Velarization

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Velarization Velarization Velarization is a secondary articulation that include the liquids [!] and ["] as well as the labi- refers to the raising of the tongue body toward als [#] and [$]. However, the phonemic status the back of the soft palate. Classical Arabic of the secondary emphatics is controversial in has four velarized phonemes, /!/, /!/, /"/, and that they are often marginal, not occurring in /#/, all of which are coronal consonants with many words or only occurring in the environ- a primary articulation in the dental-alveolar ment of a low vowel. As an example, although region of the vocal tract. S'bawayhi uses the many dialects are reported to have a velarized term mu!baq ‘covered’ in his Kit"b to describe [!], its phonological behavior is usually distinct the velarized consonants of Arabic (! !i!b"q). from the ‘primary emphatics’ /!/, /!/, /"/, and These consonants are often referred to as pha- /#/. This is because velarized [!] usually only ryngealized. In terms of articulation, a pharyn- occurs in the context of a low vowel and alter- gealized consonant is one made with the root nates with its nonvelarized counterpart when of the tongue drawn toward the back wall the low vowel is not present. An illustration of of the pharynx. Ladefoged and Maddieson this comes from Cairene Arabic, where the /r/ (1996:365) note that Arabic dialects vary as to in the word [kub%$!] ‘big [pl.]’ is pronounced whether they have velarization or pharyngeal- as velarized when adjacent to a low vowel, but ization. There is very little acoustic distinction pronounced as nonvelarized in [kabi$r] ‘big between the two; both are characterized by [sg.]’ when the low vowel is not adjacent. Pri- the lowering of the second formant. The term mary emphatics do not alternate in such a man- ‘emphatic’ is also used to refer to the velarized ner; they are pronounced as velarized regardless consonants. This is a translation of the Arabic of the vocalic context, as exemplified by the term mufaxxama, which is traditionally under- Cairene Arabic pair [na!i$"] ‘active’/[#an!%"] stood to include the uvular consonants as well. ‘more active’. Both uvular and velarized consonants are made The second question that a phonological ana- with the tongue body raised toward the back of lysis of velarization in an Arabic dialect must the soft palate; with the uvular consonants, the address is the extension of velarization beyond tongue body raising is the primary articulation, the single phoneme. In most dialects, when there whereas with the velarized consonants it is a is a velarized phoneme in the word, neighbor- secondary articulation that accompanies the ing sounds are also pronounced as velarized. In primary contact in the dental-alveolar region. some dialects it is only the vowel immediately The English term ‘emphatic’ is typically used adjacent to the velarized consonant that is only for the velarized consonants. pronounced as velarized (e.g. certain dialects of A phonological analysis of velarization in an Saudi Arabia), whereas in other dialects it may Arabic dialect must answer the following two be the entire word that is velarized (e.g. Cairene questions: what are the velarized phonemes of Arabic). Thus, depending on the dialect, the the dialect, and to what extent does velariza- common Arabic word /"ab%&/ ‘morning’ will tion affect neighboring sounds? With respect either be pronounced with just the first syllable to the first question, while almost all modern velarized or with the entire word velarized. The dialects have maintained the velarized conso- effect of velarization is particularly strong with nants, there are some differences from Clas- the low vowel, where it is generally identified sical Arabic. For example, colloquial Cairene as allophonic variation between a low back Arabic has /$/ rather than Classical Arabic /#/, vowel (velarized) and a low front vowel (nonve- while the northern Palestinian dialect described larized). Moreover, detailed investigations of in Herzallah (1990) lacks /!/. Furthermore, velarization in specific dialects (e.g. Ghazeli most dialects are reported to have a variety of 1977; Heath 1987; Herzallah 1990; Younes other velarized consonants that are sometimes 1993; Davis 1995; Shahin 1997; Watson 1999; referred to as ‘secondary emphatics’. These and Zawaydeh 1999) reveal that some dialects vvelarization_1-2.inddelarization_1-2.indd 1 77/18/2008/18/2008 8:57:028:57:02 PMPM 2 velarization display an asymmetry between velarization that B i b l i o g r a p h i c a l r e f e r e n c e s is regressive and velarization that is progressive. Davis, Stuart. 1995. “Emphasis spread in Arabic For example, in the northern Palestinian dia- and grounded phonology”. Linguistic Inquiry 26.465–498. lect discussed in Herzallah (1990), velarization Eid, Mushira and Clive Holes (eds.). 1993. Perspec- extends (or spreads) regressively to the begin- tives on Arabic linguistics, V. Amsterdam and Phila- ning of the word, but the progressive spreading delphia: J. Benjamins. Ghazeli, Salem. 1977. Back consonants and backing of velarization is limited. Thus, when a word coarticulation in Arabic. Ph.D. diss., University of has a velarized consonant at the end, the entire Texas at Austin. word is velarized, as exemplified by /xayy%!/ Heath, Jeffrey. 1987. Ablaut and ambiguity: Phonol- ‘tailor’, which is pronounced entirely velarized. ogy of a Moroccan Arabic dialect. Albany: SUNY Press. But when the velarized consonant is at the Herzallah, Rukayyah. 1990. Aspects of Palestinian beginning, only part of the word would usually Arabic phonology: A nonlinear approach. Ph.D. be velarized, as exemplified by /"ab%&/, in which diss., Cornell University. only the first syllable is velarized. Another man- Ladefoged, Peter and Ian Maddieson. 1996. The sounds of the world’s languages. Oxford: Blackwell. ifestation of the asymmetry between the regres- Royal, Anne Marie. 1985. Male/female pharyngeal- sive and progressive spreading of velarization ization patterns in Cairo Arabic: A sociolinguistic is the observation that in some dialects cer- study of two neighborhoods. Ph.D. diss., Univer- tain sounds (typically those made with a high sity of Texas at Austin. Shahin, Kimary. 1997. Postvelar harmony: An exam- tongue position) block the progressive spread ination of its bases and crosslinguistic variation. of velarization. In the rural Palestinian dialect Ph.D. diss., University of British Columbia. discussed by Younes (1993), the consonants Watson, Janet. 1999. “The directionality of emphasis /š/, /y/, and /w/ block progressive spreading, spread in Arabic”. Linguistic Inquiry 30.289–300. Younes, Munther. 1993. “Emphasis spread in two as exemplified by the word /"iy%m/ ‘fasting’, in Arabic dialects”. Eid and Holes (1993:119–145). which only the initial /"/ is pronounced velar- Zawaydeh, Bushra Adnan. 1999. The phonetics and ized; these consonants do not prevent regressive phonology of gutturals in Arabic. Ph.D. diss., Indi- ana University. spreading, as illustrated by the word /xayy%!/ ‘tailor’, in which the entire word is pronounced Stuart Davis (Indiana University) as velarized. A third asymmetry between regres- sive and progressive spreading of velarization, documented by Zawaydeh (1999) for Ammani Jordanian Arabic, is that regressive spreading is categorical, whereas progressive spreading is gradient. For example, the word /tasalla!/ ‘he overruled’ is pronounced with the first two syllables just as velarized as the third syllable, whereas in the word /%a!al%tak/ ‘your muscles’, the third syllable shows less velarization than the second, and the fourth syllable shows even less velarization than the third. The asymmetries manifested between regressive and progressive assimilation reflect that regressive velarization is stronger than progressive velarization, and this may have a basis in articulation, as sug- gested by Watson (1999). A largely unexplored topic with respect to velarization is its sociolinguistics, namely to what extent, if any, does velarization in a single dialect vary with respect to gender, style, social class, etc. An investigation undertaken by Royal (1985) documented acoustically stronger velar- ization patterns for males than for females. vvelarization_1-2.inddelarization_1-2.indd 2 77/18/2008/18/2008 8:57:038:57:03 PMPM.
Recommended publications
  • Effects of Phonetic and Inventory Constraints in the Spirantization of Intervocalic Voiced Stops: Comparing Two Different Measurements of Energy Change
    Effects of Phonetic and Inventory Constraints in the Spirantization of Intervocalic Voiced Stops: Comparing two Different Measurements of Energy Change. Marta Ortega-LLebaria University of Northern Colorado E-mail: [email protected] This paper extends the same hypothesis to the phenomenon ABSTRACT of spirantization by investigating the interaction of phonetic factors with inventory constraints. First, it This paper examines the effect of inventory constraints and examines whether the phonetic factors of stress and vowel the phonetic factors of stress and vowel context in the context, which had an effect in the lenition of Spanish lenition of English and Spanish intervocalic voiced stops. intervocalic /g/ [3], had also an effect in English Five native speakers of American English and five native intervocalic /g/ and in Spanish and English intervocalic /b/. speakers of Caribbean Spanish were recorded saying Secondly, this paper studies the interaction of inventory bi-syllabic words containing intervocalic /b/ and /g/. The constraints with phonetic factors. Inventory constraints intervocalic consonants were evaluated according to two were aimed to preserve the system of sound contrasts of a measures of energy, i.e. RMS ratio, and speed of consonant language while phonetic factors provided contexts that release. Repeated Measures ANOVAS in each measure favored consonant lenition. Hypothetically, a consonant indicated that for both languages, /b/ and /g/ were most will become lenited in a favorable context only if the lenited in trochee words, and that /g/ was most spirantized resulting sound will not impair a contrast of the language. when flanked by /i/ and /u/ vowels. Inventory constraints For example, in English, /b/ contrasts with the voiced moderated the degree of consonant lenition displayed in the fricative /v/ while in Spanish, it does not.
    [Show full text]
  • The Grammar of Coarticulation Edward Flemming Department of Linguistics & Philosophy, MIT
    The Grammar of Coarticulation Edward Flemming Department of Linguistics & Philosophy, MIT 1. Introduction It is well established that coarticulatory patterns are language-specific and therefore must be specified in the grammars of languages (e.g. Beddor, Harnsberger & Lindemann, 2002, Clumeck, 1976, Huffman, 1988, Keating & Cohn, 1988, Magen, 1984, Manuel, 1990, Manuel & Krakow, 1984, Oh, 2002), but there is less consensus on the nature of the grammar of coarticulation. Here we use evidence from the typology of coarticulatory patterns to argue for a model based on weighted constraints. Through two case studies we see that coarticulation shows typological variation that is parallel in many respects to phonological typology: there are common or universal cross-linguistic patterns, such as F0 transitions between adjacent tones, that point to universal constraints, such as physiological limitations on rates of F0 change, but language- specific variation in the details of coarticulation, e.g. variation in the timing of F0 transitions, shows that languages differ in their responses to these constraints. I propose that this is due to interaction between conflicting constraints: constraints on rate of change interact with perceptually motivated constraints requiring the realization of phonetic targets. These constraints can conflict, with conflicts being resolved by constraint prioritization. The relative priority of constraints can differ from language to language resulting in language-specific patterns of coarticulation. This is essentially the same approach to the analysis of typology that has been successful in Optimality Theoretic analyses of phonology (Prince & Smolensky, 2004). This account of coarticulation has implications for analyses that attempt to explain phonological generalizations in terms of phonologization of coarticulation.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents
    EmanQuotah Linguistics Senior Paper Hadass Sheffer, Advisor Swarthmore College December 9, 1994 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents Introduction 1 TheQuran 3 Recitation and Tajwi:d 7 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation 10 Arabic geminates 12 Nasal assimilation rules 15 Blocking of assimilation by pauses 24 Conclusion 26 Bibliography Grateful acknowledgements to my father, my mother and my brothers, and to Hadass Sheffer and Donna Jo Napoli. Introduction This paper is concerned with the analysis of certain rules governing nasality and nasal assimilation during recitation of the holy Quran. I These rules are a subset of tajwi:d, a set of rules governing the correct prescribed recitation and pronunciation of the Islamic scriptures. The first part of the paper will describe the historical and cultural importance of the Quran and tajwi:d, with the proposition that a tension or conflict between the necessity for clarity and enunciation and the desire for beautification of the divine words of God is the driving force behind tajwi:d's importance. Though the rules are functional rather than "natural," these prescriptive rules can be integrated into a study lexical phonology and feature geometry, as discussed in the second section, since prescriptive rules must work within those rules set by the language's grammar. Muslims consider the Quran a divine and holy text, untampered with and unchangeable by humankind. Western scholars have attempted to identify it as the writings of the Prophet Muhammad, a humanly written text like any other. Viewing the holy Quran in this way ignores the religious, social and linguistic implications of its perceived unchangeability, and does disservice to the beliefs of many Muslims.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk
    Background Methodology and Predictions Results Conclusions References The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau University of California, Berkeley [email protected] [email protected] Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas (SSILA) 2018 Salt Lake City, UT, USA January 6, 2018 Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Background Methodology and Predictions Results Conclusions References Acknowledgements Many thanks to the following: • Karuk master speakers Sonny Davis and the late Lucille Albers, Charlie Thom, and especially Vina Smith; • research collaborators LuLu Alexander, Tamara Alexander, Crystal Richardson, and Florrine Super (in Yreka) and Erik H. Maier, Line Mikkelsen, and Clare Sandy (at Berkeley); and • Susan Lin and the audience at UC Berkeley’s Phonetics and Phonology Forum for insightful comments and suggestions. Data in this talk is drawn from Ararahi’urípih, a Karuk dictionary and text corpus (http://linguistics.berkeley.edu/~karuk). Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Background Methodology and Predictions Metathesis Results Karuk Conclusions References Overview • Karuk V1CV2 sequences show much coarticulation of V1 into V2 w j j /uCi/ ! [uC i], /iCa/ ! [iC a], /iCu/ ! [iC u] (all high V1) • We argue that this coarticulation is a source of CV metathesis along lines that are phonologized in other languages. • Goals • To figure out the environments in which this process occurs •
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Assimilation, Reduction and Elision Reflected in the Selected Song Lyrics of Avenged Sevenfold
    Dwi Nita Febriyanti Assimilation, Reduction and Elision Reflected in the Selected Song Lyrics of Avenged Sevenfold Dwi Nita Febriyanti [email protected] English Language Studies, Sanata Dharma University Abstract This paper discusses the phenomena of phonological rules, especially assimilation, reduction and elision processes. In this paper, the writer conducted phonological study which attempts to find the phenomena of those processes in song lyrics. In taking the data, the writer transcribed the lyrics of the songs, along with checking them to the internet source, then observed the lyrics to find the phenomena of assimilation, reduction, and elision. After that, she classified the observed phenomena in the lyrics based on the phonological processes. From the data analysis, the results showed that there were three processes found both in the first and second songs: assimilation, reduction and elision. The difference is that in the first song, it has four kinds of assimilation, while from the second song only has three kinds of assimilation. Keywords: assimilation, reduction, elision Introduction brothers and sisters’ discussion or even in songs, for which songs are considered as the As English spoken by the native media for the composer to share his feelings. speakers, it sometimes undergoes simplification to ease the native speakers in Assimilation usually happens in the expressing their feelings. That is why, it is double consonants. This is a phenomenon common for them to speak English in high which shows the influence of one sound to speed along with their emotions. As the another to become more similar. While for result, they make a ‘shortcut’ to get ease of the reduction process, it can happen to the their pronunciation.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Phonological Consonant Epenthesis Possible? a Series of Artificial Grammar Learning Experiments
    Is Phonological Consonant Epenthesis Possible? A Series of Artificial Grammar Learning Experiments Rebecca L. Morley Abstract Consonant epenthesis is typically assumed to be part of the basic repertoire of phonological gram- mars. This implies that there exists some set of linguistic data that entails epenthesis as the best analy- sis. However, a series of artificial grammar learning experiments found no evidence that learners ever selected an epenthesis analysis. Instead, phonetic and morphological biases were revealed, along with individual variation in how learners generalized and regularized their input. These results, in combi- nation with previous work, suggest that synchronic consonant epenthesis may only emerge very rarely, from a gradual accumulation of changes over time. It is argued that the theoretical status of epenthesis must be reconsidered in light of these results, and that investigation of the sufficient learning conditions, and the diachronic developments necessary to produce those conditions, are of central importance to synchronic theory generally. 1 Introduction Epenthesis is defined as insertion of a segment that has no correspondent in the relevant lexical, or un- derlying, form. There are various types of epenthesis that can be defined in terms of either the insertion environment, the features of the epenthesized segment, or both. The focus of this paper is on consonant epenthesis and, more specifically, default consonant epenthesis that results in markedness reduction (e.g. Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004)). Consonant
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • An Examination of Oral Articulation of Vowel Nasality in the Light of the Independent Effects of Nasalization on Vowel Quality
    DOI: 10.17469/O2104AISV000002 CHRISTOPHER CARIGNAN An examination of oral articulation of vowel nasality in the light of the independent effects of nasalization on vowel quality In this paper, a summary is given of an experimental technique to address a known issue in research on the independent effects of nasalization on vowel acoustics: given that the separate transfer functions associated with the oral and nasal cavities are merged in the acoustic signal, the task of teasing apart the respective effects of the two cavities seems to be an intractable problem. The results obtained from the method reveal that the independent effects of nasal- ization on the acoustic vowel space are: F1-raising for high vowels, F1-lowering for non-high vowels, and F2-lowering for non-front vowels. The results from previous articulatory research performed by the author on the production of vowel nasality in French, Hindi, and English are discussed in the light of these independent effects of nasalization on vowel quality. Keywords: vowel nasality, vowel quality, articulation, acoustics, sound change. 1. Introduction A traditional characterization of vowel nasality adopts a seemingly binary classification of vowel sounds based on the relative height of the velum: nasal vowels are produced with a low velum position (and, thus, air radiation from both the oral and nasal cavities), where- as oral vowels are produced with a high velum position (and, thus, air radiation from the oral cavity alone). While it is unquestionably true that nasal vowels are produced with a lowered velum, this traditional characterization carries an implicit assumption about the state of the oral cavity for the production of a nasal vowel, i.e., that the nasal vowel maintains the same articulatory characteristics as its non-nasal counterpart in all aspects except for the height of the velum.
    [Show full text]
  • The Morphophonemics of Vowel Compensatory Lengthening in Ekegusii
    International Journal of Education and Research Vol. 1 No. 9 September 2013 The Morphophonemics of Vowel Compensatory Lengthening in Ekegusii Samwel Komenda a, Geoffrey M. Maroko b*, Ruth W. Ndung’u c Kenyatta University, P.O Box 43844 – 00100, Nairobi, Kenya Corresponding author Email: [email protected] Tel: 0 712 660 638 ________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Abstract Literature shows that not much is known about the prosodic systems in Ekegusii, a Bantu language spoken by about 2.2 million people in south western Kenya. This paper presents an analysis of vowel compensatory lengthening in Ekegusii. Synchronic evidence for hiatus resolution strategies is provided in order to describe the vowels that are lengthened compensatorily and determine the morphological processes that trigger compensatory lengthening in Ekegusii. Guided by native speaker intuition and triangulation by other native speakers, data in the form of nominals and verbals were elicited from four Ekegusii texts and qualitatively analysed for emerging patterns. Findings revealed that all the seven basic Ekegusii vowels undergo compensatory lengthening when their phonetic environments are altered. Vowel compensatory lengthening is brought out as a surface realisation of the interaction of morphemes through the morphological process of prefixation. The lengthening is further seen as a conspiracy to eliminate ill-formed sequences created by prefixation. The height of the first vowel and whether it is followed
    [Show full text]
  • Decoding Perceptual Vowel Epenthesis: Experiments & Modelling
    Decoding perceptual vowel epenthesis : experiments & modelling Adriana Guevara Rukoz To cite this version: Adriana Guevara Rukoz. Decoding perceptual vowel epenthesis : experiments & modelling. Cognitive Sciences. Université Paris sciences et lettres, 2018. English. NNT : 2018PSLEE069. tel-03288523 HAL Id: tel-03288523 https://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-03288523 Submitted on 16 Jul 2021 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. THESE` DE DOCTORAT de l’Universite´ de recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres PSL Research University Prepar´ ee´ a` l’Ecole´ Normale Superieure´ Decodage´ de l’epenth´ ese` vocalique perceptive: Experiences´ & Modelisation´ Decoding perceptual vowel epenthesis: Experiments & Modelling Ecole´ doctorale no158 ECOLE´ DOCTORALE CERVEAU-COGNITION-COMPORTEMENT (ED3C) Specialit´ e´ SCIENCES COGNITIVES COMPOSITION DU JURY : M Christophe PALLIER INSERM-CEA Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, President´ du jury M Laurent BESACIER Universite´ Grenoble Alpes, Rapporteur Mme Sophie DUFOUR Soutenue par Aix-Marseille Universite,´ Adriana
    [Show full text]