<<

Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Science and Technology 245

Speciation - What Can be Learned from a Flycatcher Zone?

CHRIS WILEY

ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS ISSN 1651-6214 UPPSALA ISBN 91-554-6737-7 2006 urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-7358                                      !   " #$$% &'$$ (  )  (    ( *)  )+ ,)    -       )+

  . + #$$%+ /   0 .)   1   (  ! ) 2  3 4       +                    #5"+ & +    + 6/7 8 0""50%9&909+

/  ( ) :  ((    )   )   (    + ;) ( ) < -              ( )    ) ( 0   0:      - ):   + ;       ) ):      (( ((     ( ( + 6 ! ( )  0  (       ( (    - ) )   (             + ,) ) ((        )   =  - (       )     > )    !+     ( ) !+ )  ?+ 2 - 6 (   )     (     (    )     (  )    ):   +       (     )      ( ):      )      )  :      - ( )     @ ):           + ,) )    ( (-   )    ( )   ( A             ) : + ,) (         (      :                -               )         )  - ( )          +    )         ):   0           + 2:  (    (  (   <    (                    + ;) ( )      (  ) )    (  )  ( ) B    C  )     +       ( )  ( )       -   (  )   (       ( )   (  (    ) -    ) ) )   (     + 2 -        )     (  )       )        ( - (       ( )      >++        ? )    ) ):  <+ D  0          )    ( ):      )  ( )               (       -) )        (   )   + ,) ) )))           - (   ((   ):    ) -      ( )   - (0 + ! )   ) -  <      (  ( )    E   )    ( 0      -        +

       (   (   :         ):   ) :           )         ( 

             ! )   !   ( )

  !     "   "  ! # $ %& !    ! "'()*+,   ! 

F ) . #$$%

6// %" 0%# 5 6/7 8 0""50%9&909  '  ''' 09&" >) 'AA +<+A 3 G '  ''' 09&" ? List of papers

This thesis is based on the following five papers, which will be referred to in the text by their Roman numerals.

I Wiley, C., Fogelberg, N., Sæther, S. A., Veen, T., Svedin, N., Vogel Kehlenbeck, J. & Qvarnström, A. Direct benefits for hybridizing Ficedula flycatchers (submitted).

II Wiley, C., Qvarnström, A. & Gustafsson, L. Effects of hy- bridization on the immunity of flycatchers and their by blood parasites (submitted).

III Wiley, C., Andersson, G., Qvarnström, A., Borge, T. & Sætre, G.-P. Multi-generation assessments of selection against hybridization in a wild bird (Manuscript).

IV Wiley, C., Svedin, N., Weissing, F. J. & Qvarnström, A. Asymmetrical hybridization: simulations and tests in a natural (submitted).

V Wiley, C., Bengtson, J. M., Svedin, N. & Qvarnström, A. (2005) Hybridization cost of delayed maturation of secondary sexual traits in the collared flycatcher. 59, 2711- 2716.

Paper V was reproduced with permission from the publisher, The Society for the Study of Evolution.

For each of the chapters included in this thesis, I was heavily involved in the planning of experiments and the collection of field data, performed much (III) or all (II) of the molecular work, performed all of the statistical analy- ses (except the assignment of genotypes in III), and prepared all manu- scripts. In all papers, co-authors contributed intellectually to the planning of experiments and final stages of manuscript preparation, as well as with field data and laboratory work (III only).

Contents

Introduction...... 7 The Ficedula hybrid zone...... 9 How maladaptive is hybridization?...... 13 Are there direct benefits of hybridization? ...... 14 Are hybrids more or less resistant to parasites?...... 16 Summarizing selection against hybridization...... 17 Why does hybridization occur?...... 20 A lack of conspecific mates ...... 20 Errors in recognition...... 23 Conclusions and future directions ...... 25 Summary in Swedish (Sammanfattning) ...... 27 Acknowledgements ...... 32 References...... 34 Abbreviations

MHC Major histocompatibility complex PHA Phytohaemagglutinin SNP Single nucleotide polymorphism F1 First-generation hybrid Introduction

The enormous diversity of life on Earth faces us with a number of important and challenging questions. How have all these species arisen? What barriers prevent incipient species from interbreeding with each other? And why aren’t new species out-competed by the old? Many of these questions are not merely philosophical, but also dictate how we should manage this biodiver- sity in a way that preserves the variation as well its potential for self- perpetuation. However, such fundamental questions are by no means easily answered, owing largely to the timescales over which the process of specia- tion typically occurs. For these reasons, despite the subject of having attracted considerable attention from the scientific world, our under- standing of the process remains fragmentary (Coyne & Orr, 2004). Natural hybrid zones, geographical regions where closely related species meet and occasionally interbreed, provide important insights into the evolu- tionary processes characterizing the final stages of speciation. This is be- cause, by focusing on young pairs of sister-species, we can better understand the types of reproductive barriers that are important for speciation to occur. For example, we can examine the relative strengths of post- and pre-zygotic isolation between that maintain their integrity in ver- sus those that fuse. We can also investigate how post-zygotic isolation (i.e. unfit hybrids) influences the evolution of pre-zygotic isolation (species rec- ognition). Overall, hybrid zones provide us with a valuable tool for investi- gating the conditions favouring the evolution of new species, and allow us to test predictions from theoretical models of speciation in . Although interspecific hybridization is a widespread phenomenon (e.g. Johnson, 1939; Grant & Grant, 1992; Dowling & DeMarais, 1993), by defi- nition it is infrequent. Populations are defined as different species when between them is sufficiently small that each remains distinct (modified version of Mayr’s [1995] biological species , as advocated by Coyne & Orr [2004]). Unless there are severe post-zygotic barriers to (e.g. hybrid sterility), gene flow between populations through hybridization ultimately leads to the breakdown of reproductive barriers and fusion into a single species (Felsenstein, 1981). On the other hand, in the face of severe post-zygotic isolation, individuals evolve to mate only with their own spe- cies (a process termed reinforcement) (Dobzhansky, 1937; Blair, 1955), thus reducing the frequency of hybridization. However, it is this transitory nature

7 of many hybrid zones that makes them ideal for studying the conditions un- der which speciation is prevented or completed.

8 The Ficedula hybrid zone

Pied (Ficedula hypoleuca) and collared flycatchers (F. albicollis) are both well-studied model , and much is known regarding their breeding and . Because of limited space within each chapter of this thesis for summarizing this knowledge, I have attempted to outline important aspects of their biology in some detail here. Pied and collared flycatchers are small, short-lived, migratory passerine birds that breed in mixed and deciduous forests in Europe. They winter in sub-Saharan Africa, and arrive at the breeding grounds in late April or early May. Soon after arrival, males aggressively occupy and defend breeding holes (Alatalo et al. 1994; Qvarnström 1997), and females subsequently in- spect a number of males/territories before selecting their partner (Dale and Slagsvold 1996). Their ready acceptance of nest-boxes makes them ideal study objects. Females typically lay five to seven eggs, and are the sole in- cubator (when they are caught and measured). Both sexes assist in the feed- ing of nestlings (when males are caught and measured), and only a single brood is produced each year. Monogamy prevails, although at least 9% of males are polygynous (Qvarnström & Gustafsson, 2006), and approximately 15% of nestlings are sired by extra-pair males (Sheldon & Ellegren, 1999). The two species probably remained geographically isolated in separate glacial refuges during the Pleistocene, and have since expanded their breed- ing ranges northward (Sætre et al., 2001). Today, they co-occur (are sympa- tric) within a broad hybrid zone that includes much of central and eastern Europe, with an isolated hybrid zone on the Swedish islands of Gotland and Öland in the Baltic Sea. The two species have remained morphologically distinct in the face of hybridization. The work included in this thesis was carried out within the Swedish hybrid zone, which constitutes a more recent contact between the two species (around 100 years) (Lundberg and Alatalo, 1992). On the Swedish islands (as in much of Europe), collared flycatchers have gradually expanded their range, excluding pied flycatchers from opti- mal breeding (Alatalo et al., 1982, 1994; Sætre et al., 1999; see Fig 1). The Swedish hybrid zone is surrounded on all sides by allopatric pied flycatchers, which is a likely source of migrants into the hybrid zone. Immi- gration into the Swedish collared , when it occurs, most likely involves individuals arising from other hybrid zones in eastern Europe.

9 Figure 1. (A) Breeding distribution of pied (red), collared (yellow) flycatchers. Hy- brid zone is shown in orange (adapted from Haavie et al., 2004). (B) Typical mixed forest inhabited by pied flycatchers. (C) Typical, high quality, deciduous forest dominated by collared flycatchers (both B and C taken in northern Öland). (D) Lo- cation of nest-box areas monitored within the Swedish flycatcher hybrid zone.

Figure 2. (A) Adult male collared flycatcher, (B) subadult male collared flycatcher, (C) male hybrid, and (D) male pied flycatcher. Photos kindly provided by N. Svedin and M. Hjernquist.

The males of both species have bold, black-and-white plumage patterns, which are involved in intraspecific (both male-male compe-

10 tition and female choice) (Pärt & Qvarnström, 1997; Qvarnstöm, 1999; Qvarnström et al., 2000). However, they delay maturation of these traits until they are two years old. Subadults (one year old) are sexually mature, but are less boldly marked than older birds, either to avoid conflicts with dominant, older males (Qvarnström, 1997), or through developmental constraints. The two species differ in the size of their white patches and the blackness of their pigmented plumage, which not only permits their identification by humans, but may also be important for females to recognize conspecific mates (Sætre et al., 1997a). Females of the two species are both dull grey-brown, and dif- fer only in subtle differences in the tone of their upperparts and the amount of white at the base of their neck feathers (Svensson 1992). Previous mate- choice trials have demonstrated that females display clear preferences for males of their own species (Sætre et al., 1997b). Males, on the other hand, appear to be less selective towards whom they display (Dale and Slagsvold 1994; Sætre et al., 1997b). Plumage, however, is unlikely to be the only cue used for mate-recognition, as the two species also have distinct songs. Song is thought to be an important sexual signal, isolating many species that oth- erwise do not differ in secondary sexual traits (Martens, 1996). However, the use of song by female flycatchers for locating conspecific mates is compli- cated by the fact that in areas of sympatry many male pied flycatchers learn to sing like collared flycatchers (Gelter, 1987; Alatalo et al., 1990; Haavie et al., 2004; Qvarnström et al., 2006). Whether this ‘mixed singing’ has evolved in males to signal territory occupancy to individuals of both species, or whether it is a maladaptive behaviour persisting from allopatry is cur- rently unclear. However, mixed singing is associated with high rates of hy- bridization (Qvarnström et al. 2006). The above-mentioned pre-mating barriers between the two species are quite strong. Heterospecific pairs typically constitute only 5.1% of the study population on Öland, and 2.4% on Gotland. When hybridization does occur, it results in the production of largely unfit F1 offspring. Females are sterile, and F1 males also have reduced fitness (Alatalo et al. 1990; Gelter et al. 1992; Veen et al., 2001). Postzygotic barriers acting on female hybrids are mostly intrinsic, although males appear to suffer mostly through their inabil- ity to compete for mates (sperm inviability is another likely possibility) (Svedin, 2006). F1 hybrids are intermediate in morphology between the two parental species (Sætre et al., 2003). They frequently resemble collared fly- catchers, although their white collar is incomplete, they have more grey in the plumage, and they possess little white in their primaries. However, past studies into the fitness of hybrids have been fraught with problems arising from the lack of genetic tools to clearly separate the different hybrid genera- tions (i.e. F1 and backcrosses). Furthermore, some aberrant collared fly- catchers (especially subadults) closely resemble hybrids, and genetic tools are required to distinguish true hybrids. Prior to this thesis, nothing was

11 known about the fitness of later generation hybrids, a situation common to almost all natural hybrid zones. Despite the low fitness of hybrids, one previous study suggested that het- erospecific pairing is sometimes adaptive (Veen et al., 2001). Female col- lared flycatchers can reduce the indirect costs of heterospecific pairing by copulating with extra-pair conspecific males and biasing the sex ratio of their offspring towards sons (Veen et al., 2001). Furthermore, the fact that such heterospecific pairs produce more fledged young (perhaps a result of direct benefits of heterospecific pairing) during the food-limited later half of the breeding season suggests that heterospecific pairing may be adaptive for late-arriving female collared flycatchers (Veen et al., 2001).

12 How maladaptive is hybridization?

Estimating the fitness of hybridizing individuals relative to those paired with conspecific mates not only provides a measure of the strength of selection against hybridization, but also of the reproductive isolation between sympa- tric populations. It is therefore important for predicting the amount of gene flow between two taxa, and is vital for inferring the likelihood that complete reproductive isolation will ultimately arise through reinforcement. When the F1 hybrids produced are sterile, hybridizing taxa are fully reproductively isolated. Because gene flow is prevented, they can be considered separate species. Furthermore, there is expected to be strong selection on individuals to avoid interspecific mating (Dobzhansky, 1937), such that eventually the two species are expected to rarely, if ever, interbreed. However, when the resultant hybrids are not fully sterile or inviable, a small amount of gene flow results. Gene flow can effectively disrupt the process of reinforcement by preventing linkage disequilibrium between genes causing prezygotic iso- lation and those causing postzygotic isolation, leading to an amalgamation of the two genomes (Barton & Hewitt, 1981; Felsenstein, 1981). Whether low levels of gene flow prevent reinforcement has been the topic of much theo- retical discussion, and depends greatly on the details of the models employed to test this (Liou & Price, 1994; Kelly & Noor, 1996; Servedio & Kirkpa- trick, 1997; Kirkpatrick & Servedio, 1999; Servedio, 2000). This is perhaps a question that is best answered by empiricists. Surveying the strengths of selection against hybrids in hybrid zones where species-boundaries easily break down versus those in which the two taxa remain distinct and display would provide the best tool for investigating the con- ditions favouring speciation via reinforcement. Unfortunately, there are very few systems for which we currently have good estimates for the strength of selection against hybridization (Arnold & Hodges, 1995; Rieseberg & Car- ney, 1998). The main reason for this lack of knowledge stems not from a lack of work into hybrid zones, but from the difficulty of obtaining accurate estimates of fitness. Fitness is defined as an individual’s contribution to the gene pool of future generations. In practice, however, it is typically estimated through a range of indirect measures, without knowing how they reflect the true cur- rency of fitness; the number of gene copies (descendents) in later generations (Benton & Grant, 2000). Such measures often include seed germination,

13 larval survival, body mass, growth rates, immunocompetence, success at attracting mates, or fertility. Situations where some fitness components are high and others are low in the same hybrids highlight the need for a holistic approach to estimating selection against hybrids. Ficedula flycatchers highlight the problem with inferring selection from single fitness components. Initial investigations into the system revealed low fertility among hybrids (Gelter et al. 1992), which resulted in the conclusion that hybridization was maladaptive. The fertility of hybrids was 45.9% of that of pure individuals. However, a later study revealed that one combina- tion of heterospecific pair (collared females  pied males) produced more chicks than conspecific pairs during certain times in the season (Veen et al., 2001), even though many of these were unfit hybrids. This suggested that there were either direct benefits to female collared flycatchers of hybridiz- ing, or hybrids possessed enhanced vigour, which was only expressed in stressful environments. Either explanation represents a fitness component that acts to counter low fertility, at least to some extent. This raised some doubt over previous suggestions that hybridization was always maladaptive in flycatchers.

Are there direct benefits of hybridization? In Chapter I, I explore the origin of the pattern that late-breeding het- erospecific pairs often produce more offspring than conspecific pairs. In particular, I investigate whether females gain direct benefits from pairing with males of the other species. These direct benefits might include when heterospecific males possess superior territories, or utilize their territory in a way that benefits the nestlings (e.g. by providing more food or complemen- tary food-types). I found that the two species fed similar items to nestlings, and that the proportion of the parental workload adopted by males was not different between the two species. Therefore, there were no indications that heterospecific males utilize their territories in ways that benefit the nestlings more than conspecific fathers. However, the territories occupied by het- erospecific pairs did differ in quality from those occupied by conspecific pairs. The effects of territory quality and having heterospecific parents were disentangled by looking at the performance of nests in the same box as het- erospecific pairs, but in other years. I found that late-breeding female col- lared flycatchers that pair with pied flycatchers have better territories than those that do not hybridize (see Figure 3). This pattern closely matched pre- viously reported fledgling rates in nests reared by such heterospecific pairs (Veen et al. 2001). Direct benefits of mate-choice are widely appreciated as an important component of intraspecific sexual selection (Thornhill, 1976; Searcy, 1979; Gwynne, 1984; Reynolds & Gross, 1990; Kirkpatrick & Ryan, 1991). De-

14 spite this, few studies have considered direct benefits to females of hybridi- zation. Most, instead, focus on the indirect costs/benefits of producing hy- brid offspring. This trend no doubt reflects the, often severe, nature of indi- rect costs of hybridization (e.g. sterility of hybrid offspring). Also, direct benefits can explain the evolution of intraspecific mate choice, while hy- bridization is normally viewed as maladaptive, and therefore unlikely to evolve in response to the existence of direct benefits. My findings do not argue against such logic. What they do suggest is that, despite the existence of indirect costs, there may be direct benefits to females of hybridization. Such benefits may be important in reducing selection against hybridization and thereby hindering the evolution of reproductive isolation. Furthermore, among hybrid zones there exists a continuum of postzygotic isolation, from complete sterility to hybrid vigour. In those hybrid zones where postzygotic isolation is weak, direct benefits of hybridization may dictate selection for /against hybridization.

90 47 80 23 63

70 59 64 16 60 3

50 2

Territory quality (total mass of chicks) 40

-10 -5 0 5 10 15 30 -5 0 5 10 Laying date (0 = yearly mean)

Figure 3. Comparison of data on fledging success of heterospecific pairs with male pied flycatchers (left panel, curve 1: from Veen et al., 2001), and the quality of their territories (right panel, triangles: from Chapter I) with those of collared flycatchers (negative slope in left panel; squares in right panel). Heterospecific pairs tend to have better territories and produce more fledglings than pairs of collared flycatcher after two days before the mean laying date of each year.

In Chapter I, I also discuss another important aspect of speciation; namely, for the coexistence of two sister taxa, they must not only be repro- ductively isolated, but should diverge sufficiently in niche to prevent com- petitive exclusion (Gause, 1934; Lack, 1946; Macarthur and Levins, 1964, 1967). Collared and pied flycatchers prefer the same breeding (Ala- talo et al., 1994), compete over the same nest-holes, and also have highly overlapping diets (Chapter I). Previous studies have indicated that pied fly- catchers have a broader tolerance of environmental stress (Sætre et al., 1999; Qvarnström et al., 2005), which allows them to persist in habitats of lowest

15 quality, while being excluded from those of the highest quality (Alatalo et al., 1994). Nevertheless, the overlap in their realized niches (including spa- tial segregation resulting from competition) is extensive, and long-term per- sistence of the two species on the Baltic islands is unlikely without further divergence in . Undoubtedly, the persistence of pied flycatchers on the two Swedish study islands is made possible through extensive immigra- tion from surrounding allopatric source populations.

Are hybrids more or less resistant to parasites? Another component of fitness potentially enhanced by hybridization is resis- tance against parasites. A high level of heterozygosity across the genome, and in particular among parts coding for the major histocompatibility com- plex (MHC) may promote overall immunity (Hughes & Hughes, 1995; Wegner et al. 2003; Westerdahl et al., 2005). Furthermore, highly - specific parasites may be unable to colonise and infect the novel host envi- ronment offered by hybrids (Whitham et al., 1994). In Chapter II, I investi- gate whether hybrid flycatchers are more or less susceptible to infection by blood parasites (Haemoproteus and ) and whether this suscepti- bility is correlated with their immune-response to a novel antigen, phyto- haemagglutinin (PHA). F1 hybrids had parasite infection rates that were in- termediate between, but not significantly different from, collared and pied flycatchers (see Figure 4). Furthermore, their immune response to PHA was similar to that of the parents. Later generation hybrids, however, had signifi- cantly stronger immune responses than the parental species (Figure 4). They also had the lowest malarial infection rate, although this was not signifi- cantly lower than the parental species. More samples are required before it is known whether this pattern is robust.

16 Figure 4. Rates of infection by avian malarial parasites (left panel) and immune response to PHA (right panel) of collared and pied flycatchers and their first- (F1) and later-generation (RECOM) hybrids (see Chapter II).

In general, this study failed to support the idea that parasite resistance is enhanced in F1 hybrids. This finding agrees with a number of recent studies indicating that parasite resistance of animal hybrids does not tend to be higher than that of the parental species (Moulia, 1999; Derothe et al., 2001; Parris, 2004; Wolinska et al., 2004). This is a comparable pattern to that observed in plants (Moulia, 1999; Fritz et al., 1999), despite vastly different immune systems.

Summarizing selection against hybridization In flycatchers, individual components of fitness are differently affected by hybridization. Fertility is reduced in F1 hybrids, but their immunity against malarial parasites is not impaired (Chapter II). Furthermore, growing up in a territory possessed by heterospecific parents is sometimes associated with direct benefits (Chapter I). Selection on flycatchers to avoid hybridization is therefore unclear, and may be lower than previously thought, based on fertil- ity alone. In Chapter III, I attempt to resolve this confusion by producing a single estimate of the selection against hybridization; a composite of all in- dividual fitness components. As mentioned earlier, estimating the fitness of hybridizing individuals is a complex problem. Not only is it difficult to find proxies of fitness that accurately reflect genetic representation in later gen- erations, but it is unclear how many generations in the future we should con- sider. Because of the frequent indirect costs/benefits of hybridization (i.e. hybrid offspring with especially unfit or vigorous hybrid offspring), esti-

17 mates of a hybridizing individual’s fitness as their genetic contribution to the next generation are unsuitable, as this may not represent their contribution to ‘future’ generations. This is widely recognized, and is why selection against hybridization is typically inferred from the reproductive output of F1 hybrids, rather than the reproductive output of hybridizing individuals themselves (see recent examples by Parris et al. 1999; Bierne et al., 2002; Pertl et al., 2002; Ramsey et al., 2003; Bleeker & Matthies, 2005; Kirk et al. 2005; Pe- terson et al., 2005). However, the fitness consequences of hybridization (hy- brid vigour or outbreeding depression) may be expressed over several gen- erations (Rieseberg & Carney, 1998; Barton, 2001; Burke & Arnold, 2001). As a result, the indirect costs or benefits of hybridization may be underesti- mated when only F1 offspring are considered. Due to the difficulty of esti- mating fitness in nature, as well as correctly identifying late-generation hy- brids, very little is currently known about for how many generations of hy- brid offspring the indirect costs of hybridization typically persist. Yet, this information is important if we are to understand how well single- and two- generation estimates of fitness (i.e. based on the reproductive success of hybridizing pairs and their F1 offspring) represent overall selection against hybridization. Until recently, the lack of genetic tools for accurately identifying late- generation hybrid flycatchers has prohibited the estimation of selection against hybridization using multiple generations of hybrid descendents. However, recently identified SNPs (Borge et al., 2005) now allow us to iden- tify F1 hybrids, first-generation backcrosses, and second-generation back- crosses. In Chapter III, I use the number of descendents three generations after hybridization to estimate selection against hybridization. Two inde- pendent methods for estimating this (comparing frequencies of the hybrid descendents and combining reproductive data from all hybrid generations) revealed similar results. Hybridizing individuals produce approximately 2% of the number of descendents arising from non-hybridizing individuals. Fur- thermore, this severe postzygotic isolation between collared and pied fly- catchers is likely to be a conservative estimate. This is because I also ob- served a high rate of ‘re-hybridization’ (mating with the species that consti- tutes the minority of the individual’s genome) among second-generation backcrosses. This suggests that the depressed fitness of hybrids persists be- yond first-generation backcrosses. Strong postzygotic isolation between collared and pied flycatchers is not surprising considering they have remained distinct in the face of extensive hybridization. Still, this was only detected once multiple generations of hy- brid descendents were considered. Chapter III highlights the importance of taking a holistic approach when estimating fitness. This study suggests a crucial role for post-zygotic isolation as a reproductive barrier between newly evolved bird species. This opposes previous suggestions that the evo- lution of behavioural barriers to interbreeding typically precedes the evolu-

18 tion of postmating isolation in birds (Grant & Grant, 1997). Continued de- velopments in the genetic tools available for identifying later-generation hybrids (backcrosses and ‘re-hybrids’) will greatly assist in quantifying postzygotic isolation in other study-systems, allowing more educated con- clusions to be drawn regarding the evolution of reproductive barriers.

19 Why does hybridization occur?

Given that selection against hybridization between flycatcher species is so strong, it is worth considering why complete pre-mating isolation has not evolved. Perhaps the two taxa have simply not spent a sufficient amount of time in sympatry for this to occur. Alternatively, other factors may have prevented the evolution of complete pre-mating isolation though reinforce- ment. Whether reinforcement is common has been a topic of much debate (Bigelow, 1965; Spencer et al., 1986; Butlin, 1987, 1989; Noor, 1999; Coyne & Orr, 2004). Coyne and Orr (2004) highlight the main objections, and I summarise these below. First, gene flow causes recombination among genes coding for and genes coding for low hybrid fitness. This often leads to a breakdown in isolation barriers. Second, if one of the two taxa is rare, they suffer higher rates of hybridization. When this is costly, as is required for reinforcement, the rare species is likely to go extinct before pre-mating isolation has evolved. Third, gene flow from outside the hybrid zone prevents local to sympatry (including the evolution of spe- cies recognition). Fourth, as reinforcement progresses, hybridization be- comes more infrequent, and this relaxed selection slows further evolution of traits influencing hybridization. To gain insight into which of these might be important in flycatcher hy- brid zones, it is important to examine which factors currently affect hybridi- zation rates. Given that hybridization is costly, there are two main reasons why an individual might still hybridize. First, it may be poorly able to distin- guish conspecific mates from heterospecific mates (i.e. there are errors in species recognition). Such a scenario arises if reinforcement has not yet oc- curred. Alternatively, there may be no errors in species recognition, but con- specific mates are simply not available, forcing hybridization. Thus, hybridi- zation may result even if reinforcement has occurred. Chapters IV and V explore these possibilities in flycatchers.

A lack of conspecific mates Even if both species display perfect preferences for their own kind, hybridi- zation may still occur when conspecific mates are lacking. If this is the pri-

20 mary reason for hybridization, we can make a number of predictions. First, the risk of hybridization experienced by individual females increases as their species constitutes a smaller proportion of the population. Second, maximum hybridization rates occur when one species is rare (Randler, 2002). Third, because females are thought to be the choosy sex, heterospecific pairs should mostly contain a female of the rare species (Wirtz, 1999). In Chapter IV, I test each of these predictions in flycatchers. Because the study islands con- sist of a large number of separate forests with differing ratios of the two spe- cies (see Figure 1) and females generally sample potential partners within the one woodlot (Dale & Slagsvold, 1996), I was able to determine how hybridization patterns varied with the relative abundance of the two species. In accordance with expected effects of lacking conspecific mates, female pied flycatchers had much higher probabilities of hybridizing in sites where conspecifics were rare. A lack of data on female collared flycatchers in pied- dominated sites prevented a similar comparison being done for them, but the available data was consistent with such a pattern (see Figure 5).

Figure 5. The effect of relative abundance on the hybridization risk of individual females of either species (see Chapter IV). Each pair of points (collared and pied) represents one or several forest sites with similar species composition.

However, the overall hybridization rate was highest in sites where the two species were at equal frequencies (Figure 6a). This pattern resulted from the fact that, while individual females had lower hybridization risks when they became more common, there were more of them. Simulations confirmed that

21 such a pattern is consistent with hybrid zones in which there are moderate levels of error in species recognition. This implies that a lack of conspecific mates, while important, is not the sole explanation of hybridization in fly- catchers. This conclusion was further supported by asymmetries in hybridization towards heterospecific pairs involving females of the rare species. Flycatcher hybrid zones in fact showed the opposite pattern; that heterospecific pairs more often contained female pied flycatchers as pied flycatchers were in- creasingly common (figure 6b). Such a pattern implies that rarity can influ- ence hybridization in ways other than through limiting conspecific mates. Rarity can affect the error rate of choosy females, either through increasing rates of mixed singing (false male signals) or as a plastic response of females to their perceived hybridization risk.

Figure 6. The relationships between relative abundance of pied flycatchers within a forest site and (a) the overall hybridization rate within a site, and (b) the bias in the direction of hybridization towards heterospecific pairs involving female pied fly- catchers (see Chapter IV for details).

Overall, much of the hybridization occurring in flycatchers results from a lack of conspecific mates. However, even when the choice between a het- erospecific and conspecific is available, errors do occur with notable fre- quency.

22 Errors in species recognition Species recognition depends upon the existence of traits that characterize each species, and preferences of individuals for those traits that signal con- specifics. Errors in mate choice (i.e. hybridization) can therefore result if the traits are not sufficiently divergent, and/or if the preferences for the trait are weak. Chapters IV and V examine components of the species recognition process in flycatchers. In flycatchers, there is a slight but significant bias towards hybridization mostly involving female pied flycatchers and male collared flycatcher (Chapter IV). From figure 6b, I conclude that this bias probably doesn’t re- sult from pied flycatchers being the generally rarer species across the field sites. I therefore tested whether differences between the females of the two species in their discriminatory ability are a likely reason for this asymmetri- cal hybridization. In Chapter IV, I present results from a song-broadcast experiment, which indicated that collared flycatchers respond more strongly to conspecific song than pied song. Pied flycatchers, on the other hand, do not display preferences for conspecific song. These differences in discrimi- natory ability are a likely cause of asymmetrical hybridization. However, the selection on such preferences to be refined within the hybrid zone is depend- ent on the reliability of the information contained within the song-signal. A large proportion of male pied flycatchers within the hybrid zone copy the songs of male collared flycatchers (Gelter, 1987; Alatalo et al., 1990; Haavie et al., 2004), increasing their probability of being chosen by female collared flycatchers (Qvarnström et al., 2006), and reducing the selection on female pied flycatchers to narrow their preference. Pied flycatchers within the Swedish hybrid zone have probably lived alongside collared flycatchers for a shorter time than Swedish collared fly- catchers have lived alongside pieds. Furthermore, there is probably substan- tial immigration into the sympatric population of pied flycatchers from allo- patric areas (Alatalo et al., 1982). Both factors make it likely that if one of the two species were less adapted to sympatry, it is expected to be the pied flycatcher. Indeed, they show weaker preferences for their own kind, and more often falsely signal their specific identity through song. Comparisons with allopatric populations of collared flycatchers are required to confirm whether their apparent adaptation to sympatry is a result of reinforcement. Migration from allopatric populations is typically implicated as a factor in- hibiting reinforcement, and this may be true for pied flycatchers. However, it can also enhance the process in collared flycatchers, by prolonging the time before the local extinction of the competitively least fit . However, some taxa that have spent a long time in sympatry and experi- ence very little gene flow from allopatry may continue to possess traits that increase their hybridization rate, especially if lacking those traits is associ- ated with costs. In Chapter V, I show that delaying the maturation of secon-

23 dary sexual characters until two years of age in collared flycatchers is asso- ciated with an increased risk of hybridization among one-year-olds (su- badults). This arises because subadults approach pied flycatchers in mor- phology (see figures 2 and 7), and they are therefore chosen by female pied flycatchers faced with a lack of conspecific mates. Despite this cost of de- layed maturation, it persists within the hybrid zone. The maturation of sexual ornaments in flycatchers may be constrained developmentally (as appears to be the case in shorebirds: Chu, 1994), such that it cannot be avoided without substantial re-organization of the developmental process. Alternatively, the benefits associated with reduced aggressive confrontation by older, dominant males (as indicated by Qvarnström, 1997) may override the risk of hybridi- zation in a hybrid zone where heterospecific females are scarce. In either case, the trait increasing hybridization (delayed maturation) is maintained because the costs of lacking it are too great. This highlights an important criticism of speciation via reinforcement; namely, once hybridization be- comes rare, further selection on traits that prevent it is weak, and such traits are unlikely to evolve if there are costs or constraints associated with them.

Figure 7. Principal components analysis of four plumage traits, showing phenotypic overlap between male pied flycatchers (triangles), subadult male collared flycatchers (squares) and adult male collared flycatchers (crosses) (see Chapter V).

24 Conclusions and future directions

This study provides one of the first comprehensive estimates of the strength of selection against natural hybridization. It also highlights that different components of fitness can be differently affected by hybridization, such that it is rarely possible to know aprioriif any particular component acts as an appropriate proxy for overall selection. By estimating fitness in a context that is biologically relevant (i.e. in nature), studies such as presented in this thesis provide a basis for inferring the range of conditions under which populations fuse or remain separate in the face of hybridization. This knowledge is fundamental for understanding and managing the diversity of life on Earth. With continuing advances in the genetic tools available for identifying later-generation hybrids and monitoring gene flow, estimating the strength of selection against hybridization may soon be an achievable goal in many other hybrid zones. I found that post-mating barriers play a crucial role in maintaining species integrity in flycatchers, while pre-mating barriers are incomplete. This con- trasts with previous suggestions that pre-mating barriers typically evolve fastest in birds (Grant & Grant, 1997). Clearly, the relative importance for speciation of pre- versus post-mating barriers to gene flow remains unclear. Investigations into the number of genes typically involved in such barriers may give insight into their likely rate of evolution, and therefore importance. Strong selection against hybridization need not always result in reinforced pre-mating isolation. I identify a number of traits (e.g. song discrimination and copying, delayed maturation of sexual ornaments) influencing hybridiza- tion, implying that these traits are under different selection within and out- side the hybrid zone. This thesis therefore provides testable predictions about the expected directions of change in the values of these traits between allo- patric and sympatric populations of each species, as predicted from rein- forcement. Such comparative studies will provide an indication of the role that reinforcement has had in causing the pre-mating isolation currently ob- served within the flycatcher hybrid zone. Finally, this thesis highlights a generally neglected reason for hybridiza- tion; a lack of conspecific mates. Even when individuals express perfect preferences for their own kind, hybridization may occur when no conspecific mates are available to choose from. Whether this is a widespread phenome- non is not known, but some studies suggest that it is (Randler, 2002). In hy- brid zones where it is more beneficial to hybridize than to not breed at all,

25 hybridization may occur as an adaptive “back-up” strategy. This may have important repercussions for reinforcement, and yet this is rarely considered. Overall, the Ficedula hybrid zone provides an ideal opportunity for study- ing a range of questions pertaining to speciation, and this thesis provides an important early step towards answering some of these.

26 Summary in Swedish (Sammanfattning)

På vår planet finns miljontals arter, men hur alla dessa arter utvecklats och hur de håller sig separerade har vi liten kunskap om. Enligt det biologiska artbegreppet utgör varje art en population av individer som är reproduktivt isolerade från andra sådana populationer. Bildandet av nya arter kan därför ses som utvecklandet av reproduktiva barriärer mellan populationer. Sådana barriärer brukar delas in i två huvudgrupper. Den ena gruppen, prezygotiska barriärer (t.ex. partnerpreferenser för artspecifika karaktärer), hindrar parn- ingar över artgränserna. Den andra gruppen, postzygotiska barriärer, orsakas av nedsatt livsduglighet eller fortplantingsförmåga hos hybrider (avkomman från korsningar mellan olika arter eller populationer). Den relativa betydel- sen av dessa barriärer, var för sig eller i samverkan, har diskuterats mycket i den vetenskapliga litteraturen om artbildningsprocesser. Ett exempel på en sådan samverkan är att nedsatt fortplantingsförmåga hos hybrider kan leda till att individer som föredrar att para sig inom den egna arten gynnas. Trots en livlig teoretisk diskussion om hur och när olika typer av reproduktiva barriärer utvecklas och samverkar vet vi mycket lite om hur det verkligen går till i naturen. Svårigheterna med att studera artbilningsprocesser kommer från den tid dessa processer kräver. Fullständig reproduktiv isolering (d.v.s. att hybrider inte är livsdugliga) tar igenomsnitt ca 4 miljoner år hos däggdjur och ännu längre tid hos fåglar. Hur ska man över huvudtaget kunna studera en så långsam och utdragen process? Hybridzoner (plaster där två nyligen bildade arter fortfarande delvis kor- sar sig med varandra) utgör en perfekt situation för att kunna studera de fak- torer och processer som är inblandade i uppkomsten av reproduktiv isoler- ing. Den svenska hybridzonen mellan svartvit och halsbandsflugsnappare utgör ett sådant exempel. Svartvit flugsnappare häckar i hela norden medans halsbandsflugnappare bara häckar på Öland och Gotland. På dessa öar före- kommer korsningar mellan de båda flugsnappararterna som resulterar i hon- liga hybrider som är sterila och hanliga hybrider som kan fortplanta sig. Trots att det uppenbarligen förekommer genflöde mellan de båda arterna är de tydligt morfologiskt och genetiskt skilda från varandra. Mitt avhandling- sarbete undersöker vilka faktorer som är avgörande för att de två flugsnap- pararterna ska kunna behålla sina identiteter och således vilka faktorer som är viktiga i artbilningsprocessen genom att studera naturlig hybridisering. Jag har fokuserat på två huvudfrågeställningar:

27 (1) Hur stark är den evolutionära urvalsprocessen (selektionen) emot hybrid- isering och hur kan man bäst uppskatta och mäta den? (2) Varför förkommer hybridisering när det leder till avkommor med nedsatt fortplantningsförmåga?

ÄR HYBRIDISERING KOSTSAMT? För individer är det oftast kostsamt, ur ett evolutionärt perspektiv, att para sig över artgränsen. Hybrider är ofta sterila eller har nedsatt fertilitet, vilket är fallet hos flugsnappare. Emellertid kan det även finnas vissa fördelar med att välja en partner som tillhör en annan art och alla livsprocesser behöver inte fungera sämre hos hybrider. Jag undersöker därför två fall där hybridis- ering kan vara gynnsamt i de två första kapitlen i min avhandling. I kapitel I visar jag att hybridisering kan vara gynnsamt för honor om ha- nar som tillhör en närbesläktad art har tillgång till bättre resurser till avkomman än hanar av den egna arten. Hanar av den andra arten kanske bidrar mer till vården av avkomman eller så har de tillgång till bättre häckn- ingsrevir. Sådana direkta fördelar kopplat till val av partner anses vara vik- tiga när en hona ska välja mellan hanar av den egna arten men de diskuteras sällan i samband med hybridisering. Jag fann bevis för att halsbandsflugsnapparhonor faktiskt tjänar på att välja en svartvit flugsnap- parhane i slutet av häckningssäsongen, då dessa hanar har tillgång till bättre revir. Därmed kan jag förklara den underliggande orsaken till att sena halsbandsflugsnapparhonor producerar fler ungar när de häckar med svart- vita flugsnapparhanar. Alltså kan direkta fördelar minska selektionstrycket mot hybridisering och därmed till en viss del underlätta genflöde mellan arter. En annan tänkbar fördel kopplat till hybridisering är immunitet mot para- siter. Hybrider har mer variation i sina gener jämfört med föräldrararterna, vilket inkluderar gener som styr immunförsvaret. Ökad variation bland dessa gener kan leda till bättre immunförsvar genom motståndskraft mot fler typer av parasiter. Dessutom skulle hybrider kunna undslippa många parasiter genom att helt enkelt utgöra en miljö som den vissa parasiter inte är anpas- sad till. Många parasiter är nämligen ofta starkt anpassade till specifika vär- dorganismer. I kapitel II undersöker jag om hybridflugsnappare har relativt färre blodparasiter och ett starkare immunförsvar. Jag fann att hybriders immunförsvar var intermediärt och inte skiljde sig från någon av föräldraar- terna. Infektionsfrekvensen av blodparasiter skiljde sig däremot mellan de båda föräldrararterna och halsbandsflugsnappare hade färre parasiter. Detta kan, i sin tur, påverka hur pass konkurrenskraftiga de båda arterna är och därmed påverka deras samexistens i hybridzonen. En viktig slutsats som man kan dra från flugsnapparna är att olika proc- esser under deras liv kan påverkas olika av hybridisering. Hybridernas fer- tilitet är nedsatt men inte deras förmåga att motstå parasiter. Dessutom kan

28 hybridisering ibland medföra direkta fördelar. En viktig fråga blir då; hur ska man mäta selektionen mot hybridisering? Hur pass evolutionärt fram- gångsrik en individ är mäts i dess relativa genetiska bidrag till kommande generationer, vilket i praktiken är lättare sagt än gjort. Istället brukar forskare mäta olika komponenter som tillsammans påverkar en individs reproduktiva framgång så som direkta fördelar eller immunförsvar. När det gäller hybrider kan en generations reproduktiva framgång vara mycket missvisande efter- som även hybridernas avkomma också kan påverkas. I kapitel III uppskattar jag den andel avkomlingar tre generationer framåt i tiden som hybridisering har bidragit med till populationen. Den typen av uppskattningar kan sällan göras ute i naturen eftersom hybrider och speciellt återkorsningar (avkom- man från hybrider eller återkorsningar som parar sig med föräldrararterna) är sällsynta och svåra att identifiera. Därför ger min studie viktig ny informa- tion om långsiktiga effekter av hybridisering i naturliga populationer. Jag visar att inte bara hybrider har nedsatt fertilitet utan också senare generation- ers återkorsningar. Återkorsningar hade också nedsatt fertilitet på grund av en hög andel av dem parade sig med individer som tillhör den art som de delar lägst andel gener med. Min slutliga uppskattning, som alltså tar hänsyn till flera generationer, visar att det är en stark selektion mot hybridisering. Flugsnappare som parar sig över artgränsen bidrar bara med 2% av antalet avkomlingar som icke-hybriserande flugsnappare bidrar med till popula- tionen. Det är alltså mycket mer kostsamt med hybridisering än vad tidigare studier visat.

VARFÖR SKER HYBRIDISERING? Eftersom det är en stark selektion mot hybridisering kan man fråga sig varför de båda arterna inte utvecklat en bättre förmåga att undvika parningar över artgränsen. En tänkbar förklaring är att de helt enkelt inte har tillbringat till- räcklig lång tid tillsammans. Alternativt finns det andra faktorer som leder till att individer ibland tvingas eller bara råkar hybridisera. Även om båda arterna har utvecklat en perfekt preferens för sin egen art så kan individer hybridisera när det finns ont om partners som tillhör den egna arten. Valet kanske står mellan att hybridisera eller inte fortplanta sig alls. I Kapitel IV, undersöker jag hur fördelningen mellan halsbands- och svartvit flugnsappare påverkar hybridisering. Som förväntat fann jag att svartvitflugsnapparhonor löpte störst risk att hybridisera när den egna arten var ovanlig. Däremot var den totala hybridiseringsfrekvensen högst i om- råden där de båda arterna förekom i lika stora antal. En tänkbar förklaring är att trots att individuella honor löper störst risk att hybridisera när den egna arten är ovanlig finns det fler individer som kan göra fel när arterna är mer jämt fördelade. Alltså är relativ vanlighet en viktig faktor när det gäller att förklara förekomst av hybridisering mellan de båda flugsnappararterna men det är inte den enda faktorn som är viktig.

29 I kapitel IV och V identifierar jag flera karaktärer som påverkar felak- tigheter i partnervalet (som orsakar hybridisering) hos flugsnapparna. Hanar från de båda arterna sjunger olika sånger som skiljer sig tydligt när de finns på olika geografiska platser. När de förekommer tillsammans händer det att svartvit flugsnapparhanar kopierar element från halsbandsflugsnapparnas sång och detta leder i sin tur till ökad hybridiseringsrisk. I kapitel IV visar jag att svartvit flugsnappare är sämre på att särskilja mellan de två arttypiska sångerna och att detta är en trolig förklaring till att de har en högre benägen- het att välja fel. I blandade par är honorna oftast svartvit flugsnappare. Denna asymmetri är mindre i områden där svartvit flugsnappare är ovanli- gare vilket betyder att individuella halsbandsflugnapparhonor ökar sin benägenhet att göra fel i sådana områden. Varför har de en sämre förmåga att särskilja den egna artens hanar när det finns gott om dem? Antingen blir honorna helt enkelt mer slarviga när risken att träffa en hane som tillhör fel art är liten. Alternativt blir det av någon anledning svårare för dem att sär- skilja mellan hanarna. En tänkbar anledning till att det blir svårare att sär- skilja hanarna är att svartvit flugsnapparhanar oftare lär sig element från halsbandsflugsnapparnas sång i områden där halsbandsflugsnappare utgör den vanligaste arten. Både benägenheten att lära sig sångelement från den andra arten och den sämre förmåga att särskilja de två arttypiska sångerna åt avspeglar antagligen den svartvit flugsnapparens sämre anpassning till hy- bridiseringsrisken. Att utvecklingen av sådana anpassningar inte har kommit längre kan förklaras av den korta tid som arterna har samexisterat på Öland och Gotland och av den stora immigrationen av svartvit flugsnappare från fastlandet. Halsbandflugnapparna på Öland och Gotland härstammar däremot från utbredningsområden där båda arterna har förekommit tillsammans under en lång tid och dessutom tycks de flesta av individerna vara födda i ö- populationen. Halsbandsflugsnapparna förväntas därför vara relativt bättre anpassade till att undvika hybridisering. Trots detta har den här arten också bibehålligt karaktärer som är associerade med hybridiseringsrisk. I kapitel V visar jag att den fjäderdräkt som ett åriga hanar har medför en ökad hybridis- eringsrisk. Detta beror på att ettåriga halsbandflugsnapparhanar inte ännu har en adult (vuxen) fjäderdräkt och därför utseendemässigt närmar sig svartvit flugsnapparhanars utseende. Unga halsbandsflugsnapparhanar väljs därför i första hand av de svartvit flugsnapparhonor som inte hittar en artegen part- ner. Trots kostnaden av avsaknaden av vuxna karaktärsdrag hos ett åriga hanar bibehålls denna egenskap antagligen på grund av en ännu högre kost- nad att anlägga en vuxen fjäderskrud för unga hanar. Kanske skulle det leda till ökad aggression från äldre hanar. Mina resultat illustrerar en vanlig kritik mot idén att nedsatt fortplantingsförmåga hos hybrider leder till en urval- sprocess som driver artbilningsprocessen framåt och avslutar den. När hy- bridisering har blivit tillräckligt ovanligt så utgör hybridiseringsrisk inte

30 längre ett viktigt selektionstryck och utvecklingen mot en fullständig repro- duktiv isolering avstannar.

SLUTSATSER Den här avhandlingen presenterar det hitintills bästa måttet på styrkan av selektionen mot hybridisering i en naturlig population. Genom att undersöka selektion mot hybridisering i en naturlig miljö kan studier som min ta reda på vilka förhållande som gör att populationer som hybridiserar antingen slås samman eller separerar från varandra. Den typen av kunskap är kritisk för vår förståelse av uppkomsten och bevarandet av biodiversitet på jorden. Ut- vecklingen av genetiska hjälpmedel gör det realistiskt att spåra hybridisering i flera generationer och i mer detalj studera genflöde mellan populationer i hybridzoner även hos andra arter än flugsnappare. Jag fann att postzygotiska barriärer mot genflöde mellan flugsnapparar- terna är starka medan de prezygotiska bärriärerna är förhållandevis ofullkomliga. Den upptäckten kontrasterar med den tidigare uppfattningen att prezygotiska barriärer generellt utvecklas snabbare hos fåglar. Jag identi- fierade ett antal karaktärer (t.ex. förmåga att särskilja den egna artens sång, sångkopiering, åldersrelaterad fjäderdräkt) som ger upphov till en ofullkom- lig prezygotisk barriär. I förlängningen leder mina resultat till testbara förutsägelser om hur dessa karaktärer kommer att utvecklas i populationer som lever inom och utanför hybridzonen. Sådana jämförelser skulle kunna avslöja om reproduktiv isolering delvis utvecklas till följd av direkt selektion för ökad isolering i hybridzoner eller om den uppstår som en sidoeffekt av processer som helt sker inom arten. Ficedula hybridzonen är ett ypperligt studiesystem för en rad frågeställningar rörande artbildning, och min avhandling representerar ett viktigt första steg för att besvara några av dessa centrala frågor om arters uppkomst.

31 Acknowledgements

First, I’d like to extend thanks to my supervisors, Anna Qvarnström and Lars Gustafsson, for allowing me the opportunity to come to Uppsala and work on what has long been a wonderful system for studying animal ecology and evolution. A special thanks to you, Anna, for always being supportive and enthusiastic (even if sometimes overly optimistic!) throughout my work. I’ve really enjoyed working with you, and hope there will be plenty more oppor- tunities for this in the future. Being one of many in the ‘flycatcher group’ in Uppsala, there was always someone happy to lend advice about the system and provide great company through those hectic field months. Thank you Thor Veen, Nina Svedin, Mårten Hjernquist, Stein Are Sæther, Joanna Sen- decka, and Jukka Forsman for being a great team to work with. In particular, thanks Mårten for proofreading the introduction to this thesis. Thanks espe- cially to Nina, who never hesitates to drop everything to help out. It has been great having someone else alongside ‘for the ride’, from learning how to measure a flycatcher to writing up a thesis…not to mention the numerous nights hanging out watching the prime of British comedy and the worst of American reality TV! To my collaborators at the University of Oslo, Glenn Peter Sætre, Gunilla Andersson and Thomas Borge, thanks for being such a pleasure to work with and for being there to fall back on when the usual lab problems arose! Thanks especially to Glenn Peter, Maria, Helena and Camilla for kindly providing a home-away-from-home during my many visits to Norway. Thank you to Franjo Weissing for inviting me to spend time with the Theoretical Group at Groningen. Your kindness and passion for science is inspiring. To Thor, thanks for all the help during my stay in the Netherlands (at least when you finally escaped Turkish imprisonment), and for always being enjoyable company both during work and play. To the super- programmer, Richel Bilderbeek, thanks for your help, and best of luck with your welding/knitting/spicy-food-eating/music-writing/theatre-lighting/stand -up comedy. Thanks also to Liz, Laura and Stephanie for making my stay in the Netherlands lots of fun. Back at Uppsala I wish to thank Mats Björklund and Göran Arnqvist for always being happy to help out whenever I needed some statistical advice. To Katherine Thuman Hjernquist, Dagmar Jonsson and Reija Dufva thanks for teaching me the art of microsatellites and helping out in the lab. Thanks especially to Reija, who not only ran many of the paternity analyses, but also

32 checked through all the parasites. For great help in the field during the vari- ous field seasons, I wish to thank Katherine, Mathilda, Isobel, Mattias, Lisa, Janette, Kalle, and Selma. In addition, I would especially like to thank Johan Träff for not only kindly offering access to heterospecific pairs within his personally managed flycatcher population on Gotland, but also for sharing his wealth of knowledge about flycatcher biology. To my collaborators on the various papers, thank you for helping make this work possible. In addi- tion to those co-authors I’ve mentioned earlier, thanks Jenny Bengtson, Nilla Fogelberg and Jenny Vogel Kehlenbeck. Thanks to my many office-mates over the years (Göran S, Cath, Mårten, Emma, Katherine, Sara, Sarah and Mårten again), and to all the others that I had the pleasure of spending time with at the department (Alexei, Anders Ö., Anders B., Björn, Bo W., Claudia, Damian, Ingrid, Johanna A., Johanna R., Mari, Marta, Martin, Mar- nie, Niclas, Olivia, Olle, Sandra, Ted, Urban). It’s never easy moving so far from home, but the friendly, social environment at work and the wonderful people I shared the workplace with has made this a thoroughly enjoyable four years. On the same note, thanks to my non-Zooeko friends (Dick, Jo- hiris, Annika, Veronica, Henrik) for making my time in Sweden so much fun. I hope to see you all in Australia some time! To my family, accepting that I would spend four years away on the other side of the world cannot have been easy. I really appreciate that you made the effort to be able to come to the defense. I hope you get to see what I have been up to all this time! To Mum, Dad, Liam and Rach, I love you.

33 References

Alatalo, R. V., Gustafsson, L. & Lundberg, A. 1982. Hybridization and breeding success of collared and pied flycatchers on the island of Gotland. Auk 99, 285-291. Alatalo, R. V., Eriksson, D., Gustafsson, L. & Lundberg, A. 1990. Hybridi- zation between pied and collared flycatchers – sexual selection and spe- ciation theory. Journal of 3, 375-389. Alatalo, R. V., Gustafsson, L., and Lundberg, A. 1994. Male coloration and species recognition in sympatric flycatchers. Proceedings of the Royal So- ciety of London B 256, 113-118. Arnold, M. L. & Hodges, S. A. 1995. Are natural hybrids fit or unfit relative to their parents? in Ecology and Evolution 10, 67-71. Barton, N. H. 2001 The role of hybridization in evolution. Molecular Ecolo- gy 10, 551-568. Barton, N. H. & Hewitt, G. M. 1981. Hybrid zones and speciation. pp 109- 145 in Atchley & Woodruf (eds.) Evolution and Speciation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Benton, T. G. & Grant, A. 2000 Evolutionary fitness in ecology: comparing measures of fitness in stochastic, density-dependent environments. Evolutionary Ecology Research 2, 769-789. Bierne, N., David, P., Boudry, P. & Bonhomme, F. 2002. Assortative fertili- zation and selection at larval stage in the mussels Mytilius edulis and M. galloprovincialis. Evolution 56, 292-298. Bigelow, R. S. 1965. Hybrid zones and reproductive isolation. Evolution 19, 449-458. Blair, W. F. 1955. Mating call and stage of speciation in the Microhyla oli- vacea-M. carolinensis complex. Evolution 9, 469-480. Bleeker, W. & Matthies, A. 2005. Hybrid zones between invasive Rorippa austriaca and native R. sylvestris (Brassicaceae) in Germany: ploidy le- vels and patterns of fitness in the field. Heredity 94, 664-670. Borge, T., Webster, T. M., Andersson, G. & Sætre, G.-P. 2005. Contrasting patterns of polymorphism and divergence on the Z chromosome and auto- somes in two Ficedula flycatcher species. 171, 1-13 Burke, J. M. & Arnold, M. L. 2001 Genetics and the fitness of hybrids. An- nual Review of Genetics 35, 31-52. Butlin, R. K. 1987. Speciation by reinforcement. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 2, 8-13.

34 Butlin, R. K. 1989. Reinforcement of premating isolation. pp 158-179 in D. Otte & J. A. Endler (eds.) Speciation and Its Consequences. Sunderland, USA: Sinauer Associates. Coyne, J. A. & Orr, H. A. 2004. Speciation. Sunderland, USA: Sinauer As- sociates. Chu, P. C. 1994. Historical examination of delayed plumage maturation in the shorebirds (Aves, Charadriiformes). Evolution 48, 327-350. Dale, S. & Slagsvold, T. 1994. Male pied flycatchers do not choose mates. Animal Behaviour 47, 1197-1205. Dale, S. & Slagsvold, T. 1996. Mate choice on multiple cues, decision rules and sampling strategies in female pied flycatchers. Behaviour 133, 903- 944. Derothe, J.-M., Le Brun, N., Loubès, C., Perriat-Sanguinet, M. & Moulia, C. 2001. Susceptibility of natural hybrids between house mouse subspecies to Sarcocystis muris. International Journal for 31, 15-19. Dobzhansky, T. 1937. Genetics and the Origin of Species.NewYork:Co- lumbia University Press. Dowling, T. E. & DeMarais, B. D. 1993. Evolutionary significance of intro- gressive hybridization in cyprinid fishes. Nature 362, 444-446. Felsenstein, J. 1981. Skepticism towards Santa Rosalia, or Why are there so few kinds of animals? Evolution 35, 124-138. Fritz, R. S., Moulia, C. & Newcombe, G. 1999. Resistance of hybrid plants and animals to herbivores, , and parasites. Annual Review of Ecology and 30, 565-591. Gause, G. F. 1934. The Struggle for Existence. Baltimore, USA: Williams and Wilkins. Gelter, H. 1987. Song differences between the pied flycatcher Ficedula hy- poleuca, the collared flycatcher F. albicollis, and their hybrids. Ornis Scandinavica 18, 205-215. Gelter, H., Tegelström, H. & Gustafsson, L. 1992. Evidence from hatching success and DNA fingerprinting for the fertility of hybrid pied  collared flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca  albicollis. Ibis 134, 62-68. Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1992. Hybridization of bird species. Science 256, 193-197. Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1997. Genetics and the origin of bird species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94, 7768-7775. Gustafsson, L. & Qvarnström, A. 2006. A test of the “sexy son” hypothesis: sons of polygynous males do not inherit their fathers’ mating status. American Naturalist 167, 297-302. Gwynne, D. T. 1984. Courtship feeding increases female reproductive suc- cess in bushcrickets. Nature 307, 361-363. Haavie, J., Borge, T., Bures, S., Garamszegi, L. Z., Lampe, H. M., Moreno, J., Qvarnström, A., Török, J. & Sætre, G.-P. 2004. Flycatcher song in al-

35 lopatry and sympatry – convergence, divergence and reinforcement. Jour- nal of Evolutionary Biology 17, 227-237. Hughes, A. L. & Hughes, M. K. 1995. on the peptide- binding regions of major histocompatibility complex . Immunogenetics 42, 233-243. Johnson, L. P. V. 1939. A descriptive list of natural and artificial inter- specific hybrids in North American forest-tree genera. Canadian Journal of Research 17, 411-444. Kelly, J. K. & Noor, M. A. F. 1996. Speciation by reinforcement: a model derived from studies of Drosophila. Genetics 143, 1485-1497. Kirk, H., Vrieling, K. & Klinkhamer, P. G. L. 2005. Maternal effects and heterosis influence the fitness of plant hybrids. New Phytologist 166, 685- 694. Kirkpatrick, M. & Ryan, M. J. 1991. The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek. Nature 350, 33-38. Kirkpatrick, M. & Servedio, M. R. 1999. The reinforcement of mating pref- erences on an island. Genetics 151, 865-884. Lack, D. 1946. Competition for food by birds of prey. Journal of Animal Ecology 15, 123-129. Liou, L. W. & Price, T. D. 1994. Speciation by reinforcement of prezygotic isolation. Evolution 48, 1451-1459. Lundberg, A. & Alatalo, R. V. 1992. The Pied Flycatcher. London: T & A D Poyser. MacArthur, R. H. & Levins, R. 1964. Competition, habitat selection and character displacement in a patchy environment. Proceedings of the Na- tional Academy of Sciences 51, 1207-1210. MacArthur, R. H. & Levins, R. 1967. The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. American Naturalist 101, 377-385. Martens, J. 1996. Vocalizations and speciation of Palearctic birds. pp 221- 240 in D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller, (eds.) Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. New York: Cornell University Press. Mayr, E. 1995.Species, classification, and evolution. pp 3-12 in R. Arai, M. Kato & Y. Doi (eds.) and Evolution. Tokyo: National Sci- ence Museum Foundation. Moulia, C. 1999. of plant and animal hybrids: are facts and fates the same? Ecology 80, 392-406. Noor, M. A. 1999. Reinforcement and other consequences of sympatry. He- redity 83, 503-508. Parris, M. J., Semlitsch, R. D. & Sage, R. D. 1999. Experimental analysis of the evolutionary potential of hybridization in leopard frogs (Anura: Rani- dae) Journal of Evolutionary Biology 12, 662-671. Parris, M. 2004. Hybrid response to infection in interspecific crosses between two amphibian species (Anura: Ranidae). Evolutionary Ecology Research 6, 457-471.

36 Pertl, M., Hauser, T. P., Damgaard, C. & Jorgensen, R. B. 2002. Male fitness of oilseed rape (Brassica napus), weedy B. rapa and their F1 hybrids when pollinating B. rapa seeds. Heredity 89, 212-218. Peterson, M. A., Monsen, K. J., Pedersen, H., McFarland, T. & Bearden, J. 2005. Direct and indirect analysis of the fitness of Chrysochus (Coleopte- ra: Chrysomelidae) hybrids. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 84, 273.286. Pärt, T. & Qvarnström, A. 1997. Badge size in collared flycatchers predicts the outcome of male competition over territories. Animal Behaviour 54, 893-899. Qvarnström, A. 1997. Experimentally increased badge size increases male competition and reduces male parental care in the collared flycatcher. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 264, 1225-1231. Qvarnström, A. 1999. Different reproductive tactics in male collared fly- catchers signalled by size of secondary sexual character. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 266, 2089-2093. Qvarnström, A., Pärt, T. & Sheldon, B. C. 2000. Adaptive plasticity in mate preference linked to differences in reproductive effort. Nature 405, 344- 347. Qvarnström, A., Svedin, N., Wiley, C., Veen, T. & Gustafsson, L. 2005. Cross-fostering reveals seasonal changes in the relative fitness of two competing species of flycatchers. Biology Letters 1, 68-71. Qvarnström, A., Haavie, J., Sæther, S.A., Eriksson, D. & Pärt, T. 2006. Song similarity predicts hybridization in flycatchers. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19, 1202-1209. Ramsey, J., Bradshaw, H. D. Jr. & Schemske, D. W. 2003. Components of reproductive isolation between the monkeyflowers Mimulus lewisii and M. cardinalis (Phrymaceae). Evolution 57, 1520-1534. Randler, C. 2002. Avian hybridization, mixed pairing and female choice. Animal Behaviour 63, 103-119. Reynolds, J. D. & Gross, M. R. 1990. Costs and benefits of female mate choice: Is there a lek paradox? American Naturalist 136, 230-243. Rieseberg, L. H. & Carney, S. E. 1998. Plant hybridisation. New Phytologist 140, 599-624. Searcy, W. A. 1979. Female choice of mates: A general model for birds and its application to red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoenicus). American Naturalist 114, 77-100. Servedio, M. R. 2000. Reinforcement and the genetics of nonrandom mating. Evolution 54, 21-29. Servedio, M. R. & Kirkpatrick, M. 1997. The effects of gene flow on rein- forcement. Evolution 51, 1764-1772. Sheldon, B. C. & Ellegren, H. 1999. Sexual selection resulting from extra- pair paternity in collared flycatchers. Animal Behaviour 57, 285-298.

37 Spencer, H. G., McArdle, B. H. & Lambert, D. M. 1986. A theoretical inves- tigation of speciation by reinforcement. American Naturalist 128, 241- 262. Svedin, N. 2006. Natural and sexual selection in a natural hybrid zone of Ficedula flycatchers. Acta Universitalis Upsaliensis, Ph.D. Thesis, Upp- sala University, Sweden. Sætre, G.-P., Moum, T., Bures, S., Král, M., Adamjan, M. & Moreno, J. 1997a. A sexually selected character displacement in flycatchers rein- forces premating isolation. Nature 387, 589-592. Sætre, G.-P., Král, M. & Bures, S. 1997b. Differential species recognition abilities of males and females in a flycatcher hybrid zone. Journal of Avian Biology 28, 259-263. Sætre, G.-P., Post, E. & Král, M. 1999. Can environmental fluctuation pre- vent competitive exclusion in sympatric flycatchers? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 266, 1247-1251. Sætre, G.-P., Borge, T., Lindell, J., Moum, T., Primmer, C. R., Sheldon, B. C., Haavie, J., Johnsen, A. & Ellegren, H. 2001. Speciation, introgressive hybridization and nonlinear rate of molecular evolution in flycatchers. Molecular Ecology 10, 737-749. Sætre, G.-P., Borge, T., Lindroos, K., Haavie, J., Sheldon, B. C., Primmer, C. & Sylvänen, A.-C. 2003. Sex chromosome evolution and speciation in Ficedula flycatchers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270, 53-59. Thornhill, R. 1976. Sexual selection and paternal investment in insects. American Naturalist 110, 153-163. Veen, T., Borge, T., Griffith, S. C., Sætre, G.-P., Bures, S., Gustafsson, L. & Sheldon, B. C. 2001. Hybridization and adaptive mate choice in flycatch- ers. Nature 411, 45-50. Wegner, K. M., Reusch, T. B. H. & Kalbe, M. 2003. Multiple parasites are driving major histocompatibility complex polymorphism in the wild. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16, 224-232. Westerdahl, H., Waldenström, J., Hansson, B., Hasselquist, D., von Schantz, T. & Bensch, S. 2005. Associations between and MHC genes in a migratory songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 272, 1511-1518. Whitham, T. G., Morrow, P. A. & Potts, B. M. 1994. Plant hybrid zones as centers of biodiversity: the herbivore of two endemic Tasma- nian eucalypts. Oecologia 97, 481-490. Wolinska, J., Keller, B., Bittner, K., Lass, S. & Spaak, P. 2004. Do parasites lower Daphnia hybrid fitness? Limnology and Oceanography 49, 1401- 1407.

38

Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Science and Technology 245

Editor: The Dean of the Faculty of Science and Technology

A doctoral dissertation from the Faculty of Science and Technology, Uppsala University, is usually a summary of a number of papers. A few copies of the complete dissertation are kept at major Swedish research libraries, while the summary alone is distributed internationally through the series Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Science and Technology. (Prior to January, 2005, the series was published under the title “Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Science and Technology”.)

ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS Distribution: publications.uu.se UPPSALA urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-7358 2006