Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Display a Place Response in a Swimming Pool: a Dissociation Between Getting There and Knowing Where

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Display a Place Response in a Swimming Pool: a Dissociation Between Getting There and Knowing Where The Journal of Neuroscience, August 1995, 75(8): 5779-5788 Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Display a Place Response in a Swimming Pool: A Dissociation Between Getting There and Knowing Where Ian Q. Whishaw,’ Jean-Christophe Cassel,* and Leonard E. Jarrard3 ‘Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada TlK 3M4, XNRS, UPR 419, Universitk Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg, France, and 3Department of Psychology, Washington and Lee University, Lexington, Virginia Some theories of hippocampal formation function postu- the platform from any location on the pool’s edge (Morris, late that it is involved in using the relationships between 1981). Rats with hippocampaldysfunction, produced by hippo- distal cues for spatial navigation. That rats with damage to campal removal (Morris et al., 1982; Sutherland et al., 1982a; the hippocampal formation are impaired in learning place Whishaw, 1987), damage to its efferents and afferents in the responses of escaping to a platform hidden just below the fimbria-fornix (Casseland Kelche, 1989; Sutherland and Rod- surface of the water of a swimming pool, supports this riguez, 1989), or cholinergic blockade (Sutherland et al., 1982b; view. Using rats with fimbria-fornix (FF) lesions, we ex- Whishaw, 1985a,b; Whishaw and Tomie, l987), are severely amined whether their impairment is related to an inability impaired. Since the samestudies show that the rats remain ex- to learn how to reach the platform as opposed to learning cellent swimmersand can quickly learn to escapeto a visible its location. In a first experiment, the FF rats were impaired platform, the results suggestthat the hippocampusis selectively in learning to swim to a hidden platform but could swim to involved in using the relational properties of ambient cues to a visible platform. In a second experiment, after being pre- guide movements. trained to swim to a visible platform, the FF rats swam to, Despite evidence that disruption of hippocampal function im- paused, and searched the vicinity of the platform’s previ- pairs spatial behavior, it is difficult to demonstratethat this is ous location when it was removed. This finding showed due to an inability to use the relations between ambient cues. that the FF rats expected to find the platform at that loca- As first demonstratedby Krechevsky (1938), problem solving tion. Additional tests confirmed that they had learned a involves at least two processes:discovering the task’s solution place response. Despite having acquired a place response, and then learning the task. Similarly, in swimming pool tasks, they still could not acquire new place responses when only animalsmust first learn to swim, discover that there is an escape, the hidden platform training procedure was used. Thus, and find that effective guidance requires distal cues (Sutherland these results in dissociating the processes of “getting and Dyck, 1984; Whishaw and Petrie, 1988). In fact, during the there” and “knowing where” suggest that the FF rats’ im- pairment may be in some process of motoric control, such first swimming trials, the rats engage in a variety of behaviors as path integration, rather than in learning the location of such as scrabbling at the edge of the pool, swimming around its the platform in relation to ambient cues. The results are edge, making sorties into the center of the pool, swimming in discussed in relation to relevant theories of hippocampal circles, etc., suggestingthat they are searchingfor an appropriate function. solution. Once a solution is found, however, new place re- [Key words: configural learning and space, fimbria-fornix sponses,even in tests in novel locations, can be acquired within and space, hippocampus and space, path integration, one trial (Whishaw, 1985c, 1989). This raises the question of place learning, rat spatial learning, spatial navigation] whether retarded place learning following hippocampaldamage residesin finding and using an appropriate solution or “getting A number of theories of hippocampal function suggest that it there,” rather than, or in addition to, loss of the ability to know has a role in using the relations between ambient cues to guide a location using ambient cues or “knowing where.” movements(Hirsh, 1974; O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978; Sutherland The presentstudy addressesthis question by demonstratingin and Rudy, 1989; Jarrard, 1993). In an exemplar task, rats placed a first experiment that rats with fimbria-fornix (FF) lesionsare in a swimming pool, in which a small escapeplatform is hidden impaired in acquiring a place response.Then in a second ex- just beneath the water’s surface, will learn to swim directly to periment, FF rats are trained with a visible cue using a gradual adaptationprocedure to determine whether first having been pro- Received Jan. 12, 1995; revised Mar. 30, 1995; accepted Apr. 6, 1995. vided with an acquisition strategy they demonstratesubsequently This research was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and En- that have acquired a place responseas do control rats (Whishaw gineering Research Council of Canada, by a fellowship from Ministere de la Recherche et de la Technologie to I.Q.W., and grants from the Direction des and Mittleman, 1986). Behavior is then assessedon probe trials Recherches et Etudes Techniques (93086). We thank Joanne Tomie for assis- with kinematic measuresof swim direction, distance,and veloc- tance. ity, to determine whether the rats expected to find a platform in Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Ian Q. Whishaw at the above address. the pool and, more importantly, to determine whether they ex- Copyright 0 1995 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/95/l 55779- 10$05.00/O pect to find it at its previous location. In addition to this central 5780 Whishaw et al. - Place Learning in Fimbria-Fornix Rats experiment, control studies were performed to characterize fur- which the automatic systems followed the average exposed surface of ther the spatial abilities of the rats. the rat did not produce reliable results.) Histology. At the completion of the experiments, the rats were deeply anesthetized, perfused with saline and saline-formalin, and the brains Materials and Methods were removed and stored in a 30% sucrose formalin solution. The brains were cut in 40 pm sections on a cryostat and alternate sections were Methods with cresyl violet and stained for acetylcholinesterase. Animals. Thirty-six male Long-Evans rats (University of Lethbridge vivarium) were used. They were housed in pairs in plastic bowls in a laboratory with room temperature maintained at 20-21°C and lighted Procedure on a 12/12 hr light/dark cycle. For 2 weeks after surgery, the rats were Two experiments were performed. In the first experiment, the perfor- taken to the test room, removed from their cages, and handled for about mance of FF rats and control rats was assessed on a place task that 5 min. At the time the experiment began, the rats weighed 400-500 consisted of escaping to a hidden platform using conventional training gm. procedures. In the second .experiment, similar groups of rats were first Surgery. Prior to the beginning of the handling procedures, the rats trained to swim to the visible platform and were then evaluated on probe received bilateral cathodal lesions that severed the axons of the fimbria- trials to determine whether they attended to distal ambient cues. The fornix. Cathodal current was passed for 40 set through 00 stainless steel same groups were subsequently given a number of additional probe and insect pins insulated with epoxylite except at the surface of their tips. training tests to confirm that they had learned a place response. Lesions were made at two sites in ea?h hemisphere using coordinates in reference to bregma and the surface of the dura: 1.3 mm posterior, 1.5 mm lateral, and 3.6 mm ventral, and 1.5 mm posterior, 0.5 mm Experiment I: the effect of FF lesions on place learning lateral, and 3.3 mm ventral. Behavioral testing began 15 d following Eleven rats with fimbria-fornix lesions and nine control rats were used. surgery. The hidden platform was located in the NE quadrant of the pool. Each Swimming pool. The swimming pool was located in a test room in rat received two trials each day and each of the two trials began at one which many cues, including windows covered by blinds, counters, cup- of the four start locations, with starting locations changed each day. The boards, etc., were present. A 146 cm diameter and 45 cm high round trials were given sequentially with an intertrial interval of about 10 sec. white swimming pool was located in the center of the room. It was After the rats had completed 28 trials, the hidden platform was replaced filled to a depth of 25 cm with 22°C water that was made opaque with the addition of 1500 cc of powdered milk. with the visible platform for six trials. Then, the rats received two more Platforms. Depending upon the task, one of two platforms was lo- trials with the platform hidden. Trials were videorecorded, swims were cated in the pool. The visible platform was an 8.5 cm2 wooden block recorded on a map of the pool, and latency was recorded with a stop painted black that protruded from the water for 3 cm. A small shelf watch. below the surface of the water served as a step to help the rats climb onto the platform. The hidden platform was made of clear Plexiglas Experiment II: pretraining on the visible platform followed by and its 11 cm* top was located 1 cm below the surface of the water probe trials where it was not visible to a viewer on the surface of the water. The surface of the platform was serrated so that the rats could obtain pur- Training.
Recommended publications
  • Dissecting the Fornix in Basic Memory Processes and 12 13 Neuropsychiatric Disease: a Review 14 Susan L
    Manuscript 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Dissecting the fornix in basic memory processes and 12 13 neuropsychiatric disease: A review 14 Susan L. Benear, Chi T. Ngo & Ingrid R. Olson 15 Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122 16 17 18 19 20 21 Address correspondence to: 22 23 Ingrid R. Olson E-mail: [email protected] 24 Department of Psychology T: (215) 204-7318 25 Temple University F: (215) 204- 5539 26 th 27 1701 N. 13 Street 28 Philadelphia, PA 19122 29 30 31 32 Number of words in abstract: 169 33 Number of tables, figures: 3 tables and 4 figures 34 35 36 Keywords: fornix, white matter, episodic memory, hippocampus, diffusion tensor imaging, 37 Alzheimer’s Disease, epilepsy, acetylcholine, theta, obesity, nucleus accumbens, deep brain 38 stimulation, schizophrenia, stress, bipolar disorder, mood 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 1 2 3 4 Abstract 5 6 The fornix is the primary axonal tract of the hippocampus, connecting it to modulatory 7 subcortical structures. This review reveals that fornix damage causes cognitive deficits that 8 closely mirror those resulting from hippocampal lesions. In rodents and non-human primates, 9 this is demonstrated by deficits in conditioning, reversal learning, and navigation. In humans, 10 this manifests as anterograde amnesia. The fornix is essential for memory formation because it 11 12 serves as the conduit for theta rhythms and acetylcholine, as well as providing mnemonic 13 representations to deep brain structures that guide motivated behavior, such as when and where 14 to eat.
    [Show full text]
  • An Afferent Hippocampal Fiber System in the Fornix of the Monkey
    An Afferent Hippocampal Fiber System in the Fornix of the Monkey CHARLES L. VOTAW AND EDWARD W. LAUER Department of Anatmy, The University of Michigan, Ann ATboT, Michigan In a previous investigation (Votaw, ’60b) and weighed from 2.2 to 5.3 kgs. Seven it was noted that, in spite of massive bilat- were used only for this investigation and eral removal of the hippocampal formation seven others were used as well in another (Ammon’s horn and dentate gyms), there investigation on the function of the hippo- was a considerable percentage of the fi- campus and are included in this study bers in the fornix, anterior to the hippo- because the lesions placed were ideally carnpal commissure, that did not degener- suited for comparison with the former ate. This would imply, since the lesions seven cases. to all intents and purposes eliminated all All animals were subjected to aseptic possible fibers going into the fornix from surgical procedures. The only surgical pro- the region of the temporal lobe, the pres- cedure on seven was the production of the ence of fibers in the fornix which are a€- lesions to be reported here; lesions were ferent with respect to the hippocampus, placed in the remaining seven after other and further, that these fibers are not com- studies had been carried out. General missural in nature. In another study anesthesia was used and consisted of (Votaw, ’60a), it was shown that lesions ether, sodium pentobarbital (20-25 mg/kg in the septa1 area would produce a de- of body weight given intravenously) or a generation which could be followed from combination of the two anesthetics, ether the area of the lesion in a caudal direction, being used to supplement a somewhat coursing through the body of the fornix, lower dose of sodium pentobarbital.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroanatomy Dr
    Neuroanatomy Dr. Maha ELBeltagy Assistant Professor of Anatomy Faculty of Medicine The University of Jordan 2018 Prof Yousry 10/15/17 A F B K G C H D I M E N J L Ventricular System, The Cerebrospinal Fluid, and the Blood Brain Barrier The lateral ventricle Interventricular foramen It is Y-shaped cavity in the cerebral hemisphere with the following parts: trigone 1) A central part (body): Extends from the interventricular foramen to the splenium of corpus callosum. 2) 3 horns: - Anterior horn: Lies in the frontal lobe in front of the interventricular foramen. - Posterior horn : Lies in the occipital lobe. - Inferior horn : Lies in the temporal lobe. rd It is connected to the 3 ventricle by body interventricular foramen (of Monro). Anterior Trigone (atrium): the part of the body at the horn junction of inferior and posterior horns Contains the glomus (choroid plexus tuft) calcified in adult (x-ray&CT). Interventricular foramen Relations of Body of the lateral ventricle Roof : body of the Corpus callosum Floor: body of Caudate Nucleus and body of the thalamus. Stria terminalis between thalamus and caudate. (connects between amygdala and venteral nucleus of the hypothalmus) Medial wall: Septum Pellucidum Body of the fornix (choroid fissure between fornix and thalamus (choroid plexus) Relations of lateral ventricle body Anterior horn Choroid fissure Relations of Anterior horn of the lateral ventricle Roof : genu of the Corpus callosum Floor: Head of Caudate Nucleus Medial wall: Rostrum of corpus callosum Septum Pellucidum Anterior column of the fornix Relations of Posterior horn of the lateral ventricle •Roof and lateral wall Tapetum of the corpus callosum Optic radiation lying against the tapetum in the lateral wall.
    [Show full text]
  • Low Wnt/B-Catenin Signaling Determines Leaky Vessels in The
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Low wnt/b-catenin signaling determines leaky vessels in the subfornical organ and affects water homeostasis in mice Fabienne Benz1, Viraya Wichitnaowarat1, Martin Lehmann1, Raoul FV Germano2, Diana Mihova1, Jadranka Macas1, Ralf H Adams3, M Mark Taketo4, Karl-Heinz Plate1,5,6,7,8, Sylvaine Gue´ rit1, Benoit Vanhollebeke2,9, Stefan Liebner1,5,6* 1Institute of Neurology (Edinger Institute), University Hospital, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany; 2Laboratory of Neurovascular Signaling, Department of Molecular Biology, ULB Neuroscience Institute, Universite´ libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium; 3Department of Tissue Morphogenesis, Max-Planck- Institute for Molecular Biomedicine, University of Mu¨ nster, Faculty of Medicine, Mu¨ nster, Germany; 4Division of Experimental Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan; 5Excellence Cluster Cardio-Pulmonary systems (ECCPS), Partner site Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany; 6German Cancer Consortium (DKTK), Partner Site Frankfurt/Mainz, Frankfurt, Germany; 7German Center for Cardiovascular Research (DZHK), Partner site Frankfurt/Mainz, Frankfurt, Germany; 8German Cancer Research Center (DKFZ), Heidelberg, Germany; 9Walloon Excellence in Life Sciences and Biotechnology (WELBIO), Wallonia, Belgium Abstract The circumventricular organs (CVOs) in the central nervous system (CNS) lack a vascular blood-brain barrier (BBB), creating communication sites for sensory or secretory neurons, involved in body homeostasis. Wnt/b-catenin signaling is essential for BBB development and *For correspondence: [email protected] maintenance in endothelial cells (ECs) in most CNS vessels. Here we show that in mouse development, as well as in adult mouse and zebrafish, CVO ECs rendered Wnt-reporter negative, Competing interests: The suggesting low level pathway activity. Characterization of the subfornical organ (SFO) vasculature authors declare that no revealed heterogenous claudin-5 (Cldn5) and Plvap/Meca32 expression indicative for tight and competing interests exist.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Anatomy
    BRAIN ANATOMY Adapted from Human Anatomy & Physiology by Marieb and Hoehn (9th ed.) The anatomy of the brain is often discussed in terms of either the embryonic scheme or the medical scheme. The embryonic scheme focuses on developmental pathways and names regions based on embryonic origins. The medical scheme focuses on the layout of the adult brain and names regions based on location and functionality. For this laboratory, we will consider the brain in terms of the medical scheme (Figure 1): Figure 1: General anatomy of the human brain Marieb & Hoehn (Human Anatomy and Physiology, 9th ed.) – Figure 12.2 CEREBRUM: Divided into two hemispheres, the cerebrum is the largest region of the human brain – the two hemispheres together account for ~ 85% of total brain mass. The cerebrum forms the superior part of the brain, covering and obscuring the diencephalon and brain stem similar to the way a mushroom cap covers the top of its stalk. Elevated ridges of tissue, called gyri (singular: gyrus), separated by shallow groves called sulci (singular: sulcus) mark nearly the entire surface of the cerebral hemispheres. Deeper groves, called fissures, separate large regions of the brain. Much of the cerebrum is involved in the processing of somatic sensory and motor information as well as all conscious thoughts and intellectual functions. The outer cortex of the cerebrum is composed of gray matter – billions of neuron cell bodies and unmyelinated axons arranged in six discrete layers. Although only 2 – 4 mm thick, this region accounts for ~ 40% of total brain mass. The inner region is composed of white matter – tracts of myelinated axons.
    [Show full text]
  • Diffusion Tensor Tractography of the Mammillothalamic Tract in The
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Difusion tensor tractography of the mammillothalamic tract in the human brain using a high spatial Received: 15 August 2017 Accepted: 19 February 2018 resolution DTI technique Published: xx xx xxxx Arash Kamali 1, Caroline C. Zhang2, Roy F. Riascos1, Nitin Tandon3, Eliana E. Bonafante-Mejia1, Rajan Patel1, John A. Lincoln4, Pejman Rabiei1, Laura Ocasio1, Kyan Younes4 & Khader M. Hasan1 The mammillary bodies as part of the hypothalamic nuclei are in the central limbic circuitry of the human brain. The mammillary bodies are shown to be directly or indirectly connected to the amygdala, hippocampus, and thalami as the major gray matter structures of the human limbic system. Although it is not primarily considered as part of the human limbic system, the thalamus is shown to be involved in many limbic functions of the human brain. The major direct connection of the thalami with the hypothalamic nuclei is known to be through the mammillothalamic tract. Given the crucial role of the mammillothalamic tracts in memory functions, difusion tensor imaging may be helpful in better visualizing the surgical anatomy of this pathway noninvasively. This study aimed to investigate the utility of high spatial resolution difusion tensor tractography for mapping the trajectory of the mammillothalamic tract in the human brain. Fifteen healthy adults were studied after obtaining written informed consent. We used high spatial resolution difusion tensor imaging data at 3.0 T. We delineated, for the frst time, the detailed trajectory of the mammillothalamic tract of the human brain using deterministic difusion tensor tractography. Limbic system of the human brain is a collection of gray and white matter structures that supports a variety of functions including emotion, behavior, motivation, long-term memory, and olfaction.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of the Fornix in Human Navigational Learning
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/391888; this version posted August 21, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. The role of the fornix in human navigational learning Carl J. Hodgetts1, Martina Stefani1, Angharad N. Williams1, Branden S. Kolarik2, Andrew P. Yonelinas3,4, Arne D. Ekstrom5, Andrew D. Lawrence1, Jiaxiang Zhang1 & Kim S. Graham1 (1) Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, Wales, UK (2) Center for the Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, University of California, Irvine, USA (3) Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, California, 95618 (4) Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, California, 95618 (5) Department of Psychology, The University of Arizona, Arizona 85721 Manuscript information Abbreviated title: The fornix and human navigational learning Number of pages: 34 Number of figures: 3 Number of tables: 0 Number of words: Abstract (193); Introduction (648); Discussion (1763) Competing financial interests: The authors declare no competing financial and non-financial interests. Corresponding author: Dr Carl J Hodgetts: Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Maindy Road, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, Wales, UK; Tel: 029 2087 0715; Email: [email protected] 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/391888; this version posted August 21, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
    [Show full text]
  • White Matter Anatomy: What the Radiologist Needs to Know
    White Matter Anatomy What the Radiologist Needs to Know Victor Wycoco, MBBS, FRANZCRa, Manohar Shroff, MD, DABR, FRCPCa,*, Sniya Sudhakar, MBBS, DNB, MDb, Wayne Lee, MSca KEYWORDS Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) White matter tracts Projection fibers Association Fibers Commissural fibers KEY POINTS Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) has emerged as an excellent tool for in vivo demonstration of white matter microstructure and has revolutionized our understanding of the same. Information on normal connectivity and relations of different white matter networks and their role in different disease conditions is still evolving. Evidence is mounting on causal relations of abnormal white matter microstructure and connectivity in a wide range of pediatric neurocognitive and white matter diseases. Hence there is a pressing need for every neuroradiologist to acquire a strong basic knowledge of white matter anatomy and to make an effort to apply this knowledge in routine reporting. INTRODUCTION (Fig. 1). However, the use of specific DTI sequences provides far more detailed and clini- DTI has allowed in vivo demonstration of axonal cally useful information. architecture and connectivity. This technique has set the stage for numerous studies on normal and abnormal connectivity and their role in devel- DIFFUSION TENSOR IMAGING: THE BASICS opmental and acquired disorders. Referencing established white matter anatomy, DTI atlases, Using appropriate magnetic field gradients, and neuroanatomical descriptions, this article diffusion-weighted sequences can be used to summarizes the major white matter anatomy and detect the motion of the water molecules to and related structures relevant to the clinical neurora- from cells. This free movement of the water mole- diologist in daily practice.
    [Show full text]
  • …By the Way, Where Is the Fornix???
    Resources …By the way, – H. Blumenfeld. Neuroanatomy through clinical cases where is the fornix??? (Sinauer 2002). – Digital anatomist: • http://www9.biostr.washington.edu/da.html An introduction to gross neuroanatomy –Sylvius: • http://www.sylvius.com/ Marco L. Loggia, PhD [email protected] Some slides kindly provided by E. Duerden, UMontreal. Brigham and Women’s Hospital (Anesthesiology) Mass General Hospital (Psychiatry) All images and animations included in this presentation are from the Digital Harvard Medical School Anatomist website, unless otherwise specified. Orientation Orientation Humans, however, have an upright posture… VENTRAL = towards the belly (=‘ventrum’ in latin) DORSAL = towards the back (=‘dorsum’in latin) ROSTRAL = towards the snout (‘rostrum’=beak in latin) ABOVE CAUDAL = towards the tail (=‘cauda’ in latin) M-D junction BELOW M-D junction In animals with a linear organization of the CNS, terminology is straightforward: = Watch out! ‘Superior’=‘Dorsal’ above the midbrain; =‘Rostral’ in the midbrain or below Blumenfeld, 2002. © Sinauer (2002) Sylvius.com Blumenfeld (adapted). © Sinauer (2002) 1 Orientation Orientation MEDIAL = close to the midline LATERAL = close to the sides Horizontal (axial/transverse) Coronal Sagittal LATERALMEDIAL LATERAL Horizontal Sagittal Coronal Think about the horizon! Imagine a tiara-like crown! Think about the bow of an archer! VENTRAL Blumenfeld. © Sinauer (2002) Major subdivisions Orientation of the encephalon Telencephalon Horizontal (axial/transverse) Coronal Sagittal -Cereb.
    [Show full text]
  • Loss of Fornix White Matter Volume As a Predictor of Cognitive Impairment in Cognitively Normal Elderly Individuals
    Research Original Investigation Loss of Fornix White Matter Volume as a Predictor of Cognitive Impairment in Cognitively Normal Elderly Individuals Evan Fletcher, PhD; Mekala Raman, BA; Philip Huebner; Amy Liu, BA; Dan Mungas, PhD; Owen Carmichael, PhD; Charles DeCarli, MD IMPORTANCE Magnetic resonance imaging markers of incipient cognitive decline among healthy elderly individuals have become important for both clarifying the biological underpinnings of dementia and clinically identifying healthy individuals at high risk of cognitive decline. Even though the role of hippocampal atrophy is well known in the later stages of decline, the ability of fornix-hippocampal markers to predict the earliest clinical deterioration is less clear. OBJECTIVES To examine the involvement of the hippocampus-fornix circuit in the very earliest stages of cognitive impairment and to determine whether the volumes of fornix white matter and hippocampal gray matter would be useful markers for understanding the onset of dementia and for clinical intervention. DESIGN A longitudinal cohort of cognitively normal elderly participants received clinical evaluations with T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging and diffusivity scans during repeated visits over an average of 4 years. Regression and Cox proportional hazards models were used to analyze the relationships between fornix and hippocampal measures and their predictive power for incidence and time of conversion from normal to impaired cognition. SETTING A cohort of community-recruited elderly individuals at the Alzheimer Disease Center of the University of California, Davis. PARTICIPANTS A total of 102 cognitively normal elderly participants, with an average age of 73 years, recruited through community outreach using methods designed to enhance ethnic diversity.
    [Show full text]
  • Dorsal/Ventral Hippocampus, Fornix, and Conditioned Place Preference
    HIPPOCAMPUS 11:187–200 (2001) Dorsal/Ventral Hippocampus, Fornix, and Conditioned Place Preference Janina Ferbinteanu* and R.J. McDonald Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada ABSTRACT: Conditioned place preference (CPP) is a learning paradigm The reason behind differences in behavioral outcomes requiring formation of associations between reward and particular loca- may be that the two lesions produce distinct types of tions. White and McDonald (Behav Brain Res 1993;55:269–281) demon- disruptions of HPC function. These may have similar strated that amygdala (AMG) lesions impair, while fornix (Fx) lesions enhance learning of this task. In the present experiments, we replicated effects in some testing paradigms, but not others. The the effects of AMG and Fx lesions, but we also found that complete HPC proper, constituted of the dentate gyrus (DG) and hippocampal (HPC) lesions interfere with normal performance. Thus, the CA3–CA1 fields, receives cortical input from the ento- effects of Fx and HPC lesions on CPP are opposite. This is in contrast with rhinal cortex (EC) through the perforant path (PP). Its spatial learning in the water maze. Because it has been demonstrated that output back to EC is generated in CA1 and the subicu- damage of dorsal HPC interferes to a greater extent with spatial learning than damage of ventral HPC, we also tested animals with either dorsal or lum (S) (Amaral and Witter, 1995). Subcortical HPC ventral HPC disruptions on CPP. Lesions limited to dorsal HPC were inputs/outputs are accomplished through the Fx, a col- followed by impairment on this task. In contrast, lesions limited to ventral lection of fibers which connects HPC with septum, nu- HPC resulted in enhanced learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Downloaded 10/09/21 02:52 AM UTC J
    LABORATORY INVESTIGATION J Neurosurg 127:209–218, 2017 Comparative anatomical analysis of the transcallosal-transchoroidal and transcallosal-transforniceal-transchoroidal approaches to the third ventricle João Luiz Vitorino Araujo, MD, PhD,1,2 José C. E. Veiga, MD, PhD,2 Hung Tzu Wen, MD, PhD,1 Almir F. de Andrade, MD, PhD,1 Manoel J. Teixeira, MD, PhD,1 José P. Otoch, MD, PhD,1 Albert L. Rhoton Jr., MD,3 Mark C. Preul, MD,4 Robert F. Spetzler, MD,4 and Eberval G. Figueiredo, MD, PhD1 1Division of Neurosurgery, University of São Paulo Medical School; 2Discipline of Neurosurgery, Santa Casa de São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil; 3Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida; and 4Division of Neurological Surgery, Barrow Neurological Institute, St. Joseph’s Hospital and Medical Center, Phoenix, Arizona OBJECTIVE Access to the third ventricle is a veritable challenge to neurosurgeons. In this context, anatomical and morphometric studies are useful for establishing the limitations and advantages of a particular surgical approach. The transchoroidal approach is versatile and provides adequate exposure of the middle and posterior regions of the third ventricle. However, the fornix column limits the exposure of the anterior region of the third ventricle. There is evidence that the unilateral section of the fornix column has little effect on cognitive function. This study compared the anatomical exposure afforded by the transforniceal-transchoroidal approach with that of the transchoroidal approach. In addition, a morphometric evaluation of structures that are relevant to and common in the 2 approaches was performed. METHODS The anatomical exposure provided by the transcallosal-transchoroidal and transcallosal-transforniceal- transchoroidal approaches was compared in 8 fresh cadavers, using a neuronavigation system.
    [Show full text]