PROCESSES in BIOLOGICAL VISION: Including

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

PROCESSES in BIOLOGICAL VISION: Including PROCESSES IN BIOLOGICAL VISION: including, ELECTROCHEMISTRY OF THE NEURON This material is excerpted from the full β-version of the text. The final printed version will be more concise due to further editing and economical constraints. A Table of Contents and an index are located at the end of this paper. James T. Fulton Vision Concepts [email protected] April 30, 2017 Copyright 2004 James T. Fulton The Retina 3- 1 3 Description of the Retina1 3.1 Introduction A major finding of this work is that the retinula of the Insecta and some Mollusca eyes do not employ opsin as a substrate for the appropriate chromophores, which remain the rhodopsin()’s of the retinine family (section 3.6). The retinines are deposited directly on the villi emanating from the dendrites of the sensory receptor neurons. This finding has wide ramifications relating to the conventional concepts of the sensory receptors of Chordata. The opsin of these sensory receptors is used to control the orientation of the retinine liquid crystalline chromophores. It does not participate in the transduction process. Innumerable representations of the retina appear in the literature, at all different scales and using every imaginable artistic technique. Most of them do not show an absolute scale or even the coordinates of the retinal area being discussed. They virtually never show the direction and optical quality of the incident radiation or the location of the focal plane of the optical system. When these parameters are used as a foundation of discussion, many sketches, photographs and electron micrographs are found inadequate. Some become interpretable from a different perspective. The purpose of this Chapter is to provide a more comprehensive view of the retina, including a more coordinated description of the different zones of the retina, and a broader understanding of the top level architecture of the visual signaling system pertaining to the retina. Recently, good progress has been made in determining the distribution of different chromophorically sensitive photoreceptors in the retina. However, the results are still largely statistical. Initially, this chapter will examine all photosensitive surfaces that contribute to the visual capabilities of an animal. In this context, the group of retinula found in the eyes of Arthropoda will be considered a retina. This provides a useful element in the development of an overall framework for discussing the retina. Traditionally, the eyes of animals have been separated into two categories, vertebrate and invertebrate. Complete books have been written based on this distinction2. Bernhard has provided a comprehensive study of the compound eye3. Laughlin has provided an extensive comparison between these two categories4. However, this use of a dichotomy is constraining. As discussed briefly in Chapter 1, there are clearly three categories of eyes that are well aligned with the conventional phylogenic tree. These are the compound eye, and its prototypical simple eye, of Arthropoda, the complex eye with a direct retina of Mollusca, and the complex eye with a reversed retina of Chordata. Evolution has led to incongruities within this classification but they are minor–and illustrative. This work will employ the above trichotomy instead of the conventional dichotomy. The above trichotomy provides a much better framework for interpreting both the detailed form of the types of photoreceptor cells found in eyes and the degree of structure in the retinas of these phyla. The above trichotomy of eye types should not be confused with the spectral capability of eyes. The fundamental architecture of biological vision is tetrachromatic. Provision is made in the photochemistry of vision to form photoreceptors of four spectral types and many superfamilies and families of animals are tetrachromatic. It is only within phyla that one finds trichromats and they are of two distinct types. Arthropoda are generally short wavelength trichromats, employing photoreceptors sensitive to the ultraviolet, short and medium wavelength spectral ranges. Chordata are generally tetrachromatic, except in the physically larger animals and sometimes as a result of evolution to satisfy an ecological niche. Tetrachromats have photoreceptors sensitive to the ultraviolet, short, medium and long wavelength regions of the visible spectrum. Many birds5, fish and rodents are known to be tetrachromatic. Tetrachromaticity is common among fish6. However, many species exhibit tetrachromatic vision as 1Released: April 30, 2017 2Autrum, H. ed. (1979) Comparative Physiology and Evolution of Vision in Invertebrates: A. Invertebrate Photoreceptors. NY: Springer-Verlag 3Bernhard, C. (1965) The Functional Organization of the Compound Eye. NY: Pergamon Press 4Laughlin, S. (1981) Common principles for vertebrate and invertebrate visual systems. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VII/6B, “Comparative physiology and evolution of vision in invertebrates,” Autrum, H. ed. NY: Springer-Verlag pg. 263 5Altshuler, D. et. al. Xxx Evol. Ecol. Res. vol. 3, p 767 6Neumeyer, C. & Arnold, K. (1989) Tetrachromatic colour vision in goldfish and turtle. Xxx In 2 Processes in Biological Vision juveniles but frequently change to long wavelength trichromats with age (or more fundamentally, as the lens group grows and thickens). The larger members of Chordata, particularly the larger terrestrial members such as Human, have lost their ultraviolet sensitivity due to the thickness of the lens group serving the eye. It is not clear that the retinas of all large Chordata have completely lost their ultraviolet capability through evolution. Humans exhibit at least some ultraviolet capability when the lens group is removed. The large chordates are long wavelength trichromats, having photoreceptors sensitive to the short, medium and long wavelength portions of the visual spectrum. The data available for Mollusca is inadequate to determine their general capability. 3.1.1 Background 3.1.1.1 Order and tetra-chromaticity in the photoreceptor arrays of the retinas By examining the trichotomy of animal retinas, there is a clear trend with regard to the statistical order of the arrays of photoreceptors. There also appears to be an order with respect to these arrays in many animals as a function of their maturity. Franceschini has provided a striking example of the orderliness of the retinal array in Arthropoda7. This orderliness even extends to the lamina behind the retina. The orderliness is clearly traceable to the prototypical simple eye. The orderliness is reminiscent of that of a crystalline structure. Bowmaker & Kunz have described a similar level of orderliness in the immature brown trout8. They noted that the orderliness appeared to fall with age and surmised that the ultraviolet photoreceptors tended to disappear from the retina. Their images were primarily of small areas of the retina. Their spectrograms of the four chromophores of vision, although somewhat limited at the extreme wavelengths and plotted on a normalized linear ordinate, are in close agreement with those predicted by this work. Kouyama & Marshak studied the statistics of two mosaics of retinal neurons in the primate retina9. The areas were in the mid-periphery, typically six millimeters from the macaque fovea, and consisted of about 150 photoreceptors and about twice as many signal processing neurons described as bipolar cells. Unfortunately, they did not employ optical stimuli to excite the arrays. They employed staining of what were believed to be photoreceptors and bipolar cells associated with the short wavelength spectral channels of vision. Therefore, the arrays were not directly correlated with the spectral performance of the visual system. Chan, et. al. provide both morphology and statistics on the distribution of horizontal cells of the 1st lateral matrix in new world monkeys, with some comparisons with the macaque monkey10. They provide a picture of one horizontal cell, at 6.9 millimeters from the fovea, with a neuritic arborization of less than 50 microns in diameter but an axon that is 958 microns long. This length approaches the maximum reported for neurons processing signals in the electrotonic (analog) domain. Chan, et. al. also reference the paper by Dacey defining two distinct types of horizontal cells that appear to correspond to the P-channel and Q-channel horizontal cells of this work11. The H2 horizontal cells respond to short wavelength stimulation. Both the H1 and H2 type cells respond to mid-wavelength stimulation. The H1 type cells respond to both mid and long wavelength stimulation but do not respond to short wavelength stimulation. The H1 cells generate Q-channel chrominance signals and the H2 cells generate P-channel chrominance signals according to the nomenclature of this work. Chan, et. al. confirm the polymorphism of color vision in the marmoset based primarily on their ability to get it to make color matches following training. They claim trichromatism in females of that family. However, they claim the other animals are dichromats because they did not respond. 7Franceschini, N. (1985) XXX 8Bowmaker, J. & Kunz, Y. (1987) Ultraviolet receptors, tetrachromatic colour vision and retinal mosaics in the broun trout (Salmo trutta): age-dependent changes. Vision Res. vol. 27, no. 12, pp. 2101-2108 9Kouyama, N. & Marshak, D. (1997) The topographical relationship between two neuronal mosaics in the short wavelength-sensitive system of the primate retina. Visual Neuroscience, vol. 14, pp. 159-167 10Chan,
Recommended publications
  • Seeing Through Moving Eyes
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/083691; this version posted June 1, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 Seeing through moving eyes - microsaccadic information sampling provides 2 Drosophila hyperacute vision 3 4 Mikko Juusola1,2*‡, An Dau2‡, Zhuoyi Song2‡, Narendra Solanki2, Diana Rien1,2, David Jaciuch2, 5 Sidhartha Dongre2, Florence Blanchard2, Gonzalo G. de Polavieja3, Roger C. Hardie4 and Jouni 6 Takalo2 7 8 1National Key laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing, Beijing Normal 9 University, Beijing 100875, China 10 2Department of Biomedical Science, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 T2N, UK 11 3Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Center for the Unknown, Lisbon, 12 Portugal 13 4Department of Physiology Development and Neuroscience, Cambridge University, Cambridge CB2 14 3EG, UK 15 16 *Correspondence to: [email protected] 17 ‡ Equal contribution 18 19 Small fly eyes should not see fine image details. Because flies exhibit saccadic visual behaviors 20 and their compound eyes have relatively few ommatidia (sampling points), their photoreceptors 21 would be expected to generate blurry and coarse retinal images of the world. Here we 22 demonstrate that Drosophila see the world far better than predicted from the classic theories. 23 By using electrophysiological, optical and behavioral assays, we found that R1-R6 24 photoreceptors’ encoding capacity in time is maximized to fast high-contrast bursts, which 25 resemble their light input during saccadic behaviors. Whilst over space, R1-R6s resolve moving 26 objects at saccadic speeds beyond the predicted motion-blur-limit.
    [Show full text]
  • Circadian Clocks in Crustaceans: Identified Neuronal and Cellular Systems
    Circadian clocks in crustaceans: identified neuronal and cellular systems Johannes Strauss, Heinrich Dircksen Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, Svante Arrhenius vag 18A, S-10691 Stockholm, Sweden TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. Abstract 2. Introduction: crustacean circadian biology 2.1. Rhythms and circadian phenomena 2.2. Chronobiological systems in Crustacea 2.3. Pacemakers in crustacean circadian systems 3. The cellular basis of crustacean circadian rhythms 3.1. The retina of the eye 3.1.1. Eye pigment migration and its adaptive role 3.1.2. Receptor potential changes of retinular cells in the electroretinogram (ERG) 3.2. Eyestalk systems and mediators of circadian rhythmicity 3.2.1. Red pigment concentrating hormone (RPCH) 3.2.2. Crustacean hyperglycaemic hormone (CHH) 3.2.3. Pigment-dispersing hormone (PDH) 3.2.4. Serotonin 3.2.5. Melatonin 3.2.6. Further factors with possible effects on circadian rhythmicity 3.3. The caudal photoreceptor of the crayfish terminal abdominal ganglion (CPR) 3.4. Extraretinal brain photoreceptors 3.5. Integration of distributed circadian clock systems and rhythms 4. Comparative aspects of crustacean clocks 4.1. Evolution of circadian pacemakers in arthropods 4.2. Putative clock neurons conserved in crustaceans and insects 4.3. Clock genes in crustaceans 4.3.1. Current knowledge about insect clock genes 4.3.2. Crustacean clock-gene 4.3.3. Crustacean period-gene 4.3.4. Crustacean cryptochrome-gene 5. Perspective 6. Acknowledgements 7. References 1. ABSTRACT Circadian rhythms are known for locomotory and reproductive behaviours, and the functioning of sensory organs, nervous structures, metabolism and developmental processes. The mechanisms and cellular bases of control are mainly inferred from circadian phenomenologies, ablation experiments and pharmacological approaches.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Properties of Human Retinal Ganglion Cells
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/766170; this version posted August 14, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. Visual properties of human retinal ganglion cells Katja Reinhard1,2,3* & Thomas A. Münch1,4* 1 Retinal Circuits and Optogenetics, Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Otfried-Müller-Str. 25, 72076 Tübingen, Germany; 2 Neuroscience Graduate School, University of Tübingen, Österbergstraße 3, 72074 Tübingen Germany; 3 Current address: Neuro- electronics Research Flanders, Kapeldreef 75, 3001 Leuven, Belgium; 4 Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen, Elfriede-Aulhorn-Straße 7, 72076 Tübingen, Germany * Corresponding authors Katja Reinhard: [email protected] and Thomas A. Münch: [email protected] The retinal output is the sole source of visual information for the brain. Studies in non-primate mammals estimate that this information is carried by several dozens of retinal ganglion cell types, each informing the brain about different aspects of a visual scene. Even though morphological studies of primate retina suggest a similar diversity of ganglion cell types, research has focused on the function of only a few cell types. In human retina, recordings from individual cells are anecdotal or focus on a small subset of identified types. Here, we present the first systematic ex-vivo recording of light responses from 342 ganglion cells in human retinas obtained from donors.
    [Show full text]
  • Attention and Individual Behavioural Variation in Small-Brained Animals – Using Bumblebees and Zebrafish As Model Systems
    Attention and individual behavioural variation in small-brained animals – using bumblebees and zebrafish as model systems Mu-Yun Wang Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Queen Mary, University of London 2013 1 Abstract A vital ability for an animal is to filter the constant flow of sensory input from the environment to focus on the most important information. Attention is used to prioritize sensory input for adaptive responses. The role of attention in visual search has been studied extensively in human and non-human primates, but is much less studied in other animals. We looked at attentional mechanisms, especially selective and divided attention where animals focus on multiple cues at the same time, using a visual search paradigm. We targeted bumblebee and zebrafish as model species because they are widely used as tractable models of information processing in comparatively small brains. Bees were required to forage from target and distractor flowers in the presence of predators. We found that bees could selectively attend to certain dimension of the stimuli, and divide their attention to both visual foraging search and predator avoidance tasks simultaneously. Furthermore, bees showed consistent individual differences in foraging strategy; ‘careful’ and ‘impulsive’ strategies exist in individuals of the same colony. From the calculation of foraging rate, it is shown that the best strategy may depend on environmental conditions. We applied a similar behavioural paradigm to zebrafish and found speed-accuracy tradeoffs and consistent individual behavioural differences. We therefore continued to test how individuality influences group choices. In pairs of careful and impulsive fish, the consensus decision is close to the strategy of the careful individual.
    [Show full text]
  • Anatomy of the Regional Differences in the Eye of the Mantis Ciulfina
    J. exp. Biol. (i979). 80, 165-190 165 With 17 figures Printed in Great Britain ANATOMY OF THE REGIONAL DIFFERENCES IN THE EYE OF THE MANTIS CIULFINA BY G. A. HORRIDGE AND PETER DUELLI Department of Neurobiology, Research School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University, Canberra, A.C.T. 2601, Australia (Received 8 August 1978) SUMMARY 1. In the compound eye of Ciulfina (Mantidae) there are large regional differences in interommatidial angle as measured optically from the pseudo- pupil. Notably there is an acute zone which looks backwards as well as one looking forwards. There are correlated regional differences in the dimensions of the ommatidia. 2. The following anatomical features which influence the optical perform- ance have been measured in different parts of the eye: (a) The facet diameter is greater where the interommatidial angle is smaller. This could influence resolving power, but calculation shows that facet size does not exert a dominant effect on the visual fields of the receptors. (b) The rhabdom tip diameter, which theoretically has a strong influence on the size of visual fields, is narrower in eye regions where the inter- ommatidial angle is smaller. (c) The cone length, from which the focal length can be estimated, is greater where the interommatidial angle is smaller. 3. Estimation of the amount of light reaching the rhabdom suggests that different parts of the eye have similar sensitivity to a point source of light, but differ by a factor of at least 10 in sensitivity to an extended source. 4. There is anatomical evidence that in the acute zone the sensitivity has been sacrificed for the sake of resolution.
    [Show full text]
  • Diurnal Changes in Angular Sensitivity of Crab Photoreceptors Leggett, L.M.W.; Stavenga, D.G
    University of Groningen Diurnal Changes in Angular Sensitivity of Crab Photoreceptors Leggett, L.M.W.; Stavenga, D.G. Published in: Journal of Comparative Physiology A DOI: 10.1007/BF00612803 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 1981 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Leggett, L. M. W., & Stavenga, D. G. (1981). Diurnal Changes in Angular Sensitivity of Crab Photoreceptors. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 144(1), 99-109. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00612803 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.
    [Show full text]
  • Measuring Compound Eye Optics with Microscope and Microct Images
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.11.422154; this version posted December 12, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 1 Title (<120 characters): Measuring Compound Eye Optics with Microscope and MicroCT Images 2 Article Type: Tools and Resources 3 Author Names and Affiliations: John Paul Currea1, Yash Sondhi2, Akito Y. Kawahara3, and Jamie 4 Theobald2 5 1Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, U.S.A. 6 2Department of Biological Sciences, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, U.S.A. 7 3Florida Museum of Natural History, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, 32611, U.S.A. 8 Abstract (<150 words): 9 The arthropod compound eye is the most prevalent eye type in the animal kingdom, with an impressive 10 range of shapes and sizes. Studying its natural range of morphologies provides insight into visual 11 ecology, development, and evolution. In contrast to the camera-type eyes we possess, external 12 structures of compound eyes often reveal resolution, sensitivity, and field of view if the eye is 13 spherical. Non-spherical eyes, however, require measuring internal structures using imaging 14 technology like MicroCT (µCT). Thus far, there is no efficient tool to automate characterizing 15 compound eye optics. We present two open-source programs: (1) the ommatidia detecting algorithm 16 (ODA), which automatically measures ommatidia count and diameter, and (2) a µCT pipeline, which 17 calculates anatomical acuity, sensitivity, and field of view across the eye by applying the ODA.
    [Show full text]
  • Feller, KD, Wilby, D., Jacucci, G., Vignolini, S., Mantell, J., Wardill, TJ
    Feller, K. D., Wilby, D., Jacucci, G., Vignolini, S., Mantell, J., Wardill, T. J., Cronin, T. W., & Roberts, N. W. (2019). Long-Wavelength Reflecting Filters Found in the Larval Retinas of One Mantis Shrimp Family (Nannosquillidae). Current Biology, 29(18), 3101-3108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.070 Peer reviewed version License (if available): CC BY-NC-ND Link to published version (if available): 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.070 Link to publication record in Explore Bristol Research PDF-document This is the author accepted manuscript (AAM). The final published version (version of record) is available online via Elsevier at https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982219309509 . Please refer to any applicable terms of use of the publisher. University of Bristol - Explore Bristol Research General rights This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the reference above. Full terms of use are available: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/red/research-policy/pure/user-guides/ebr-terms/ 1 Title 2 Long-wavelength reflecting filters found in the larval retinas of one mantis shrimp family 3 (Nannosquillidae) 4 5 Authors 1 ,2,3* 3,4 5 5 6 6 Kathryn D. Feller † , David Wilby †, Gianni Jacucci , Silvia Vignolini , Judith Mantell , 1 ,2 7 3 7 Trevor J. Wardill † , Thomas W. Cronin , Nicholas W. Roberts 8 9 Affiliations 1 10 Ecology, Evolution and Behavior Department, 1479 Gortner Avenue, University of 11 Minnesota, St. Paul, Minnesota, 55108, USA 2 12 Physiology Development and Neuroscience Department, Physiological Laboratories, 13 Downing Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EG, UK.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Properties and Functional Organization of Small Bistratified Ganglion Cells in Primate Retina
    The Journal of Neuroscience, November 28, 2007 • 27(48):13261–13272 • 13261 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Spatial Properties and Functional Organization of Small Bistratified Ganglion Cells in Primate Retina Greg D. Field,1* Alexander Sher,2* Jeffrey L. Gauthier,1* Martin Greschner,1 Jonathon Shlens,1 Alan M. Litke,2 and E. J. Chichilnisky1 1Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, California 92037, and 2Santa Cruz Institute for Particle Physics, University of California, Santa Cruz, California 95064 The primate visual system consists of parallel pathways initiated by distinct cell types in the retina that encode different features of the visual scene. Small bistratified cells (SBCs), which form a major projection to the thalamus, exhibit blue-ON/yellow-OFF [S-ON/(LϩM)- OFF] light responses thought to be important for high-acuity color vision. However, the spatial processing properties of individual SBCs and their spatial arrangement across the visual field are poorly understood. The present study of peripheral primate retina reveals that contrary to previous suggestions, SBCs exhibit center-surround spatial structure, with the (LϩM)-OFF component of the receptive field ϳ50% larger in diameter than the S-ON component. Analysis of response kinetics shows that the (LϩM)-OFF response in SBCs is slower thantheS-ONresponseandsignificantlylesstransientthanthatofsimultaneouslyrecordedOFF-parasolcells.The(LϩM)-OFFresponse in SBCs was eliminated by bath application of the metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist L-APB. These observations indicate that the (LϩM)-OFF response of SBCs is not formed by OFF-bipolar cell input as has been suspected and suggest that it arises from horizontal cell feedback. Finally, the receptive fields of SBCs form orderly mosaics, with overlap and regularity similar to those of ON-parasol cells.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptation in Cone Photoreceptors Contributes to an Unexpected Insensitivity of on Parasol Retinal Ganglion Cells to Spatial Structure in Natural Images
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.06.29.450295; this version posted June 30, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. Adaptation in cone photoreceptors contributes to an unexpected insensitivity of On parasol retinal ganglion cells to spatial structure in natural images Zhou Yu*, Maxwell H. Turner*, Jacob Baudin, Fred Rieke Department of Physiology and Biophysics University of Washington * equal contributions Abstract Neural circuits are constructed from nonlinear building blocks, and not surprisingly overall circuit behavior is often strongly nonlinear. But neural circuits can also behave near linearly, and some circuits shift from linear to nonlinear behavior depending on stimulus conditions. Such control of the linearity or nonlinearity of circuit behavior is fundamental to neural computation. Here we study a surprising stimulus dependence of the responses of On (but not Off) parasol retinal ganglion cells: these cells respond nonlinearly to spatial structure in temporally-modulated grating stimuli but linearly to spatial structure in flashed gratings and natural visual inputs. We show that this unexpected response linearity can be explained by a shift in the balance of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs that originates at least in part from adaptation in the cone photoreceptors. More generally, this highlights
    [Show full text]
  • (Lepidoptera: Lycaenidae) Und Ihre Bedeutung Fur¨ Partnerwahl Und Arterkennung
    Flavonoidinduzierte ph¨anotypische Plastizit¨at in der Flugelf¨ ¨arbung des Bl¨aulings Polyommatus icarus (Lepidoptera: Lycaenidae) und ihre Bedeutung fur¨ Partnerwahl und Arterkennung Dissertation zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften – Dr. rer. nat – der Fakult¨at fur¨ Biologie, Chemie und Geowissenschaften der Universit¨at Bayreuth vorgelegt von Helge Knuttel¨ Januar 2003 Die experimentellen Arbeiten fur¨ die vorliegende Arbeit wurden in der Zeit von November 1996 bis Dezember 2000 am Lehrstuhl Tier¨okologie I der Universit¨at Bayreuth in der Arbeitsgruppe von Herrn Prof. Dr. Konrad Fiedler angefertigt. Die sehphysiologischen Untersuchungen wurden bei Herrn Prof. Dr. Rudolf Schwind am Lehrstuhl fur¨ Zoologie VI an der Universit¨at Regensburg durchgefuhrt.¨ Vollst¨andiger Abdruck der von der Fakult¨at fur¨ Biologie, Chemie und Geowissenschaf- ten der Universit¨at Bayreuth genehmigten Dissertation zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften (Dr. rer. nat). Tag der Einreichung: 06.02.2003 Tag des Kolloquiums: 16.07.2003 Erstgutachter: Prof. Dr. Konrad Fiedler Zweitgutachter: Prof. Dr. Rudolf Schwind Prufungsvorsitzender:¨ Prof. Dr. Gerhard Rambold Weitere Prufer:¨ Prof. Dr. Konrad Dettner, Prof. Dr. Gerhard Platz Einige Ergebnisse dieser Arbeit wurden bereits ver¨offentlicht: • Knuttel,¨ H. & K. Fiedler (1999) Flavonoids from larval food plants determine UV wing patterns in Polyommatus icarus (Lepidoptera: Lycaenidae). Zoology 102(Suppl. 2 (DZG 92.1)): 83. • Burghardt, F., H. Knuttel,¨ M. Becker & K. Fiedler (2000) Flavonoid wing pigments increase attractiveness of female common blue (Polyommatus icarus) butterflies to mate-searching males. Naturwissenschaften 87(7): 304–307. • Knuttel,¨ H. & K. Fiedler (2000) On the use of ultraviolet photography and ultra- violet wing patterns in butterfly morphology and taxonomy.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hermann Grid Illusion Revisited
    Perception, 2005, volume 34, pages 1375 ^ 1397 DOI:10.1068/p5447 The Hermann grid illusion revisited Peter H Schiller, Christina E Carvey Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; e-mail: [email protected] Received 12 October 2004, in revised form 12 January 2005; published online 23 September 2005 Abstract. The Hermann grid illusion consists of smudges perceived at the intersections of a white grid presented on a black background. In 1960 the effect was first explained by a theory advanced by Baumgartner suggesting the illusory effect is due to differences in the discharge characteristics of retinal ganglion cells when their receptive fields fall along the intersections versus when they fall along non-intersecting regions of the grid. Since then, others have claimed that this theory might not be adequate, suggesting that a model based on cortical mechanisms is necessary [Lingelbach et al, 1985 Perception 14(1) A7; Spillmann, 1994 Perception 23 691 ^ 708; Geier et al, 2004 Perception 33 Supplement, 53; Westheimer, 2004 Vision Research 44 2457 ^ 2465]. We present in this paper the following evidence to show that the retinal ganglion cell theory is untenable: (i) varying the makeup of the grid in a manner that does not materially affect the putative differ- ential responses of the ganglion cells can reduce or eliminate the illusory effect; (ii) varying the grid such as to affect the putative differential responses of the ganglion cells does not eliminate the illusory effect; and (iii) the actual spatial layout of the retinal ganglion cell receptive fields is other than that assumed by the theory.
    [Show full text]