, L.L., Liang, C.T., 1995. Food and foraging behavior 127–131. of the Lanius cristatus in Taiwan. Proc. Western Yosef, R., Pinshow, B., 1988. Polygyny in the Northern Shrike, La- Found. Vert. Zool. 6, 194–199. nius excubitor, in Israel. Auk 105, 581–582. Smith, S.M., 1972. The ontogeny of impaling behavior in the Log- Yosef,R., Pinshow, B., 1989. Cache size influences reproductive suc- gerhead Shrike Lanius ludovicianus. Behaviour 42, 232–247. cess in the Northern Shrike, Lanius excubitor. Auk 106, 418–421. Smith, S.M., 1973. Food manipulation by young Passerines and the Yosef, R., Whitman, D.W., 1992. Predator exaptations and defensive possible evolutionary history of impaling by shrikes. Wilson Bull. adaptations in evolutionary balance: no defense is perfect. Evol. 85, 318–322. Ecol. 6, 527–536. Sonnenschein, E., Reyer, H.-U., 1984. Biology of the Slatecoloured Yosef, R., Carrel, J.E., Eisner, T., 1996. Contrasting reactions of Boubou and other bush shrikes. Ostrich 55, 86–96. Loggerhead Shrikes to two types of chemically defended insect St. Paul, U.V., Gwinner, E., 1967. Raubwurger (Lanius excubitor) prey. J. Chem. Ecol. 22, 173–180. und Rotkopfwurger (L. senator) enstacheln Hymenopteren. Die Yosef, R., Mitchell, W.A., Pinshow, B., 1991. The proximate costs Vogelwelt, 2. and benefits of polygyny to male Northern Shrikes. Wilson Bull. Tryjanowski, P., Karg, M.K., Karg, J., 2003. Diet composition and 103, 146–149. prey choice by the Red-backed Shrike Lanius collurio in western Young,K.V.,Brodie Jr., E.D., Brodie III, E.D., 2004. How the horned Poland. Belg. J. Zool. 133, 157–162. lizard got its horn. Science 304, 65.