Learning and Memory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Learning and Memory Chapter 19 Learning and Memory et me begin by telling a little story. because we practice–repeat something over When I was a graduate student we had and over. A further definition says, Lto take an exam that Cornell does in “[Learning is] either a case of differential an interesting way. They put you in a swivel- strengthening of one from a number of chair surrounded by your committee responses evoked by a situation of need, or composed of 4-5 faculty members. You are the formation of receptor-effector spun around, and a question comes from the connections de novo; the first occurs direction you face when you stop. One of the typically in simple selective learning and the questions I was asked was, “What will be second in conditioned reflex learning" (Hull, the most important accomplishment in the 1943). It is the strengthening of existing field of neuroscience in the next 10 years?” responses and the formation of new Knowing that knowledge usually advances responses to existing stimuli that make this in small steps, I said that we would definition unique. So what is learning? It gradually know more about most aspects of isn’t clear that we have an inclusive the field, but I didn’t expect any major definition. It appears that learning is the breakthroughs. To a man, the committee strengthening of existing responses or agreed that in 10 years we would have a formation of new responses to existing complete solution to the problem of learning stimuli that occurs because of practice or and memory. The word complete was their repetition. How much practice? Sometimes a term, presumably meaning that we would single practice session is sufficient as in know everything there was to know about avoidance of painful or noxious stimuli. learning and memory. Well, at the 10-year Sometimes a lot of practice is necessary as mark, I sent them all a letter saying, “I told in learning to drive a car. you so!” Their predicted event didn’t What then is memory? Again happen. However, we have made some according to Kandel (2000), ". memory is progress; that progress is what this chapter is the process by which that knowledge of the about. world is encoded, stored, and later retrieved." By this definition, memory is not Definitions a thing; it’s a process. Interesting! In another What is learning? According to Eric definition, "Memory is a phase of learning . Kandel (2000) “Learning is the process by . learning has three stages: 1. acquiring, which we acquire knowledge about the wherein one masters a new activity . or world.” While this definition is erudite, it memorizes verbal material . 2. retaining doesn’t help us much in knowing what to the new acquisition for a period of time; and study. Another definition (Kimble, 1961), 3. remembering, which enables one to "Learning refers to a more or less permanent reproduce the learned act or memorized change in behavior which occurs as a result material. In a narrower sense learning of practice," is a little better. It tells us that merely means acquiring skill . ." (Sargent learning is more or less permanent; it won’t & Stafford, 1965). From these definitions, always be there, but often will. It also tells we see that memory has to do with keeping us that this is something that happens “knowledge” someplace and then retrieving it when it is needed. What we don’t see here when given only the first few letters of the is that the “knowledge” doesn’t have to word. At the same time, they deny ever come into consciousness. I have two having learned the word previously. Implicit cars–one with an automatic transmission, memory is often further parceled as one with a stick shift. I don’t have to bring associative and non-associative. There are into consciousness the process for shifting two well-known types of non-associative gears when I get into the car that requires me learning: habituation and sensitization. to do that–I just do it! Habituation is a decrease in response to a benign stimulus when the stimulus is Types of Memory presented repeatedly. A dog will be aroused There are actually two basic kinds of when a strange tone is played. If the tone is learning and memory. One is declarative or played over and over, the dog will explicit; the other is non-declarative or eventually no longer be aroused by the tone. implicit. Knowledge of facts–what we know We say that it has habituated. This kind of about places, things and people–and the learning makes sense; it is not efficient for meaning of these facts is explicit memory. an organism to go on responding to a These things must be recalled into stimulus that has no meaning. The other consciousness to be used. Patients who have form of non-associative learning, bilateral medial temporal lobe lesions have sensitization, is an enhanced response to an inability to learn and remember items of many different stimuli after experiencing an factual knowledge. They can’t remember intense or noxious one. For example, an people that they met the day before. They animal responds more vigorously to a tone can’t remember what they did the day of lesser intensity once a painfully loud tone before. Some people will further parcel has been played. Here we say that the animal explicit memories as episodic (we remember is sensitized. events) or semantic (we remember facts). As These two forms of learning also Kandel (2000) points out, in either case the interact. Once a response has been content of all explicit memories can be habituated, it can be restored by expressed by declarative statements such as sensitization. In this case, we say that the “I was here yesterday” (episodic) and “The animal is dishabituated. As an example: a hippocampus has something to do with habituated startle response to a noise can be memory” (semantic). restored by strongly pinching the skin. Implicit memory involves In non-associative learning, it is not information about how to perform necessary that the animal learns to associate something; it’s recalled unconsciously. We the stimuli involved (thus the name). For use implicit memory in trained, reflexive example, the dishabituated animal does not motor or perceptual skills. I know how to learn to associate the noise with the pinch. drive my car; I know how to get to work. As we shall see shortly, this is the hallmark The same people with bilateral medial of associative learning. Not all forms of non- temporal lobe lesions can learn simple associative learning are as simple as reflexive skills–they habituate and are habituation and sensitization. For example, sensitized, they can be classically and we learn language by imitation of people operantly conditioned (see later). They can who already speak. This involves no learn certain perceptual tasks. For example, association of stimuli and is clearly more they can recall a word learned previously complicated than habituation. In associative learning, we “learn” conditioned stimulus (CS). The response to that two stimuli are associated with each it (again salivation) is called the conditioned other or that a response is associated with a response (CR). The UR and the CR are given event or has a given consequence. usually similar but often not identical in type Perhaps important in clinical considerations, or strength. a person can also learn that an outcome is Initially investigators thought that not associated with a response. So a person classical conditioning involved simply may learn that what happens to him is not learning that two stimuli were related to what he does. Two sorts of contiguous–that they occurred close together associative learning have been well studied: in time, one after the other. Now we think classical conditioning and operant that what the animals learn is conditioning. Classical conditioning is well contingencies–that existence of something demonstrated by Pavlov’s famous depends upon existence of something else. experiment in which he presented meat With this in mind, it is possible to see that powder to a dog, causing it to salivate. He simply learning that two stimuli were repeated the presentation, and each time the contiguous could often lead to behaviors that dog salivated. If he repeatedly rang a bell were maladaptive, with animals associating just before presenting the meat powder (they environmental events that had no real were paired), the animal came to associate relationship. On the other hand, the the bell with the presentation of the meat existence of superstitious behaviors, even in powder, and it would begin to salivate when humans, suggests that this does occur. the bell was rung. In fact, for a while it It is tempting to think of extinction would salivate if the bell was rung but no as an example of forgetting, but alas it is not. meat powder was presented (they were The difference is that something new is unpaired). After a while, the bell stopped learned during the process of extinction–the predicting the presentation of meat powder animal learns that the CS is no longer a for the dog, and it ceased salivating when it signal that the US is about to occur, rather it was rung. (This process is called extinction.) is a signal that the US will not occur. It should be noted that for classical In operant conditioning (sometimes conditioning to occur the ringing of the bell called trial-and-error learning), a person or must precede the presentation of the meat animal learns that it gets a reward if it does powder, often by a certain critical interval of something.
Recommended publications
  • Cognition and Performance: the Relation to Neural Mechanisms of Consequence, Confidence, and Competence
    2 COGNITION AND PERFORMANCE: THE RELATION TO NEURAL MECHANISMS OF CONSEQUENCE, CONFIDENCE, AND COMPETENCE Karl H. Pribram Stanford University. California INTRODUCTION In 1968 at a UNESCO meeting on brain and human behavior, Skinner stated that he was interested in the environmental contingencies that produce reinforce­ ment. I remarked that if this were so, then reinforcement must be conceived as a process whose locus could be nowhere else than the brain. In contributing this chapter, then, I take the opportunity in examining some of the issues that have made the concept of reinforcement, from a structural point of view, so central in behavior theory. Skinner (1976) has clearly opted for a descriptive functionalism in his search for the laws governing behavior. Those of us contributing to this volume are, for the most part, searching for process and mechanism. This ap­ proach shows kinship with cognitive psychology, and one of the staples of cog- . nitive psychology is the distinction between surface and deep structure, between performance and competence. Thus one of the aims of this chapter will be to bridge the current chasm between functional behavior theory and the structures of cognitive psychology. These "structures" are conceived as patterns describing processes (see, e.g., Saussure, 1922; Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960; Merleau­ Ponty, 1963). Thus, the keystone to the bridge between behavioral performances (processes of organism-environment interaction) and structural competence (neural processes) may well be furnished by the concept of reinforcement, a concept that was originally invoked to explain learning, i.e., sustained changes in behavior. Biology of Reinforcement Copyright © 1980 Academic Press, Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • 3 Learning and Conservation Behavior: an Introduction and Overview
    3 Learning and conservation behavior: an introduction and overview ZACHARY SCHAKNER AND DANIEL T. BLUMSTEIN 3.1 CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND Learning is a key aspect of behavior that may greatly enhance the survival and fecundity of animals, especially in a changing environ- ment. Wildlife conservation problems often involve increasing the population of threatened or endangered species, decreasing the popu- lation of species deemed over abundant or encouraging animals to move to or from certain areas. Learning is an example of reversible plasticity (for review see Dukas 2009), which typically remains open to change throughout life. Old associations can be replaced, relearned and reinstated, facilitating behavioral modifications across an individual’s lifetime. Because learning is potentially demographically important, and because it can be used to modify individual’s behavior, it may therefore be an important tool for conservation behaviorists (Blumstein &Ferna´ndez-Juricic 2010). Our aim in this chapter is to introduce the fundamentals of learning that will later be developed and applied in subsequent chapters. Animal learning theory defines learning as experience that elicits a change in behavior (Rescorla 1988, Heyes 1994). There are three basic mechanisms, or types of experiences, that underlie animal learning. The simplest learning process is non-associative because it involves an indivi- dual’s experience with a single stimulus. During this process, exposure to thesinglestimulusresultsinachangeinthemagnitudeofresponseupon subsequent exposures to that stimulus. If the response increases, the processiscalledsensitization; if the response decreases, the process is called habituation. More complex associative learning mechanisms involve a change in behavior as a result of experience with two stimuli through Pavlovian conditioning (also referred to as classical conditioning), Conservation Behavior: Applying Behavioral Ecology to Wildlife Conservation and Management,eds.O.Berger- Tal and D.
    [Show full text]
  • Units of Which Behavior Is Composed. Knowl
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1986, 469 219-242 NUMBER 2 (SEPrEMBER) UNITS OF ANALYSIS AND KINETIC STRUCTURE OF BEHA VIORAL REPERTOIRES TRAVIS THOMPSON AND DAVID LUBINSKI UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA It is suggested that molar streams of behavior are constructed of various arrangements of three elementary constituents (elicited, evoked, and emitted response classes). An eight-cell taxonomy is elaborated as a framework for analyzing and synthesizing complex behavioral repertoires based on these functional units. It is proposed that the local force binding functional units into a smoothly articulated kinetic sequence arises from temporally arranged relative response probability relation- ships. Behavioral integration is thought to reflect the joint influence of the organism's hierarchy of relative response probabilities, fluctuating biological states, and the arrangement of environmental and behavioral events in time. Key words: behavioral units, units of analysis, response class, organization of behavior, behavioral taxonomy, relative response probability PROTASIS edge of such units is not axiomatic but as in An impala gamboling across an East Afri- the case of our knowledge of other biological can Serengeti plain or a 16-month-old child units, must be arrived at empirically. Whether confronted with a novel toy presents seamless fundamental behavior units can be discovered streams of kinetic motion, free of beginnings depends largely on the criteria used for as- or endings. The scant frequency with which sessing and selecting behavior classes. Given behavioral scientists have concerned them- criteria, establishing which units satisfy those selves with identifying the fundamental units criteria is an empirical matter. Approximately of behavioral phenomena is, in part, due to three quarters of a century have been devoted the conspicuous integrity of such naturally oc- to research dealing with a range of behavioral curring episodes; indeed, at times they seem units without explicit criteria for assessing to defy scientific analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • The Essence of Scientific and Engineering Thinking and Tools to Promote It
    Paper ID #18172 The Essence of Scientific and Engineering Thinking and Tools to Promote It Prof. Osman Yasar, State University of New York, Brockport Osman Yasar is an endowed professor and director of the CMST Institute at The College at Brockport, SUNY. He established the first undergraduate degree program in computational science in the United States and developed a computational pedagogical content knowledge (CPACK) framework for teacher education. His research interests include engineering and science education, computational pedagogy, computational and scientific thinking as well as fluid dynamics, engine ignition modeling, and parallel computing. Yasar has a PhD in engineering physics and two MSs in computer science and nuclear engi- neering from the University of Wisconsin–Madison. He graduated with a BS in engineering physics and a MS in theoretical physics from Hacettepe University in Turkey. Contact him at [email protected]. Dr. Jose Maliekal, State University of New York, Brockport Jose Maliekal is the Dean of the School of Arts and Sciences at The College at Brockport, State University of New York. He is an atmospheric scientist and have been teaching meteorology and climatology courses. His research interests include climate change and computational pedagogy. Address: School of Arts and Sciences, The College at Brockport, Brockport, NY 14420 Phone: 585-395- 5598 Email: [email protected] Dr. Peter Veronesi, State University of New York, Brockport Peter Veronesi is Associate Professor and Program Coordinator for the Adolescence Science Education Programs at The College at Brockport. Dr. Leigh J. Little, State University of New York, Brockport Leigh Little received his doctorate in Mathematics from Arizona State University.
    [Show full text]
  • International Journal of Comparative Psychology
    eScholarship International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Pavlov + Skinner = Premack Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1v20v1dg Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 27(4) ISSN 0889-3675 Author Killeen, Peter R Publication Date 2014 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California 2014, 27(4), 544-568 Stanley J. Weiss, Editor Special Issue Peer-reviewed Pavlov + Skinner = Premack Peter R. Killeen Arizona State University Behavior is a sequence of actions. Premackian conditioning occurs when one of those actions permits an animal to engage in more biologically potent positive responses—reinforcement—or forces them to engage in less positive (or negative) responses — punishment. Signals of the transition from one class of actions to another focus the instrumental responses in the first class and inform the contingent responses in the second class. The signals may be innate (USs) or learned (sign-learning); excitatory (leading to more positive actions) or inhibitory (leading to less positive actions). The potency of an action has phylogenetic origins, but may be conditioned by proximity to more potent responses, such as consummation of a reinforcer. With practice instrumental responses may take on increased strength, and in some cases become motivationally autonomous—become habits. Stimuli or responses that signal the availability of more positive actions may become incentive motivators that animals will approach. Discriminative stimuli do not have intrinsic value as reinforcers, but only the value derived from the responses that they release. These forces bend an animal’s trajectory through its stimulus-action-time context into a path that leads more directly to positive actions.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavior Analysis and Learning, Third Edition
    Third Edition BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS AND LEARNING W. David Pierce • Carl D. Cheney Behavior Analysis and Learning Behavior Analysis and Learning Third Edition W. David Pierce University of Alberta Carl D. Cheney Utah State University LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOCIATES, PUBLISHERS 2004 Mahwah, New Jersey London Editor: Bill Webber Editorial Assistant: Kristin Duch Cover Design: Kathryn Houghtaling Lacey Textbook Production Manager: Paul Smolenski Full-Service Compositor: TechBooks Text and Cover Printer: Hamilton Printing Company This book was typeset in 10.25/12pt. Goudy. The heads were typeset in Optima and Goudy. Copyright C 2004 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by photostat, microfilm, retrieval system, or any other means, without prior written permission of the publisher. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers 10 Industrial Avenue Mahwah, New Jersey 07430 www.erlbaum.com Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Pierce, W. David. Behavior analysis and learning / W. David Pierce & Carl D. Cheney.—3rd ed. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-8058-4489-9 (alk. paper) 1. Behaviorism (Psychology) 2. Learning, Psychology of. I. Cheney, Carl D. II. Title. BF199.P54 2004 150.19434—dc21 2003001719 Books published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates are printed on acid-free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and durability. Printed in the United States of America 10987654321 To B. F. Skinner When an organism acts upon the environment in which it lives, it changes that environment in ways that often affect the organism itself. Some of these changes are what the layman calls rewards, or what are generally referred to technically as reinforcers: when they follow behavior in this way they increase the likelihood that the organism will behave in the same way again (Ferster & Skinner, 1957, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Predicting Recovery After a Stroke
    Neuroplasticity and Motor Learning James Lynskey, PT, PhD Associate Professor Department of Physical Therapy A.T. Still University October 13, 2018 Neuroplasticity • Definition: – Ability of neurons to change function, chemical profile or structure – A natural innate process within the nervous system underlying learning and adaptation – An essential function of the nervous system to recover from damage – Occurs throughout the lifespan – Not inherently “good” – Can be adaptive or maladaptive Neuroplasticity • Is the change that happens in the brain that results in changes in function (memory, movement, reaction time, skill improvement, skill reduction….) • The idea of neuroplasticity was first proposed by William James in 1890 – “Organic matter, especially nervous tissue, seems endowed with a very extraordinary degree of plasticity...” The Principles of Psychology Neuroplasticity • Another important theory on neuroplasticity was proposed by Donald Hebb in 1949 – “When an axon of cell A is near enough to excite cell B and repeatedly or persistently takes part in firing it, some growth process or metabolic change takes place in one or both cells such that A's efficiency, as one of the cells firing B, is increased.” The Organization of Behavior (1949) • Cells that fire together, wire together Synaptic Plasticity • Plasticity at the synapse take the form of: – Alterations in amount of neurotransmitters – Alterations in number, density and type of receptors – Alterations in membrane excitability • Taken together these plastic changes will increase or decrease the efficacy of current synapses – Think long term potentiation/depression – Sensitization and Habituation Structural Plasticity • Structural Plasticity can take the form of: – Axonal sprouting, regeneration, or atrophy – Dendritic sprouting or atrophy – Dendritic spine alterations – Alterations in number of neurons Wang et.
    [Show full text]
  • From Neo-Behaviorism to Social Constructivism?: the Paradigmatic Non-Evolution of Albert Bandura
    FROM NEO-BEHAVIORISM TO SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIVISM?: THE PARADIGMATIC NON-EVOLUTION OF ALBERT BANDURA By Scott D. Simon Adviser: Frank Pajares A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Emory College of Emory University in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Bachelor of Arts with Honors Division of Educational Studies 1999 Revised August 2001 Table of Contents LIST OF FIGURES. iv ABSTRACT. .v INTRODUCTION. 1 Statement of the Problem. 2 Bandura in Textbooks . 5 Bandura in Books of Theory . 6 Bandura on the World Wide Web . 7 Current Developments . .. ..9 Purpose of the Study . .. 10 Significance of the Study. 10 CONSTRUCTIVISM'S PHILOSOPHICAL ROOTS: A HISTORICAL ANALYSIS. 11 The Foundations of Constructivism. 11 Modern Constructivism . .14 PRINCIPLES OF CONSTRUCTIVIST THOUGHT . .19 The Active Construction of Meaning . 19 Social Influences on Construction. 20 Importance of Self-Regulatory Practices. 22 The Role of Mental Operations . .23 Constructivism, Truth, and Experience . .23 Summary . 25 OVERVIEW OF SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY. .26 SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY AS SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIVISM . 32 The Active Construction of Meaning. .32 Construction is a Process of Equilibration. 33 People Choose their Environments. 35 People are Active Agents, Not Passive Recipients . 36 Active Construction Occurs in Diverse Settings. 38 ii Socially Situated Symbol Use . .39 Symbols are Constructed Internally. 39 Symbol Creation Requires an Interaction with Society. 40 The Use of Symbols is Essential to Development. 42 Observational Learning is Dependent on the Use of Symbols. .43 Self-Regulation . 44 Self-regulation Occurs in Diverse Settings . 45 Personal Agency Characterizes Meaning Construction . 46 Formalized Operations . .. 48 People Anticipate Events; They do not Simply React to Stimuli .
    [Show full text]