G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

1

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

j ournal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/dcn

2 Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral

3 networks in the human brain

a a,b,∗

4 Q1 Kristafor Farrant , Lucina Q. Uddin

a

5 Q2 Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States

b

6 Neuroscience Program, University of Miami, Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL, United States

7

a r a

t i c b s t

8 l e i n f o r a c t

9

10 Article history: Two neural systems for goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention have been described in

11 Received 30 June 2014

the adult human brain; the (DAN) centered in the frontal eye fields

12 Received in revised form 26 January 2015

(FEF) and (IPS), and the ventral attention network (VAN) anchored in the

13 Accepted 4 February 2015

temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and ventral frontal cortex (VFC). Little is known regarding

14 Available online xxx

the processes governing typical development of these attention networks in the brain. Here

15

we use resting state functional MRI data collected from thirty 7 to 12 year-old children and

16 Keywords:

thirty 18 to 31 year-old adults to examine two key regions of interest from the dorsal

17 Dorsal attention network

and ventral attention networks. We found that for the DAN nodes (IPS and FEF), children

18 Ventral attention network

showed greater functional connectivity with regions within the network compared with

19

adults, whereas adults showed greater functional connectivity between the FEF and extra-

20 Resting state fMRI

21 Typical development network regions including the posterior cingulate cortex. For the VAN nodes (TPJ and VFC),

22 Functional connectivity adults showed greater functional connectivity with regions within the network compared

with children. Children showed greater functional connectivity between VFC and nodes

of the salience network. This asymmetric pattern of development of attention networks

may be a neural signature of the shift from over-representation of bottom-up attention

mechanisms to greater top-down attentional capacities with development.

© 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND

license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

23 1. Introduction 2008). The DAN shows sustained activation when focusing 33

attention on an object (Corbetta et al., 2008), and is thought 34

24Q3 It is well established that there are two partially to be responsible for goal-directed, top-down processing 35

25 segregated attention networks in the human brain; the (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). The second network, the 36

26 so-called dorsal and ventral attention networks (Corbetta ventral attention network (VAN), is comprised of the tem- 37

27 and Shulman, 2002). The dorsal attention network (DAN) poroparietal junction (TPJ) and ventral frontal cortex (VFC), 38

28 includes bilateral intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the frontal and responds to relevant external environmental stim- 39

29 eye fields (FEF), and is concerned with orientating ones uli. The VAN is dominant in the right hemisphere, and is 40

30 focus on a particular task. Previous work has demonstrated generally activated when an unexpected event occurs and 41

31 that the FEF and IPS exert top-down influences on visual breaks ones attention from the current task (i.e. bottom- 42

32 areas during visual orienting of attention (Bressler et al., up processing) (for a full review of the DAN & VAN see 43

Corbetta et al., 2008). This network’s key function is to 44

direct attention to stimuli outside of the current focus of 45

∗ processing and is referred to as the ‘circuit breaking’ section 46

Corresponding author at: University of Miami, P.O. Box 248185-0751,

of the two attention networks (Shulman et al., 2002). Cor- 47

Coral Gables, FL 33124, United States. Tel.: +1 305 284 3265.

E-mail address: [email protected] (L.Q. Uddin). betta and colleagues argue that only behaviorally relevant 48

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

1878-9293/© 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license

(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

2 K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

49 environmental stimuli trigger the VAN, and that the differences in the dorsal attention network found stronger 110

50 response of the VAN is suppressed when irrelevant infor- within-network connectivity in the DAN in 11–13 year-old 111

51 mation is presented (Corbetta et al., 2008). The VFC has children compared with 19–25 year old adults (Jolles et al., 112

52 been identified to be mainly active when reorienting is 2011). Seed-based approaches can offer complementary 113

53 unexpected and requires cognitive control or is coupled information to that derived from ICA, namely allowing for 114

54 to a response (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). The two core hypothesis-driven analyses of specific functional networks 115

55 regions of the VAN are typically co-activated, as well as of interest (Uddin et al., 2010). To the best of our knowledge, 116

56 functionally connected (Fox et al., 2006; He et al., 2007). no previous studies have used seed-based approaches to 117

57 Another brain system termed the ‘salience network’ explore developmental differences in functional connectiv- 118

58 has also been linked with functions that partially overlap ity of DAN and VAN nodes, or inter-network relationships. 119

59 with functions ascribed to the VAN, including responding Here we use R-fMRI to explore the typical development 120

60 to behaviorally relevant stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007). The of the DAN and VAN in the human brain. We aimed to 121

61 salience network is comprised of dorsal anterior cingu- test the following hypotheses: (1) R-fMRI can be used to 122

62 late, subcortical, and limbic structures, as well as bilateral identify the DAN and VAN in children, and these networks 123

63 anterior insular cortices adjacent to or overlapping the VFC have a similar topological organization to that observed in 124

64 node of the VAN (Uddin, 2015), and has been demonstrated adults; (2) long-range functional connectivity of DAN and 125

65 to show within- and between-network developmental VAN nodes will be more predominant in adults than in 126

66 changes including increases in functional and structural children; and (3) between- and within-network functional 127

67 connectivity with age (Uddin et al., 2011). While some connectivity patterns will show developmental changes. 128

68 investigators see the high degree of functional and anatom-

69 ical overlap between the VAN and salience network as

2. Materials and methods 129

70 evidence that they are part of the same system (Kucyi et al.,

71 2012), the majority have conceptualized these networks as

2.1. Participants and data acquisition 130

72 distinct entities (Cole et al., 2013; Power et al., 2011).

73 Resting state fMRI (R-fMRI) studies capitalize on the fact

The present study included a total of 60 healthy, 131

74 that large-scale neurocognitive networks can be reliably

right-handed neurotypical individuals from the pub- 132

75 identified in the absence of task-related processing (Biswal

lically available Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange 133

76 et al., 1995; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2009).

(ABIDE; http://fcon 1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/abide/) 134

77 Fox et al. (2006) were the first to use R-fMRI to examine

(Di Martino et al., 2014). We examined only data collected 135

78 attention networks in the human brain. They identified a

from the New York University Langone Medical Center 136

79 bilateral DAN and right lateralized VAN solely on the basis

(NYU) for consistency. All participants had a full-scale IQ 137

80 of seed-based functional connectivity analyses of resting

score of >80, and the groups did not differ significantly on 138

81 state fMRI data, thus providing evidence that these atten-

IQ. The group of 60 was split into two groups of children 139

82 tion networks are intrinsically coupled in the brain.

(mean age: 10.2, range 7–12, 11 females) and adults 140

83 The use of R-fMRI to address developmental questions

(mean age: 24.2, range 18–31, 6 females) consisting of 141

84 allows us to avoid many of the issues related to task

30 participants each (Table 1). Participants were also 142

85 performance that can confound interpretation of develop-

carefully selected based on motion parameters. No partic- 143

86 mental findings (Casey et al., 2005; Uddin

ipant was selected with absolute displacement > 1.95 mm. 144

87 et al., 2010). R-fMRI studies have demonstrated that over

The range of motion parameters for the child group was 145

88 the lifespan, the long-range connections within the DAN

0.09–1.95 mm mean absolute displacement (0.27 ± 0.33). 146

89 become more dominant until around the age of ∼30 years

The range of motion parameters for the adult group was 147

90 (Cao et al., 2014). Cao and colleagues found inverted U-

0.15–0.89 mm mean absolute displacement (0.30 ± 0.18). 148

91 shaped trajectories mainly within the DAN and language

No group differences in absolute displacement were 149

92 regions, which are argued to be amongst the last regions

observed (p = 0.7). The NYU institutional review board 150

93 to mature (Casey et al., 2000). This research is in line with

approved all procedures for data collection and shar- 151

94 previous studies which suggest that short-distance func-

ing. Written informed consent was obtained from each 152

95 tional connectivity is greater in children than it is in adults

participant. 153

96 (Dosenbach et al., 2010; Fair et al., 2009; Supekar et al.,

97 2009) and that the long-range connections observed in

98 adults are enhanced throughout development (Fair et al.,

Table 1

99 2009; Kelly et al., 2009; Supekar et al., 2009). It is argued

Participant characteristics.

100 that the process of synaptic “pruning” in which the abun-

Characteristic Children (n = 30) Adults (n = 30) (standard

101 dance of short-range connections in typically developing

(standard deviation) deviation) [range]

102 children are eliminated, generally in the pubertal stage of

[range]

103 development, contributes to the prominence of long-range

Age (years) 10.2 (1.74) 24.2 (3.34)

104 connectivity in adults (Huttenlocher, 1990).

[7.19–12.97] [18.59–31.78]

105 Though there is an abundance of literature surround-

Sex (No.)

106 ing human attention networks, there is surprisingly little

Male 19 24

107 research that focuses on the typical development of these Female 11 6

Full-Scale IQ 116.17 (13.89) 114.93 (10.09)

108 networks (Konrad et al., 2005). A previous independent

[80–142] [91–139]

109 component analysis (ICA) study examining developmental

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 3

154 Inclusion as a typically developing individual (TD) was and applied a band pass filter capturing the fMRI signal 211

155 based on the absence of any current Axis-I disorders as between the frequencies 0.01–0.08 Hz. The data were spa- 212

156 determined by the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders tially smoothed with a 5 mm full-width half-maximum 213

157 and -Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS- Gaussian kernel. To further reduce motion-related arti- 214

158 PL) administered to each child and his/her parent, and the facts we “scrubbed” our data using the cut (delete) option 215

159 Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders- available in the DPABI-A toolbox using the following 216

160 Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/NP) and Adult ADHD Clinical parameters: FD threshold for bad time points = 0.05, scrub- 217

161 Diagnostic Scale (ACDS) interviews for adults. bing time points before bad time points = 1, scrubbing time 218

162 All subjects were scanned using a 3T Allegra, in points after bad time points = 2 (Power et al., 2012). The 219

163 a separate visit following the diagnostic assessment data were registered to the subject’s individual anatomical 220

164 (typically within 3 months). The R-fMRI scans were col- space and then into MNI standard space. 221

165 lected using an echo-planner imaging (EPI) sequence

166 (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 15 ms; flip angle = 90 ; FOV = 240 mm; 2.4. Individual and group-level analyses for seed-based 222

167 voxel size = 3 mm × 3 mm × 4 mm; number of slices = 33, whole-brain functional connectivity analyses 223

168 4 mm slice thickness). The scan lasted for 6 min, which con-

169 sisted of 180 volumes. Participants were asked to relax with For each participant, we extracted the mean 224

170 their eyes open, while a white cross hair against a black time-series of each of the four attention network 225

171 background was projected on a screen. ROIs individually using the functional connectivity 226

172 A high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical image voxel-wise option in the REST toolbox version 1.8 227

173 was acquired using a magnetization prepared gradient (http://www.restfmri.net/forum/REST V1.8). We then 228

174 echo sequence (TR = 2530 ms; TE = 3.25 ms; inversion compared whole-brain functional connectivity patterns 229

175 time = 8.07 min; flip angle = 7 ; 128 slices; 1 volume between adults and children for each ROI. Data were cor- 230

176 FOV = 256 mm). All details regarding scanning protocols rected for multiple comparisons using Gaussian Random 231

177 are outlined in (Di Martino et al., 2014). Field Theory Multiple Comparison Correction (voxel-level 232

p-value = .01; cluster-level p-value = .05, two-tailed). 233

178 2.2. Region-of-interest (ROI) selection Two-tailed t-tests between results for the children and 234

adult groups were conducted for each ROI. These analyses 235

179 Four ROIs were selected based on previous research con- produced thresholded Z-score maps for each of the four 236

180 ducted by Fox and colleagues (Fox et al., 2006). In this ROIs. 237

181 earlier study, four ROIs (two in the DAN and two in the VAN)

182 were determined via meta-analyses. In the DAN, the ROIs 2.5. Seed-to-seed analyses within networks 238

183 were located in right intraparietal sulcus (IPS; 32, −56, 54)

184 and frontal eye fields (FEF; 28, −8, 52), and in the VAN the In addition to the seed-based whole-brain functional 239

185 ROIs were located in right temporoparietal junction (TPJ; connectivity analyses, which showed developmental dif- 240

186 60, −48, 22) and ventral frontal cortex (VFC; 42, 20, −6). ferences in functional connectivity of specific DAN and VAN 241

187 For the current study, spherical ROIs were created with a ROIs, we also conducted analyses between seed regions 242

188 radius of 6 mm based on these coordinates as reported by within the DAN and within the VAN. We used the same 243

189 Fox et al. (2006). Exploratory follow-up analyses related coordinates listed in Section 2.2 to represent the two seeds 244

190 to the salience network were conducted using a seed in within each network. Using the REST toolbox we calcu- 245

191 the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; 6, 24, 32) derived from lated pair-wise functional connectivity between the seeds 246

192 a previous study (Uddin et al., 2011). All coordinates are within the DAN (rIPS-rFEF) and within the VAN (rVFC-rTPJ). 247

193 reported in MNI standard space. Two-sample t-tests were conducted to explore group dif- 248

ferences in within-network functional connectivity in the 249

194 2.3. Data preprocessing two attention networks. 250

195 Data were preprocessed using the Data Process- 2.6. Seed-to-seed analyses between networks 251

196 ing Assistant for Resting-state fMRI Advanced Edition

197 (DPARSF-A) toolbox, which is part of the Data Processing To explore potential group differences in between- 252

198 and Analysis of Brain Imaging (DPABI) toolbox version 3.1, network functional connectivity, we conducted the fol- 253

199 (http://rfmri.org/dpabi) (Chao-Gan and Yu-Feng, 2010). lowing seed-to-seed correlation analyses of the following 254

200 The first 5 volumes were removed from each subject’s ROI pairs: rIPS-rVFC, rFEF-rTPJ, rIPS-rTPJ, rFEF-rVFC. Two- 255

201 resting-state fMRI data. Data were preprocessed in series of sample t-tests were conducted to explore group differences 256

202 steps including slice-timing correction, brain extraction of in between-network functional connectivity. 257

203 T1 images, and segmentation using SPM priors for cere-

204 brospinal fluid (CSF) and white matter (WM). We used 3. Results 258

205 the WM and cerebral CSF mean time-series as nuisance

206 regressors in the general linear model (GLM) to reduce the 3.1. DAN and VAN topological organization in children 259

207 influence of physiological noise (Margulies et al., 2007). and adults 260

208 Global signal regression was not used (Saad et al., 2012).

209 Additionally, we regressed out the nuisance covariates To visualize the organization of the DAN and VAN in chil- 261

210 using the Friston 24-parameter model (Friston et al., 1996) dren and adults, the whole-brain functional connectivity 262

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

4 K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

263 maps associated with each region of interest within each

264 network are depicted in Fig. 1. As can be seen in Fig. 1A,

265 the overlap of the functional connectivity map of the

266 IPS seed (blue) and the FEF seed (purple) delineates the

267 regions described in (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) and

268 (Fox et al., 2006) as belonging to the DAN. The areas of

269 overlap (violet) include bilateral IPS, bilateral FEF, and ven-

270 tral temporal visual areas, in both children and adults.

271 As can be seen in Fig. 2A, the overlap of the functional

272 connectivity maps of the TPJ (blue) and VFC (purple) delin-

273 eates the regions previously described as belonging to the

274 VAN. The areas of overlap (violet) include middle frontal

275 gyrus, anterior insula, VFC and TPJ, in both children and

276 adults.

277 3.2. Developmental differences in whole-brain functional

278 connectivity of dorsal attention network nodes

279 For both adults and children, IPS showed strong func-

280 tional connectivity with other regions of the DAN including

281 frontal eye fields and visual cortical regions. For both adults

282 and children, FEF showed strong functional connectivity

283 with the entire DAN including intraparietal and visual cor-

284 tical regions.

285 We found that for both ROIs representing the DAN (IPS

286 and FEF), children showed greater functional connectiv-

287 ity within the network (e.g. with the IPS) than did adults.

Fig. 1. Conjunction of correlation maps. (A) Voxels significantly correlated

288 Adults showed greater functional connectivity of the FEF

with the IPS (blue), FEF (purple), and both, defined as the dorsal attention

289 with regions outside of the DAN (e.g. posterior cingu-

network (DAN; purple). (B) Voxels significantly correlated with the TPJ Q7

290 late cortex, PCC) than did children (Fig. 2). Table 2 lists

(blue), VFC (purple), and both, defined as the ventral attention network

291 coordinates of significant group differences in functional (VAN; purple). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure

legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

292 connectivity for each ROI.

Fig. 2. Individual and group comparisons of resting state functional connectivity of seed ROIs IPS (A) and FEF (B). Yellow maps (first panel) show individual

one sample t-test for adults. Red maps (second panel) show individual one sample t-test for children. Remaining panels show two-sample t-tests comparing

adults with children. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 5

Table 2

Group differences in functional connectivity of DAN and VAN ROIs.

Seed Contrast Functional connectivity MNI coordinates Z-score Cluster size (voxels)

x y z

FEF Children > adults Left superior parietal lobule/superior LOC −30 −58 58 4.7216 611

Children > adults Right precentral gyrus 24 −10 44 5.4054 492

Adults > children Left posterior cingulate gyrus −6 −40 40 3.7597 338

Adults > children Left postcentral gyrus −40 −18 40 3.5311 332

− −

Adults > children Left posterior cingulate gyrus 2 20 40 3.1041 23

IPS Children > adults Right superior LOC 22 −74 58 3.9278 521

TPJ Adults > children Left posterior cingulate gyrus −4 −18 32 4.5237 295

VFC Children > adults Left thalamus 0 −24 12 4.8568 1736

Children > adults Left insular cortex −28 24 6 5.1591 814

Children > adults Right insular cortex 28 26 6 4.6307 551

Adults > children Right temporal pole 34 4 −38 3.7281 332

293 3.3. Developmental differences in whole-brain functional 3.4. Developmental differences in within- and 313

294 connectivity of ventral attention network nodes between-network ROI-to-ROI connections 314

295 For both adults and children, TPJ showed strong func- To examine whether between- and within-network 315

296 tional connectivity with other regions of the VAN including functional connectivity patterns show developmental 316

297 VFC and middle frontal gyrus. Strong functional connec- changes, we conducted ROI-to-ROI analyses. No group dif- 317

298 tivity was also observed between TPJ and regions of the ferences were found for within DAN (IPS-FEF) or within 318

299 (posterior cingulate and medial pre- VAN (TPJ-VFC) connections. In other words, children and 319

300 frontal cortex). For both adults and children, VFC showed adults did not differ on strength of functional connectiv- 320

301 strong functional connectivity with other regions of the ity within either of the attention networks. Likewise, the 321

302 VAN including middle frontal gyrus and TPJ (Fox et al., between-network ROI comparisons (rIPS-rVFC, rFEF-rTPJ, 322

303 2006). In addition, strong functional connectivity was rIPS-rTPJ, rFEF-rVFC) yielded no significant group differ- 323

304 observed between VFC and the anterior cingulate cortex ences (Fig. 4). 324

305 (ACC) and insular cortex.

306 We found that for both ROIs representing the VAN (TPJ

3.5. Relationship between VAN and salience network 325

307 and VFC), adults showed greater functional connectivity

308 with regions within the network than children. For the

On the basis of our finding that children showed greater 326

309 VFC ROI, children showed greater functional connectiv-

functional connectivity of VFC with regions of the salience 327

310 ity than adults with the anterior insula and ACC, regions

network compared with adults, we sought to further 328

311 comprising the salience network (Seeley et al., 2007)

explore this finding using ROI-to-ROI comparisons. Upon 329

312 (Fig. 3).

closer inspection, we noted that the seed from the Fox 2006 330

Fig. 3. Individual and group comparisons of resting state functional connectivity of seed ROIs TPJ (A) and VFC (B). Yellow maps (first panel) show individual

one sample t-test for adults. Red maps (second panel) show individual one sample t-test for children. Remaining panels show two-sample t-tests comparing

adults with children. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

6 K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

Fig. 4. ROI-to-ROI connectivity within dorsal and ventral attention networks nodes and between dorsal and ventral attention network nodes.

4. Discussion 347

Considerable evidence from behavioral, electrophysio- 348

logical, and neuroimaging work suggests two anatomically 349

separable fronto-parietal systems for attention exist in 350

the human brain, (1) a dorsal attention network (DAN) 351

involved in top-down orienting, and (2) a ventral atten- 352

tion network (VAN) for bottom-up salience detection 353

(Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). While these neural sys- 354

tems have been well characterized in neuroimaging studies 355

of adults (Corbetta et al., 2008), little is known about 356

the typical development of these brain networks. Here 357

we explored the functional connectivity of the dorsal and 358

ventral attention networks in adults and children using 359

seed-based functional connectivity to explore within- and 360

between-network developmental changes. The current 361

results provide unique insights into the typical devel- 362

opment of the DAN and VAN in the human brain. We 363

Fig. 5. ROI-to-ROI connectivity between VFC and VAN node (TPJ) and find asymmetries in functional connectivity patterns asso- 364

between VFC and salience network node (ACC).

ciated with key nodes of these attention networks. In 365

both children and adults, the attention networks could 366

331 paper corresponding to the VFC (42, 20, −6) falls within be identified as the overlap of functional connectivity 367

332 the anterior insular cortex, a key node of the salience net- maps associated with the IPS and FEF (DAN) and TPJ and 368

333 work. To test whether this VFC ROI more accurately reflects VFC (VAN). Thus, children and adults seem to exhibit 369

334 a VAN node or a salience network node, we computed func- DAN and VAN network topology that is anchored in 370

335 tional connectivity of the following: VFC-TPJ (within VAN) these key nodes and is quite similar in overall spatial 371

336 and VFC-ACC (within salience network). We reasoned that extent. For the DAN nodes (IPS and FEF), children showed 372

337 if VFC-ACC coupling was stronger than VFC-TPJ coupling, greater functional connectivity with regions within the 373

338 than the VFC node might actually be more representative network compared with adults, whereas adults showed 374

339 of the salience network than of the VAN. We found that nei- greater functional connectivity between the FEF and extra- 375

340 ther VFC-TPJ nor VFC-ACC comparisons showed significant network regions including the posterior cingulate cortex. 376

341 group differences. However, as can be seen in Fig. 5, the VFC For the VAN, the most striking developmental finding 377

342 ROI showed significantly stronger functional connectivity was that children showed greater functional connectivity 378

343 with ACC (a salience network node) than with TPJ (a VAN between VFC and nodes of the salience network (ante- 379

344 node) in both adults and children (p < 0.01). There was a rior insula and ACC), suggesting reduced segregation of the 380

345 trend suggesting greater connectivity of VFC with ACC in VAN and salience networks in childhood compared with 381

346 children compared with adults (p = 0.09). adulthood. 382

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 7

383 4.1. Developmental studies of attention greater within-DAN connectivity in children, though the 440

effect was not significant. 441

384 Evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging studies For the DAN, adults showed greater functional con- 442

385 suggests that specific attention processes undergo pro- nectivity of the FEF with regions outside the network 443

386 tracted periods of developmental maturation. In particular, including the PCC, a key hub of the default mode network 444

387 children are more susceptible to interference and less (Greicius et al., 2003; Raichle et al., 2001). This increased 445

388 able to inhibit responses than adults (Bunge et al., 2002). communication between the DAN and the default mode 446

389 Children show developmental differences such that they network observed in adults compared with children might 447

390 exhibit greater activation than adults in anterior cingulate enable greater top-down attentional capacities in adult- 448

391 cortex and lateral prefrontal cortices during selective atten- hood (Rubia, 2013). The ability to modulate or suppress 449

392 tion (Booth et al., 2003) and response inhibition (Casey default mode network activity has been linked with suc- 450

393 et al., 1997). This work is in line with the general develop- cessful goal-directed attention and cognition (Anticevic 451

394 mental principle that brain activation during a particular et al., 2012). The same direction of effects was observed 452

395 cognitive task tends to progress from more diffuse to for the VAN, where we observed greater functional connec- 453

396 more focal with age (Durston et al., 2006). Taken together, tivity of the TPJ with PCC and the VFC with temporal pole 454

397 the findings of decreased task-related frontal activation in adults compared with children. Overall, it appears that 455

398 with age are thought to index the maturation of cognitive adults exhibited more flexible patterns of extra-network 456

399 strategies from childhood through adolescence and into connections of attention network nodes compared with 457

400 adulthood (Dumontheil et al., 2010). The development of children. These results are largely consistent with previous 458

401 cognitive control processes has been the focus of the major- work demonstrating increasing long-range functional con- 459

402 ity of previous investigations (Durston and Casey, 2006). nectivity strength with development (Uddin et al., 2010). 460

403 Surprisingly little neuroimaging work has examined

404 developmental differences in the relative contributions of 4.3. Ventral attention network and salience network 461

405 the DAN and VAN. A previous study conducted by (Konrad

406 et al., 2005) used a task based fMRI paradigm to explore The VFC in the VAN showed greater functional connec- 462

407 the development of attention networks for alerting (fronto- tivity in children compared with adults, particularly with 463

408 parietal), orienting (TPJ and VFC), and executive attention ACC and anterior insula. The ROI selected to represent VFC 464

409 (anterior cingulate/lateral prefrontal) in adults and chil- was centered at MNI coordinates 42, 20, −6 based on previ- 465

410 dren. They found that children showed significantly less ous work (Fox et al., 2006). Upon closer examination, this 466

411 activation in the right mid-brain regions during alerting, region appears to fall within the anterior insular cortex, 467

412 in the right TPJ during reorienting, and in DLPFC during and is anatomically very close to a region located at 39, 468

413 executive control. At the same time, children exhibited sig- 23, −4 that has been implicated in playing a causal role in 469

414 nificantly more activation in superior frontal gyrus during switching between the central executive network and the 470

415 reorienting and superior temporal gyrus during executive default mode network in adults and children (Uddin et al., 471

416 control of attention. The data presented by Konrad et al. 2011). A large body of work indicates that the insula plays 472

417 (2005) suggest that there is a transition from the imma- a dominant role in detection of novel salient stimuli across 473

418 ture functional systems that support attentional functions multiple modalities (Crottaz-Herbette and Menon, 2006; 474

419 in children to more mature systems in adults. Downar et al., 2000). The role of the insula in detection of 475

salient stimuli and its inclusion in the “salience network” 476

along with the anterior cingulate cortex has now been well 477

420 4.2. Resting state fMRI and brain network development documented (Menon and Uddin, 2010; Seeley et al., 2007). 478

It is worth noting that there is still work to be done to dis- 479

421 Over the past ten years, resting state fMRI has emerged tinguish whether and to what extent the salience network 480

422 as a powerful complementary approach to examination of and the so-called “cingulo-opercular network” (Dosenbach 481

423 task-activation. A key principle that has emerged from this et al., 2008) should be conceptualized as distinct entities 482

424 literature is that anterior–posterior long-range functional (Uddin, 2015). To further explore the potential overlap of 483

425 connectivity is slow to develop, particularly in brain net- the VFC node with regions of the salience network, we 484

426 works underlying higher cognitive functions (Fair et al., conducted ROI-to-ROI analyses looking at VFC-TPJ and VFC- 485

427 2008; Kelly et al., 2009; Supekar et al., 2010). Human ACC connectivity to see which network (the VAN or the 486

428 brain network development appears to involve simulta- salience network) was most closely associated with this 487

429 neous segregation (decrease of short-range connections) ROI. We find that in both adults and children, the VFC shows 488

430 and integration (increase of long-range connections) (Fair stronger functional connections to the salience network 489

431 et al., 2007). A study using independent component analy- than to the VAN. We also observed a trend toward greater 490

432 sis (ICA) found evidence for increased connectivity within VFC-ACC connectivity in children compared with adults. 491

433 the DAN in children compared with adults (Jolles et al., We propose that the VAN and the salience network 492

434 2011), providing evidence for early segregation of the DAN. have overlapping nodes in the region surrounding the 493

435 In the current study we also find greater within-network VFC and anterior insula. We speculate that in children, 494

436 connectivity of the DAN in children, particularly in the IPS these two networks may be less segregated than in adults, 495

437 region, as revealed by group differences in whole-brain and that bottom-up salience processes and attention to 496

438 functional connectivity of key DAN nodes. The ROI-to- environmental stimuli may be over-represented in the 497

439 ROI connectivity (IPS-FEF) corroborated these findings of child’s brain. Consistent with the “circuit breaker” function 498

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

8 K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

499 attributed to the VAN, it has been demonstrated in adults functional connectivity patterns (Blakemore et al., 2010). 558

500 that signals in the VAN causally influence activity in the Given the as yet unknown effects of puberty on develop- 559

501 DAN (Sridharan et al., 2007). Perhaps the over-connectivity ment of attention networks in the brain, and the fact that 560

502 in the VAN in children compared with adults that we in the current sample participants’ pubertal status was not 561

503 observe is a neural signature of the differential and explicitly characterized, we chose to avoid examining the 562

504 asymmetric maturation of attention processes through- adolescent age range in the current study. We believe that 563

505 out development. The fact that children exhibit more careful consideration of pubertal status is a very important 564

506 widespread connectivity in the VFC region may explain question for future research (Uddin et al., 2013). 565

507 why they are behaviorally more susceptible to interrup- The second reason age was dichotomized was due to 566

508 tion by environmental stimuli and less able to maintain the sample available in the public dataset that was utilized 567

509 activities requiring top-down attentional control (Bunge in the study. As there were not a large enough number of 568

510 et al., 2002). An alternative speculation is that this VFC subjects to examine age continuously, we chose to sim- 569

511 region may need to be extensively connected in childhood plify by comparing children and adult groups, as several 570

512 to ensure detection of salient environmental stimuli, which previous developmental neuroimaging studies have done 571

513 would be important for basic survival. (Jolles et al., 2011; Kelly et al., 2009; Supekar et al., 2010; 572

514 Our results are somewhat consistent with the previ- Uddin et al., 2011). We believe that an important direc- 573

515 ous task activation study (Konrad et al., 2005). However, tion for future research will be to examine this question 574

516 we provide evidence to suggest that the second anatom- in larger datasets containing more individuals at various 575

517 ical component of the reorienting network, VFC, shows developmental stages in order to examine more complex 576

518 greater functional connectivity for children than in adults maturational patterns. 577

519 with regions of the salience network. We argue that the

520 reason for this region showing more widespread func-

5. Conclusion 578

521 tional connectivity in children than in adults is because

522 of its anatomical location. We show that the VFC, which

The current study demonstrates asymmetric develop- 579

523 is anatomically very close to the anterior insula cortex, is

mental patterns of functional connectivity of the dorsal and 580

524 significantly correlated with the ACC. The anterior insular

ventral attention networks of the human brain. Whereas 581

525 cortex and ACC has been described as the core that forms

for the DAN nodes (IPS and FEF), children show greater 582

526 the salience network, which is important for the facilita-

functional connectivity with regions within the network 583

527 tion and detection of environmental stimuli (Uddin, 2015).

compared with adults, and adults showed greater func- 584

528 The attention characteristics of the salience network are

tional connectivity between with extra-network regions 585

529 somewhat similar to those characteristics of the VAN; both

including nodes of the default mode network, for the VAN 586

530 are thought to be important for detection of stimuli outside

nodes (TPJ and VFC), adults showed greater functional 587

531 of direct focus. The anatomical proximity of anterior insula

connectivity with regions within the network compared 588

532 and VFC points toward a linkage between the two regions

with children and children showed greater functional 589

533 and possibly the shared responsibilities of salience detec-

connectivity between VFC and nodes of the salience net- 590

534 tion. We see that in children, these two salience detection

work. We suggest that because the VFC is within close 591

535 systems are less segregated than in adults. Future work will

proximity of anterior insula, an important hub within 592

536 explore the extent to which segregation between the VAN

the salience network, there may be overlap between the 593

537 and salience network throughout development relates to

VAN and salience network at this node. This asymmet- 594

538 increased attentional abilities using behavioral and task-

ric pattern of development of attention networks may be 595

539 based neuroimaging paradigms.

a neural signature of the shift from over-representation 596

of bottom-up attention mechanisms to greater top-down 597

540 4.4. Limitations

attentional capacities that emerge between childhood and Q4 598

adulthood. Future research should explore how alterations 599

541 The field has not yet come to a consensus as to best

in development of these attention networks might impact 600

542 practices for dealing with motion artifacts in resting-state

developmental disorders of attention including attention 601

543 fMRI data, though it is clear that such artifacts can bias

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Vaidya, 2012). 602

544 results (Power et al., 2012; Van Dijk et al., 2012). There

545 have not yet been validation studies to determine the exact

Conflict of interest 603

546 effects of the motion scrubbing procedure that has been put

547 forth as a potential solution. We believe that the effects

None declared. 604

548 of motion scrubbing on group differences functional con-

549 nectivity in developmental contexts will require additional

550 methodological study. Acknowledgments 605

551 A limitation of the current study is that age is

552 dichotomized, rather than continuously examined. We We gratefully acknowledge the Autism Brain Imag- 606

553 opted to examine the development of attention networks ing Data Exchange (ABIDE; http://fcon 1000.projects. 607

554 using two age categories in the current study for the fol- nitrc.org/indi/abide/) for providing the publicly available 608

555 lowing reasons. One was to avoid the period of puberty, data used in the current work. This work was supported 609

556 which occurs during adolescence, and is accompanied by by a National Institute of Mental Health Career Develop- Q5 610

557 protracted (and not very well-characterized) changes in ment Award (K01MH092288) and a Slifka/Ritvo Innovation 611

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 9

612 in Autism Research Award to from the International Society Durston, S., Davidson, M.C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, 684

J.A., et al., 2006. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with 685

613 for Autism Research to LQU.

development. Dev. Sci. 9 (1), 1–8. 686

Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Miezin, F.M., Barch, 687

D.M., et al., 2008. The maturing architecture of the brain’s default 688

614 Appendix A. Supplementary data

network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105 (10), 4028–4032. 689

Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Power, J.D., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Miezin, 690

615 Supplementary data associated with this article can be F.M., et al., 2009. Functional brain networks develop from a local to 691

distributed organization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5 (5), e1000381. 692

616 found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/

Fair, D.A., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Cohen, A.L., Brahmbhatt, S., Miezin, 693

617 j.dcn.2015.02.001.

F.M., et al., 2007. Development of distinct control networks through 694

segregation and integration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104 (33), 695 13507–13512. 696

618 References

Fox, M.D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Raichle, M.E., 2006. Spon- 697

taneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral 698

619 Anticevic, A., Cole, M.W., Murray, J.D., Corlett, P.R., Wang, X.J., Krystal, attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103 (26), 10046–10051. 699

620 J.H., 2012. The role of default network deactivation in cognition and Friston, K.J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R.S., Turner, R., 1996. 700

621 disease. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16 (12), 584–592. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35 701

622 Biswal, B., Yetkin, F.Z., Haughton, V.M., Hyde, J.S., 1995. Functional connec- (3), 346–355. 702

623 tivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., Menon, V., 2003. Functional con- 703

624 MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34 (4), 537–541. nectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode 704

625 Blakemore, S.J., Burnett, S., Dahl, R.E., 2010. The role of puberty in the hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100 (1), 253–258. 705

626 developing adolescent brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31 (6), 926–933. He, B.J., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Epstein, A., Shulman, G.L., Corbetta, 706

627 Booth, J.R., Burman, D.D., Meyer, J.R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B.L., Davenport, N.D., M., 2007. Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal net- 707

628 et al., 2003. Neural development of selective attention and response works underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53 (6), 708

629 inhibition. Neuroimage 20 (2), 737–751. 905–918. 709

630 Bressler, S.L., Tang, W., Sylvester, C.M., Shulman, G.L., Corbetta, M., 2008. Huttenlocher, P.R., 1990. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex 710

631 Top-down control of human by frontal and parietal development. Neuropsychologia 28 (6), 517–527. 711

632 cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. J. Neurosci. 28 (40), Jolles, D.D., van Buchem, M.A., Crone, E.A., Rombouts, S.A., 2011. A com- 712

633 10056–10061. prehensive study of whole-brain functional connectivity in children 713

634 Bunge, S.A., Dudukovic, N.M., Thomason, M.E., Vaidya, C.J., Gabrieli, J.D., and young adults. Cereb. Cortex 21 (2), 385–391. 714

635 2002. Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in chil- Kelly, A.M., Di Martino, A., Uddin, L.Q., Shehzad, Z., Gee, D.G., Reiss, P.T., 715

636 dren: evidence from fMRI. Neuron 33 (2), 301–311. et al., 2009. Development of anterior cingulate functional connec- 716

637 Cao, M., Wang, J.H., Dai, Z.J., Cao, X.Y., Jiang, L.L., Fan, F.M., et al., 2014. tivity from late childhood to early adulthood. Cereb. Cortex 19 (3), 717

638 Topological organization of the human brain functional 640–657. 718

639 across the lifespan. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 76–93. Konrad, K., Neufang, S., Thiel, C.M., Specht, K., Hanisch, C., Fan, J., et al., 719

640 Casey, B.J., Galvan, A., Hare, T.A., 2005. Changes in cerebral functional orga- 2005. Development of attentional networks: an fMRI study with chil- 720

641 nization during cognitive development. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15 (2), dren and adults. Neuroimage 28 (2), 429–439. 721

642 239–244. Kucyi, A., Hodaie, M., Davis, K.D., 2012. Lateralization in intrinsic func- 722

643 Casey, B.J., Giedd, J.N., Thomas, K.M., 2000. Structural and functional brain tional connectivity of the temporoparietal junction with salience- 723

644 development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. and attention-related brain networks. J. Neurophysiol. 108 (12), 724

645 54 (1–3), 241–257. 3382–3392. 725

646 Casey, B.J., Trainor, R.J., Orendi, J.L., Schubert, A.B., Nystrom, L.E., Giedd, Margulies, D.S., Kelly, A.M., Uddin, L.Q., Biswal, B.B., Castellanos, F.X., 726

647 J.N., et al., 1997. A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal Milham, M.P., 2007. Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior 727

648 activation during performance of a go-no-go task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9 cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 37 (2), 579–588. 728

649 (6), 835–847. Menon, V., Uddin, L.Q., 2010. Saliency, switching, attention and control: 729

650 Chao-Gan, Y., Yu-Feng, Z., 2010. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for pipeline a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214 (5–6), 730

651 data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, 13. 655–667. 731

652 Cole, M.W., Reynolds, J.R., Power, J.D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., Braver, T.S., Power, J.D., Barnes, K.A., Snyder, A.Z., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, S.E., 2012. 732

653Q6 2013. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI 733

654 control. Nat. Neurosci. networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59 (3), 2142–2154. 734

655 Corbetta, M., Patel, G., Shulman, G.L., 2008. The reorienting system of the Power, J.D., Cohen, A.L., Nelson, S.M., Wig, G.S., Barnes, K.A., Church, J.A., 735

656 human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58 (3), et al., 2011. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neu- 736

657 306–324. ron 72 (4), 665–678. 737

658 Corbetta, M., Shulman, G.L., 2002. Control of goal-directed and stimulus- Raichle, M.E., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D.A., 738

659 driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 (3), 201–215. Shulman, G.L., 2001. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. 739

660 Crottaz-Herbette, S., Menon, V., 2006. Where and when the anterior cingu- Sci. U. S. A. 98 (2), 676–682. 740

661 late cortex modulates attentional response: combined fMRI and ERP Rubia, K., 2013. Functional brain imaging across development. Eur. Child 741

662 evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18 (5), 766–780. Adolesc. Psychiatry 22 (12), 719–731. 742

663 Damoiseaux, J.S., Rombouts, S.A., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C.J., Smith, Saad, Z.S., Gotts, S.J., Murphy, K., Chen, G., Jo, H.J., Martin, A., et al., 743

664 S.M., et al., 2006. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy 2012. Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences 744

665 subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103 (37), 13848–13853. become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect. 2 (1), 745

666 Di Martino, A., Yan, C.G., Li, Q., Denio, E., Castellanos, F.X., Alaerts, K., et al., 25–32. 746

667 2014. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale Seeley, W.W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A.F., Keller, J., Glover, G.H., Kenna, H., 747

668 evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol. Psychiatry et al., 2007. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience 748

669 19 (6), 659–667. processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27 (9), 2349–2356. 749

670 Dosenbach, N.U., Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, S.E., 2008. Shulman, G.L., d’Avossa, G., Tansy, A.P., Corbetta, M., 2002. Two attentional 750

671 A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. processes in the parietal lobe. Cereb. Cortex 12 (11), 1124–1131. 751

672 12 (3), 99–105. Smith, S.M., Fox, P.T., Miller, K.L., Glahn, D.C., Fox, P.M., Mackay, C.E., et al., 752

673 Dosenbach, N.U., Nardos, B., Cohen, A.L., Fair, D.A., Power, J.D., Church, J.A., 2009. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during 753

674 et al., 2010. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106 (31), 13040–13045. 754

675 329 (5997), 1358–1361. Sridharan, D., Levitin, D.J., Chafe, C.H., Berger, J., Menon, V., 2007. Neural 755

676 Downar, J., Crawley, A.P., Mikulis, D.J., Davis, K.D., 2000. A multimodal cor- dynamics of event segmentation in music: converging evidence for 756

677 tical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. dissociable ventral and dorsal networks. Neuron 55 (3), 521–532. 757

678 Nat. Neurosci. 3 (3), 277–283. Supekar, K., Musen, M., Menon, V., 2009. Development of large-scale func- 758

679 Dumontheil, I., Hassan, B., Gilbert, S.J., Blakemore, S.J., 2010. Develop- tional brain networks in children. PLoS Biol. 7 (7), e1000157. 759

680 ment of the selection and manipulation of self-generated thoughts Supekar, K., Uddin, L.Q., Prater, K., Amin, H., Greicius, M.D., Menon, V., 760

681 in adolescence. J. Neurosci. 30 (22), 7664–7671. 2010. Development of functional and structural connectivity within 761

682 Durston, S., Casey, B.J., 2006. What have we learned about cognitive devel- the default mode network in young children. Neuroimage 52 (1), 762

683 opment from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia 44 (11), 2149–2157. 290–301. 763

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001

G Model

DCN 266 1–10 ARTICLE IN PRESS

10 K. Farrant, L.Q. Uddin / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

764 Uddin, L.Q., 2015. Salience processing and insular cortical function and Uddin, L.Q., Supekar, K.S., Ryali, S., Menon, V., 2011. Dynamic recon- 773

765 dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16 (1), 55–61. figuration of structural and functional connectivity across core 774

766 Uddin, L.Q., Supekar, K., Menon, V., 2010. Typical and atyp- neurocognitive brain networks with development. J. Neurosci. 31 (50), 775

767 ical development of functional human brain networks: 18578–18589. 776

768 insights from resting-state FMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, Vaidya, C.J., 2012. Neurodevelopmental abnormalities in ADHD. Curr. Top. 777

769 21. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 49–66. 778

770 Uddin, L.Q., Supekar, K., Menon, V., 2013. Reconceptualizing functional Van Dijk, K.R., Sabuncu, M.R., Buckner, R.L., 2012. The influence of head 779

771 brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Front. motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59 (1), 780

772 Hum. Neurosci. 7, 458. 431–438. 781

Please cite this article in press as: Farrant, K., Uddin, L.Q., Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention

networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001