Colour and the Cortex: Wavelength Processing In

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Colour and the Cortex: Wavelength Processing In CHAPTER4 COLOUR AND THE CORTEX: WAVELENGTHPROCESSING IN CORTICALACHROMATOPSIA CHARLESA. HEYWOOD, ROBERTW. KENTRIDGE, AND ALAN COWEY Introduction One of the perceptual consequencesof being able to processwavelength differences in the distribution of spectrallight is the chromatic world in which we live. The privacy of colour experienceis undisputedand is reflectedin the term'quale'coinedby C.I. Lewisin 1929to describesuch qualitative content of mental states.However, notwithstanding the philo- sophicaldebate that such a notion hasfuelled, few would deny that we live in a colourful world-never more so,perhaps, since the l8-year-old William Henry Perkin serendipitously discovereda purple aniline dye while attempting to synthesizequinine from coal tar in the mid-nineteenthcentury, and spawnedthe wealthof dyesand pigmentsthat arecommon- placetoday. What doesit mean to havecolour vision, i.e.what are its defining characteristics,and what advantagesdoes it confer on its possessor?Ascribing colour vision to an organism requiresno more, and no less,than the demonstrationthat two spectrallydifferent stimuli, made equally bright with respectto an animal's spectral sensitivity,are discriminable. Alternatively,colour vision can be demonstratedwhen discrimination remains possible when random fluctuations in brightness are introduced into two stimuli of different spec- tral distributions,i.e. the discrimination must be made on the basisof colour differences alone.Almost all vertebratesand some invertebrateshave colour vision, some in a rudi- mentary form, but differ in the number of photoreceptortypes and their spectralcharac- teristics.Trichromacy, however, is the norm for all Old World monkeys,apes, and people. But what role doescolour play in vision?One way this canbe addressedis by assessingthe variation in colour vision acrossa variety of specieswith respectto the visual environ- ments they inhabit. Good examplesare the demonstration that the spectraltuning of long- and middle-wavelengthretinal conepigments of frugivorousplatyrrhine monkeysis optimal for detecting their dietary fruits concealedagainst a variegatedfoliage (Regan et al., 1998)whereas that of African Old World monkeys and chimpanzeesis appropriatelr' tuned for detectingyoung green shoots among other and lesstasty foliage.This revealstn'o of the selectivepressures that haveoperated on primate vision during the evolutionot dichromacyand trichromacy.One role of colour vision, therefore,appears to be the rapid detectionof a particularobiect colour rvhenluminance differences alone rvould providc CULUUK AI\U TN ambiguousclues to its location. Moreover,a stationary object standsout i': ' '.rck- ground on the basisof, among others,luminance, texture, and chromati. ':.es. Spatialor temporalvariations of luminancewould maskan object'scontours ,, :Jr it invisible to a monochromatic observer,suggesting a further role for colour '. - :he segmentationof the visual scene.Finally, colour variation assistsin the rc'., - ': ,ri objects.For example,there is ampleevidence that colour playsa conspicuousr,' . ...r1 signalling,the identificationof conspecificsor, to usea much citedexample, an ..' . -:: of the ripenessof dietaryfruit from its externalappearance. However,there are attendant problems in the designof a systemwhere reflect:.. - ' differentially reflect light of wavelengthsthat constitute the visible spectrunr spectralcomposition of the illuminant can vary widely from moment to mt':- throughout the day.In the natural environment,much of the former variation dc:'. whether an object is illuminated by direct sunlight or the shorterwavelengths ot .. illumination producedby Rayleighscattering. The latter arisesfrom a combinati,': greatersusceptibility of long-wavelengthlight to atmosphericabsorption and thc :: . attenuationof shorterwavelengths as a resultof scattering,the effectof which incr..,- - ' the sun approachesthe horizon.It haseven been suggested that the principalopf,'::. axis of trichromatic vision, blue-yellowand red-green,is an adaptationto such nat..:. - occurring variation in terrestrial illumination (Shepard,1992) . Yet colours remaiIr ; . ceptuallyconstant, and suchconstancy must be a prerequisiteof a biologicalsystem rr'ir :. i-. fulfils the purported roles assignedto it, namely the facilitation of object detection. segmentationand, most notably,identification. Another clue to the role of colour vision can be derivedfrom behaviouralstudies oi peoplein whom colour vision is depleted,as in casesof retinal colour blindness,or r.'hcr. colour vision hasbeen perhaps entirely deletedas a result of a cerebralaccident. It is n,'',' establishedthat damageto human ventromedialoccipitotemporal cortex .un .J.15..;t,., '..: vision, a condition known ascerebral achromatopsia (Damasio et al., 1980;Kijlmcl. " ' Heywood et al., l99l). What light can the examinationof such patientsshed on thc :'. - siblecosts and benefitsto thosewho possesschromatic vision? One consequenceof brain injury canbe a selectivedisorder of vision.f{1r\r{iii. ii - selectivityof a deficitis only asnarrow or broadas the specificitvof the bch.rvi,,.:r.,.i.,'r that is usedto studyit. Examplescome from visualagnosia (the lossoi lisu.rl l.u'.::, ': :.,- : . - orauditoryrecognitionofobjects,withoutlinguistic,sensor\-,orattentiott.tl l,'.. ..:: :' -.' agnosia,appropriate tasks can further pinpoint the impairn.rentin sontcP.rli(r::. .,\ .: \- .. tiveloss of visualrecognition of living comparedu'ith non-living,itenr:. or litL ..:. ":'i, other patients.Alternatively, the recognitionof facesmav be selectilclv J i.l .:: :'. .: ..- ' prosopagnosia,but evenin the latterdisorder deficits can be confinedto .iit:i.'.:.:.r. i . recognitionoffacialexpression,gender,oridentity(seeCorver'. 199-1.J,';;1'. 1q:. .'.l -- the refinementof behaviouraltasks can more accuratelv def-ine n'hat is lti.l.'.r::.:: ' ':' is equallyinformative about the componentProcesses of a particularI irtt.i.: -.:r.: i certainvisual functions, particularly those that arecomnronlv dc'scri[.cc .i. .' ;' might seemunlikelv that the surprisingdissociations of the sort descril'cJi:: ::'.. : * -- orderprocessing of objectand face rvill be observed. For ntnenronic or liniu:.::- .:':'-." colourprocessing, brain damage can disrupt the abiliti'to nantL'.1 it,..';.::r.i '..'- :' ' - l.t'rintto ir namedcolour, as in colouranomia iOxburv cr,l/.. 1969 . (r!-\::.:.::': :.-- .- but sparethe latter,as in disordersof short-term colour memory (Davidoffand Ostergaard, 1984).The deficit may be confined to a failure to respond correctly when askedto provide the appropriatecolour name when confrontedwith the verballabel of a common object, 'What for example, colour is a banana?'(Kinsbourne and Warrington, 1967;Lszzattiand Davidoff, 1994).However, in none of theseinstances is there any difficulty in telling colours apart and colour vision itself, that is the experienceand discrimination of colours, remainsundisturbed. Nevertheless, our experiencewith monochrome imagesreadily allowsus to imagine a world devoid of colour. This, and the evidencethat colour can be processedpreattentively (Tieisman and Gelade,1980) and may therefore constitute a visual primitive, together with the abundant physiological evidencethat colour is processedrela- tively independently,might encourageus to believethat processingof wavelengthvariation in the visual scenecan be selectivelydestroyed. The descriptionofpatients with cerebral achromatopsia,also called cortical colour blindness,where brain damageappears to have selectivelyabolished colour vision, confirms this belief (seeZeki, 1990;Cowey and He1'wood,1997, for reviews).But perhapsthe very easewith which we can (unlike, for example,the perceptionof shape)imagine a world in which colour hasbeen deleteddis- couragesexamination of the degreeto which other aspectsof wavelengthprocessing may be spared.Thus, colour vision being the phenomenalcounterpart of the processingof wavelengthvariation, the loss of the former may be held to imply an absenceof the latter. Does the absenceof colour vision preclude the use of wavelengthdifferences to determine other objectproperties? It is becoming increasinglyapparent that the preoccupationwith the discrimination paradigm to characterizethe qualities ofthe visual input, by requiring decisionsto be made about a stimulus array,neglects the nature of the responserequirements (seeMilner and Goodale,1996,for review).Conventionally, it is assumedthat whether visual choices in a discriminationtask entail a verbal,pointing, or any other motor responseis unimpor- tant. Recentreports make it is clear that certain motor output systemshave privileged accessto different visual inputs. For example, intact visual pathways can still accomplish visuomotor or other orienting responses,albeit in the absenceof consciousawareness of the properties of the visual stimuli that elicit them. And the clinical condition of blindsight is characterizedby an absenceof acknowledgedvisual awarenesspatients in patients who neverthelessperform well at forced-choiceguessing tasks. Blindsight has been considered 'unconscious an example of vision',but equally likely it is the residual motor capacitiesthat remain, but which do not contribute to the consciouspercept. This view has been used to account for visuomotor abilities in casesof visual form agnosia,notably patient DF, whose impaired discrimination of orientation and shapecoexists with accurateand appropriate visuomotor actsrequiring the coding of thesestimulus attributesfor their successful execution
Recommended publications
  • Cortical Mechanisms of Colour Vision
    REVIEWS CORTICAL MECHANISMS OF COLOUR VISION Karl R. Gegenfurtner The perception of colour is a central component of primate vision. Colour facilitates object perception and recognition, and has an important role in scene segmentation and visual memory. Moreover, it provides an aesthetic component to visual experiences that is fundamental to our perception of the world. Despite the long history of colour vision studies, much has still to be learned about the physiological basis of colour perception. Recent advances in our understanding of the early processing in the retina and thalamus have enabled us to take a fresh look at cortical processing of colour. These studies are beginning to indicate that colour is processed not in isolation, but together with information about luminance and visual form, by the same neural circuits, to achieve a unitary and robust representation of the visual world. SENSORY SYSTEMS It has been known since the nineteenth century that In the retinal ganglion cells, three channels convey there are three types of photoreceptor for daylight information from the eye to the brain7,8. In the L + M or vision in the human eye1,2. Since then, the shapes of the luminance channel, the signals from L- and M-cones spectral absorption functions of the different cones are added to compute the intensity of a stimulus. In the have been determined with increasing precision in L – M colour-opponent channel, the signals from L- and psychophysical experiments and verified using various M-cones are subtracted from each other to compute electrophysiological methods3. On the molecular the red–green component of a stimulus.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychophysical Investigations Into the Neural Basis of Synaesthesia V
    doi 10.1098/rspb.2000.1576 Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia V. S. Ramachandran* andE.M.Hubbard Center for Brain and Cognition, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, LaJolla, CA 92093-0109, USA We studied two otherwise normal, synaesthetic subjects who `saw' a speci¢c colour every time they saw a speci¢c number or letter. We conducted four experiments in order to show that this was a genuine perceptual experience rather than merely a memory association. i)The synaesthetically induced colours could lead to perceptual grouping, even though the inducing numerals or letters did not. ii)Synaesthetically induced colours were not experienced if the graphemes were presented peripherally. iii)Roman numerals were ine¡ective: the actual number grapheme was required. iv)If two graphemes were alternated the induced colours were also seen in alternation. However, colours were no longer experienced if the graphemes were alternated at more than 4 Hz. We propose that grapheme colour synaesthesia arises from `cross-wiring' between the `colour centre' area V4 or V8)and the `number area', both of which lie in the fusiform gyrus. We also suggest a similar explanation for the representation of metaphors in the brain: hence, the higher incidence of synaesthesia among artists and poets. Keywords: synaesthesia; multisensory; extrastriate visual areas; psychophysics that the visual number grapheme rather than the numer- 1. INTRODUCTION ical concept is required for evoking colours. However, they The bizarre phenomenon of synaesthesia, e.g. `seeing both noted that if they `imaged' the corresponding Arabic sounds' or seeing speci¢c colours upon seeing speci¢c numeral then the corresponding colour was evoked, but numerals Galton 1880, 1883; Cytowic 1989; Paulesu et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Cerebral Mechanisms Operating in the Presence and Absence of Inflammatory Pain
    Annals ofthe Rheumatic Diseases 1996; 55: 411-420 41 REVIEW: PAIN Series editor: Bruce L Kidd Ann Rheum Dis: first published as 10.1136/ard.55.7.411 on 1 July 1996. Downloaded from Cerebral mechanisms operating in the presence and absence of inflammatory pain Anthony K P Jones, Stuart W G Derbyshire During episodes ofinflammatory arthritic pain, surprising if such parallel processing did not the brain has the task of integrating new also exist for non-primary sensations such as information from receptors of muscle, tendon, pain. There is now growing evidence that pain and skin afferents with nociceptive information is processed by a network or 'matrix' of struc- from the joint capsule and, possibly, peri- tures in the brain (fig 1), some components of articular bone. This integration will incorpor- which could be considered to comprise parallel ate motivational, affective, discriminative, at- but integrated processing. tentional, and motor responses that may result in changes of behaviour and modification of central nervous system (CNS) processing of NEUROANATOMY OF NOCICEPTION IN ANIMALS pain. It is as yet unclear what the nature of this Studying pain in animals presents an insoluble CNS modification may be, and suggested problem. Human beings can call out and influences on pain related affective-motor express their pain in ways that are commonly responses or the inflammatory process itself understandable to other members of their remain largely speculative. species; unfortunately, there is no equivalent It is likely to be some time before we begin frame of reference when dealing with animals. to have a proper understanding of the complex Increased blood pressure, movements of integrated processes that operate during withdrawal, dilatation of the pupil, increased arthritic pain and other chronic pains.
    [Show full text]
  • Retinotopy and Color Sensitivity in Human Visual Cortical Area V8
    © 1998 Nature America Inc. • http://neurosci.nature.com articles Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 Nouchine Hadjikhani, Arthur K. Liu, Anders M. Dale, Patrick Cavanagh and Roger B. H. Tootell Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129, USA Correspondence should be addressed to N.H. ([email protected]) Prior studies suggest the presence of a color-selective area in the inferior occipital-temporal region of human visual cortex. It has been proposed that this human area is homologous to macaque area V4, which is arguably color selective, but this has never been tested directly. To test this model, we compared the location of the human color-selective region to the retinotopic area boundaries in the same subjects, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), cortical flattening and retinotopic mapping techniques. The human color-selective region did not match the location of area V4 (neither its dorsal nor ventral subdivisions), as extrapolated from macaque maps. Instead this region coincides with a new retinotopic area that we call ‘V8’, which includes a distinct representation of the fovea and both upper and lower visual fields. We also tested the response to stimuli that produce color afterimages and found that these stimuli, like real colors, caused preferen- tial activation of V8 but not V4. In Old World primates such as macaque monkeys and humans, ing studies, a small patch of color-selective activity near the mid- visual information about color is processed in anatomically dle of the collateral sulcus has been named ‘V4’ (ref.
    [Show full text]
  • The Anatomy of Cerebral Achromatopsia: a Reappraisal and Comparison of Two Case Reports
    The anatomy of cerebral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports. Paolo Bartolomeo, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten To cite this version: Paolo Bartolomeo, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten. The anatomy of cere- bral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports.: Anatomy of cerebral achro- matopsia. Cortex, Elsevier, 2014, 56, pp.138-44. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.013. inserm-00803853 HAL Id: inserm-00803853 https://www.hal.inserm.fr/inserm-00803853 Submitted on 23 Mar 2013 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Preprint version of the paper published on Cortex, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.013 The anatomy of cerebral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports Paolo Bartolomeo1-3, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi4-7, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten1,8 1 Inserm U975; UPMC-Paris6, UMR_S 975; CNRS UMR 7225, Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, 75013 Paris, France 2 AP-HP,
    [Show full text]
  • Diamond for Neural Interfacing: a Review
    Carbon 102 (2016) 437e454 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Carbon journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/carbon Review article Diamond for neural interfacing: A review * David J. Garrett a, , Wei Tong a, David A. Simpson a, Hamish Meffin b a School of Physics, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia b National Vision Research Institute, Department of Optometry and Vision Science University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia article info abstract Article history: The variety of materials that fall in the diamond category is vast and increasing. From single, to micro to Received 9 December 2015 nano-crystalline, diamond can be highly electrically insulating or synthesized to possess varying degrees Received in revised form of conductivity. Diamond in all its forms is without peer in terms of chemical stability exhibits excellent 3 February 2016 biocompatibility and is famously mechanically robust. Diamond is however, a difficult material with Accepted 20 February 2016 which to fabricated devices owing to its extreme hardness, lack of ductility and weldability. New syn- Available online 26 February 2016 thesis and fabrication methods in recent decades have overcome some of these drawbacks and diamond has enjoyed a surge in interest as a biomedical material. In the field of neural interfaces a grand goal is permanent, high fidelity connections with neural populations. Diamond's longevity, biocompatibility and biochemical inertness make it a highly promising material with which to achieve this goal. This review covers recent uses of diamond in the three critical areas of neural interfacing: diamond as a growth substrate, as a neurochemical sensor electrode material and as a direct neural recording and stimulation material.
    [Show full text]
  • Computational Model of Visual Perception: from Colour to Form
    ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en Computational Model of Visual Perception: From Colour to Form A dissertation submitted by SeyedArash Akbarinia to the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona in fulfil- ment of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Departament de Ciències de la Computació. Bellaterra, July 13, 2017 Director Dr. C. Alejandro Párraga Centre de Visió per Computador Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Thesis Dr. Marcelo Bertalmío committee Department of Information and Communication Technologies Universitat Pompeu Fabra Dr. Nicolai Petkov Intelligent Systems University of Groningen Dr. Joost van de Weijer Centre de Visió per Computador Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona International Dr. Casimir Ludwig evaluators School of Experimental Psychology University of Bristol Dr. Thorsten Hansen Department of General and Experimental Psychology Justus-Liebig-Universität This document was typeset by the author using LATEX 2". The research described in this book was carried out at the Centre de Visió per Computador, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. Copyright © 2017 by SeyedArash Akbarinia. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the author.
    [Show full text]
  • Very Early Responses to Colour Stimuli Detected in Prestriate
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/106047; this version posted February 6, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 Very Early Responses to Colour Stimuli Detected in Prestriate Visual Cortex by Magnetoencephalography (MEG) Yoshihito Shigihara, Hideyuki Hoshi and Semir Zeki Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London Authors: Yoshihito Shigihara, Hideyuki Hoshi, and Semir Zeki Affiliation: Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT Corresponding author: Semir Zeki ([email protected]); University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT TEL/FAX, +44 (0) 20 7679 7316 Journal section: Research Articles Short title: Very early responses to colour Keywords: colour vision, early cortical latencies, Area V4, prestriate cortex, V1-bypassing input. bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/106047; this version posted February 6, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 2 Abstract Our previous studies with the visual motion and form systems show that visual stimuli belonging to these categories trigger much earlier latency responses from the visual cortex than previously supposed and that the source of the earliest signals can be located in either the prestriate cortex or in both the striate (V1) and prestriate cortex. This is consistent with the known anatomical connections since, in addition to the classical retino-geniculo-striate cortex input to the prestriate visual areas, there are direct anatomical inputs from both the lateral geniculate nucleus and the pulvinar that reach the prestriate visual cortex without passing through striate cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurological Basis of Synesthesia
    OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN P a r t V NEUROLOGICAL BASIS OF SYNESTHESIA 224_Simner_C24.indd4_Simner_C24.indd 447373 55/29/2013/29/2013 110:10:130:10:13 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN 224_Simner_C24.indd4_Simner_C24.indd 447474 55/29/2013/29/2013 110:10:130:10:13 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN Chapter 24 Synesthesia and functional imaging Edward M. Hubbard Introduction Researchers have debated the neural mechanisms that give rise to synesthesia since the earliest days of synesthesia research (e.g., Flournoy 1893). However, it is only with the advent of sophisticated neuroimaging techniques like positron emission tomography (PET) and, more recently, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that these questions could be empirically addressed by examining patterns of brain activation in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike. Since the fi rst attempt to measure brain activ- ity related to synesthetic experiences over 25 years ago (Cytowic and Stump 1985), our understanding of brain functions and the sophistication of neuroimaging methods has increased dramatically. Th ese advances have led to a number of neurophysiologically sophisticated models of synesthesia, and to a wealth of studies aimed at testing them. Here, I will not dis- cuss studies using methods such as electroencephalography (Brang et al. 2011; Niccolai, Wascher, and Stoerig 2012) or magnetoencephalography (Brang et al. 2010) although in many cases the fi ndings using these other methods converge with those from neuroim- aging methods (for a review of studies using these other methods, see Hubbard et al. 2011; J ä ncke, Chapter 28; Ramachandran and Brang, Chapter 48, this volume).
    [Show full text]
  • Grapheme-Colour and Tone-Colour Synaesthesia Is Associated
    1 Grapheme-Colour and Tone-Colour Synaesthesia is Associated with Structural Brain Changes in Visual Regions Implicated in Colour, Form and Motion Michael J Banissy1,2, Lauren Stewart2, Neil G Muggleton1, 3, Timothy D Griffiths4, 5, Vincent Y Walsh1, Jamie Ward6 & Ryota Kanai1 1 UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London, U.K.. 2 Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, U. K. 3 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli 320, Taiwan. 4 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, U.K. 5 Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle, U.K. 6 Sackler Centre for Consciousness Sciences and School of Psychology, University of Sussex, U.K. Address correspondence to :- Michael Banissy, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AR, U.K. Tel. : 020-7679-5438, Fax. : 020-7916-8517 E-mail : [email protected] 2 Abstract Synaesthesia is a rare condition in which stimulation in one modality leads to a secondary experience in another sensory modality. Varying accounts attribute the condition to either neuroanatomical differences between the synaesthetes and non- synaesthetes or functional differences in how sensory brain regions interact. This study employed voxel-based-morphometry to examine whether synaesthetes who experience both grapheme-colour and tone-colour synaesthesia as their evoked sensation show neuroanatomical differences in gray matter volume compared to non- synaesthetes. We observed that synaesthetes showed an increase in gray matter volume in left posterior fusiform gyrus, but a concomitant decrease in anterior regions of left fusiform gyrus and left MT / V5. These findings imply that synaesthesia for colour is linked to neuroanatomical changes between adjacent regions of the visual system.
    [Show full text]
  • 'How One Can See What Is Not There': Neural Mechanisms of Grapheme
    ‘How one can see what is not there’: Neural mechanisms of grapheme-colour synaesthesia Tessa M. van Leeuwen Front cover: My synaesthesia for letters and digits, overlaid on the surface of a brain (brain image generated with SPM, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The research reported in this thesis was carried out at the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, of the Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands, with financial support from the Volkswagen-Foundation [grant number I/80 743]. ISBN 978-94-91027-10-9 Printed by Ipskamp Drukkers, Enschede, the Netherlands © Tessa M. van Leeuwen, 2011 ‘How one can see what is not there’: Neural mechanisms of grapheme-colour synaesthesia Een wetenschappelijke proeve op het gebied van de Sociale Wetenschappen Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. mr. S.C.J.J. Kortmann, volgens besluit van het College van Decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op woensdag 14 september 2011 om 15:30 uur precies door Tessa Marije van Leeuwen geboren op 26 juli 1980 te Leiderdorp Promotor: Prof. dr. P. Hagoort Manuscriptcommissie: Prof. dr. M. Ullsperger Dr. R. Rouw (Universiteit van Amsterdam) Dr. R. Willems Contents 1. Introduction: About synaesthesia 7 2. Colour specificity in the human V4 complex 21 3. Synaesthetic colour in the brain: Beyond colour areas 37 4. Effective connectivity determines the nature of subjective 63 experience in grapheme-colour synaesthesia: A brief communication 5. Altered colour processing as indicated by alpha band oscillations can 79 provide an explanation for grapheme-colour synaesthesia 6.
    [Show full text]
  • Achromatopsia, Color Vision, and Cortex Charles A
    Neurol Clin N Am 21 (2003) 483–500 Achromatopsia, color vision, and cortex Charles A. Heywood, DPhila,*, Robert W. Kentridge, PhDb aDepartment of Psychology and Wolfson Research Institute, University of Durham, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom bDepartment of Psychology, University of Durham, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom Few researchers would deny the advantages that color vision confers on those who possess it. In low-level vision, color assists in segmenting the visual scene on the basis of chromatic boundaries, when fluctuating shadows create spurious luminance borders that camouflage the contour of an object, thereby rendering it invisible to a monochromatic observer. In higher-level vision, color enhances identification and recognition. It can provide in- formation about the state of objects, for example its oft-cited role in in- dicating the ripeness of dietary fruit. In this respect, it is notable that the spectral tuning of retinal photoreceptors in African Old World monkeys and chimpanzees are exquisitely matched to maximize the discriminability of young green shoots concealed among less succulent foliage [1]. In fru- givorous platyrrhine monkeys, such tuning is optimal for the detection of dietary fruits concealed against variegated foliage [2,3]. In addition, color may be used in the identification of conspecifics or to signal sexual recep- tivity. Color also improves visual recognition memory for natural scenes by facilitating the encoding and retrieval of images, particularly when they are endowed in their natural and characteristic colors [4–6]. The ubiquity of color is reflected in the considerable amount of neural tissue devoted to the analysis of color information. Neuroanatomic and neurophysiologic studies have described the functional properties of visual pathways in the primate.
    [Show full text]