<<

ൢ֙ـं

(ᖪ 26: 11-26 (2006ٿέ៉ Formosan Entomol. 26: 11-26 (2006)

α৳֑෪̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆׎யӉઐр̝үϡ

!ཱི 38 ޒ ᖪጯࡁտٙ! έΔξᘲ೻჊ྮα߱ 224ٿᑛԂฝ! ໅ࡶࢁ! ߸୿૽+! ઼ϲέ៉̂ጯ

ၡ!!ࢋ

ΰ ΰ 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐 緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐

ᜰᗤຠȈ緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐緐。

-ࠉ!!ِ Hopkins’ host-selection principle ă neo Hopkins principle ᄃ Chemical legacy ࢴ׌჌ٕ׌჌פזॡĂ఼ hypothesisĄHopkins ៍၅ۏࢴٕயӉ۞૙͹ങפᖪдᏴፄٿ ࢴι̏གྷዋᑕפ჌Ăົઐр޺ᜈۏ჌ᙷ ͽ˯૙͹۞ۏઐр׎ρᖪഇഅགྷགྷរ࿅۞ങົ૱ Hopkins, 1917; Schoonhoven et al., 1998)Ă ۞૙͹ (Hopkins, 1917; Barron, 2001)Ą҃) ˠ૟̝ྋᛖࠎјᖪ૙͹Ᏼፄ۞ઐрົ۞ֽޢ ׽ؠ۞૙͹ઐрܧ֭˯ࠎ˞ྋᛖ఺ֱд᏷็ ੃ጸ۞ᇆᜩĂ˵ಶߏρᖪഇٙۏρᖪ၆ࢴזצ ன෪Ăѣˬ჌நኢజ೩΍ֽĂ̶Ҿࠎ۞

*ኢ͛ᓑᘭˠ e-mail: [email protected] ̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 11 ᇽৠགྷր௚Ăͷົ ࠎ۞јᖪགྷរ఼૱ѣ˘ᙯᔣഇ (critical̚ٺࢴ੃ጸົхдפԛј۞ ߱ॡ̈˘۞˳̙ޢࢴٕ period)Ă఼૱ߏдјᖪҀ̼פјᖪഇĂЯ҃ᇆᜩјᖪזགྷ࿅តၗ঻х ;யӉ۞ઐр (Thrope, 1930; Dethier, 1954; ม̰ (Jaisson, 1980; Hoffmann, 1988 Manning, 1967; Phillips, 1977; Corbet, Kester and Barbosa, 1991; Bjorksten and 1985; Szentesi and Jermy, 1990; Rietdorf Hoffmann, 1998; Breed et al., 1998)Ąѩӈ and Steidle, 2002)Ăߏࠎ Hopkins’ host- neo-Hopkins principle (Jaenike, 1983)Ă΁ ̈˘۞ޢଂུҀ̼ٺᗕਂ ࣇ͹ૺ૙͹ઐрΪՙؠٺຐĄ҃၆ޥ͕̚۞ selection principle ۞זρᖪ۞੃ጸ۞ቁਕૉ ߱јᖪ݈གྷរĂ˵ಶߏјᖪҀ̼ॡٙତᛈ˞ځϫᙀᙷ۞ࡁտ˵ᙋ ᒖဩ̖ߏᇆᜩ૙͹ઐр۞ᙯᔣ (Smith and ڍјᖪॡഇĂTully et al. (1994) ೩ֻזᖼொ ᙀρ Cornell, 1979; Jaenike, 1988; Caubet andڍᐌӈග̟࿪ᑝĂਕֹޢקᙀρᖪߙ჌ঈ ;Ă֭ͷѩன Jaisson, 1991; van Emden et al., 1996קᔖฟᄃѩ˘࿪ᑝᙯా۞ঈזᖪጯ ,ćRay (1999) Barron and Corbet, 1999; Rojas and Wyattזᔘਕ៍၅͇ 8 ޢᙀҀ̼ڍٺ෪ Ą(1999 ͢۞ק૟छᙀϐ᛬ρᖪ࿶ዳдӣѣ̙Тঈ ۞ھρᖪӈ ௐˬ࣎நኢ݋ߏΒӣቑಛྵࠎᇃٺயϠઐрĂ҃קढĂਕֹјᖪ၆ѩঈ Chemical legacy hypothesis (Corbet, 1985)Ă ז૟ུ̼ॡĂ૟੊ቚ࿅۞ρᖪ۞ৠགྷ௡ᖐொങ ࢬ׌࣎நኢ݈ٺϏ੊ቚ࿅۞ρᖪĂ˵ਕֹϏ੊ቚ࿅۞छᙀјᖪ ఺࣎நኢٙ೩΍۞ྋᛖ̙Т ᖪវ̰ٕវܑ۞຋̼̈ጯٿٺᙀ۞ࡁտ˵ ۞ߏĂధкхдڍઐр۞ͅᑕćΩγ၆קயϠ၆ঈ ࢴٕפᏴፄᙷҬᒖဩֽޢᖪᐌٿኳĂົᆧΐۏ វᄃᛈ֎ཧ̝มېᎽٺពϯӈֹགྷ࿅តၗĂҜ ॡഇĂܜЇңјٺ๥ᛇᓑඛৠགྷ̪൒঻хĂ҃఺˵ߏ̂ཝ̚ࢦ யӉ۞ΞਕّĂ఺Ξਕ൴Ϡ۞ ኳߏଂ݈˘ॡഇ็ᅍֽ҃Ă҃ۏࢋ۞๥ᛇጯ௫͕̚ (Tissot et al., 1997; ٙତᛈ۞̼ጯ ጡĂѩநኢᄮצTissot and Stocker, 2000)Ą ਕᇆᜩ̚ᇽৠགྷր௚ٕ̼ጯຏ ρᖪ۞གྷរົՙؠјᖪ۞ҖࠎĂ఺ᕇֹ ࠎ఺჌జ͔੓۞૙͹ઐр̙֭ᅮࢋጯ௫ٕ੃ к ጸ۞ણᄃĄޝזצ Hopkins’ host-selection principle ኳႷ (Jaenike, 1983; van Emden et al., Ҍϫ݈ࠎͤĂសਂϫ (Wiklund, 1973; 1996; Rojas and Wyatt, 1999)Ąጐგ఺࣎ந Papaj, 1986; Landolt and Molina, 1996; ρᖪགྷរົᇆᜩ Cunningham et al., 1998; Rojas andٺຍĂҭߏ၆ڦ۞̂ޝזצኢ ;ᙋፂĄЯ Wyatt, 1999)ăᗕ ਂ ϫ (Jaenike, 1983, 1988۞ڇܫѣ΄ˠ͌ޝᖪ૙͹Ᏼፄᔘߏٿ ,ᖪ׎ৠགྷ௡ᖐдតၗॡົࢦ Cooley et al., 1986; Hoffmann, 1988; RayٿࠎԆБតၗ۞ ;௡֭ͷົ቙ౄາ۞ాඕ (Technau and 1999)ăቯਂϫ (Caubet and Jaisson, 1991 Heisenberg, 1982; Truman, 1990)ĂЯѩ̙ Kester and Brabosa, 1991; van Emden et (੃ጸ۞঻хĄ֭ͷ˵ѣጯ۰Բෞѝ al., 1996; Bjorksten and Hoffmann, 1998זΞਕ࠻͉ ;ഇٙઇ۞૙͹ઐр၁រ՟ѣ૟Ҁ̼۞јᖪᄃ ̈́ᐘਂϫ (Hopkins, 1917; Phillips, 1977 ᖪ࠰ѣ൴ٿρᖪ۞ᒖဩѣड़۞̶ฟ (Jaenike, 1988)ĄΩ Rietdorf and Steidle, 2002) ඈ അགྷགྷរ࿅۞૙͹யϠઐр۞னٺᖪ னѩᙷ၆ٿγѣధкࡁտ޽΍Ăјᖪഇ۞གྷរົᇆᜩ ઐрҖࠎ (Jaenike, 1988; Vet et al., 1995; ෪Ą۞ (Barron and Corbet, 2000)Ă҃఺ֱົᇆᜩҖ α৳֑෪ ( maculatus

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 12 ࣗᐼचᖪĂਕПचк჌ ّĄΩϺຐ౅࿅አଠᓁ૙͹֑ᇴณ࠹ТĂҭഴ۞ۏߏ˘჌ࠎच֑ࡊങ Ă׎ρᖪҋ ͌α৳֑෪ઐр۞૙͹̝ᇴณĂᆧΐ̙ઐр۞ޢ˯૙͹֑ٺĂᅬᖪயӉۏࡊങ֑ ૙͹֑̰ԆјĂ߇ҋρ ૙͹̝ᇴณĂֽෞҤ၆׎யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩĂͽٺӉြ̼ҌјᖪҀ̼࠰ ซ˘Վଣ੅α৳֑෪Яᑕ૙͹ּͧ҃አፋ۞ ۏጯ̼۞ۏᖪҌјᖪҀ̼ѝഇĂᖪវᄃ૙͹ങ ৳Ϗֽ၆αٺ૟ѣӄڍኳღ૜ତᛈĂயϠጯ௫ٕᇆᜩ۞፟ົ̙̈Ăҭ யӉͅᑕĄ҃ѩྏរඕ Ąځࢴٕјᖪѝഇ ֑෪ᅬᖪጯ௫གྷរԛё̈́ਕ˧۞ଣפ࿅ΝࡁտଂϏរᙋ֑෪ρᖪ གྷរ၆јᖪயӉઐр۞ᇆᜩĄ߇ώྏរࢵАຐ ౅࿅ͧྵ࿶ֈҋ̙Т૙͹ă̙ТҀ̼ᒖဩ (૟ ؅ਟᇄПݲ Т૙͹̝ৰϐ̚) ۞α৳֑෪၆̙ٺ΍ཉפུ ҋϤயӉᏴፄྏរඕ ΙȃၐᡛᙫྛЅႷᎴПݲܧТ૙͹ซҖҋϤ̙̈́ ᖪࠎα৳֑෪ 4C6-4 ݡրĂ׎໚ٿྏצ ࢴགྷរĂԺٕјᖪѝഇགྷរפĂᑭീρᖪ۞ڍ ξ઴ࡓٺࡧᒋ౾̀ވέ̂ࣗᐼचᖪࡁտٺ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр̝ड़ᑕĄ ҋ ͽ੼̰ވѣᙯα৳֑෪јᖪഇ۞ጯ௫ࡁտ̏ѣధ ֑̚ଳะֽ҃ (Hu, 1989)Ăд၁រ кಡӘĄ࿅Νࡁտ޽΍Ăα৳֑෪၆૙͹֑۞ ฯ 6ă7 ཱིݡրࡓ֑ (adzuki ) ( ჌ᙷă̂̈̈́׎˯̝ӉᇴӮ׍ѣᏰᙊਕ˧ angularis Wight) ௢΃࿶ዳкѐĂ෹࿅ 100 পّᘦؠĄ૙͹֑ͽৌ۩Β྅ۏMitchell, 1975, 1990)ĄHu et al. (1995) ۞ ͵΃ͽ˯ĂϠ) 18oC ۞ҽࣜऱ྆Ҍ͌– ٺϡ݈ཉֹٺࡁտ൴ன༊೩ֻ̙Тּ̝ͧ̂ă̈ࡓ֑ගα৳ ᔉˢĂ ᖪٕٿ෪யӉॡĂ֑෪ព඾ઐрд̂ࡓ֑˯யӉĂ ׌ฉͽ˯Ăͽ୭Ѫ૙͹֑̚Ξਕхд۞֑ ໢˭а໢Ҍ͌׌ฉͽ˯ĄވГொҌޢĂ൒ۏགྷរ࿅̂ă̈ࡓ֑ٙҫּͧ۞ᇆᜩ Ϡזצͷົ֭ Ąтͽ̙Тּ̝ͧ ࿶ዳα৳֑෪ॡĂ೩ֻϤშϫ 5.5 mm ۞ᎡშޘԼត၆̂ࡓ֑۞ઐр඀҃ ࡓ֑ᄃქ֑஄Ъ೩ֻගα৳֑෪யӉĂ݋ົ൴ ٙᎡᏴ΍ۡश 5.5 mm ͽ˯ͷЍ໣࿸႕̝ࡓ ࡓ֑۞ּͧົᐌ඾೩ֻࡓּ֑ͧ ֑үࠎ૙͹֑Ąα৳֑෪࿶ዳдۡश 9.5ٺன׎யӉ ᆧΐ҃ᆧΐĂពϯ೩੼ઐр૙͹ּͧ˵ົព cmĂ੼ 5.5 cm ̝๬ቱ஼̚Ă׎̚ΐˢࡗ 1000۞ Shiau et al., ᔺࡓ֑֭ତˢ 13 ၆ᅬăฯјᖪĄ࿶ዳᒖဩࠎ) ޘ඾ᇆᜩᅬᖪ۞யӉઐр඀ ቐ̚ĄΩܜĄ҃˘ਠᄮࠎдࡓ֑ăქ֑௡ЪҋϤᏴ 28 ± 1oCăRH 50Ƃ70% ̝БຳϠ(1994 ཱི έݑ 5זमள γ૟Ϥࡓ֑࿶ዳ۞α৳֑෪ତ჌̈̂ٺፄ˭Ăα৳֑෪ព඾ઐрࡓ֑ߏϤ ٙ࡭ (Thanthianga and Mitchell, 1990; ქ֑ͽ࠹Т͞ё࿶ዳ˘΃Ąྏរ̝૙͹֑ࠎ੼ Hu et al., 1995; Cheng et al., 2003)Ąҭώ ฯ 6ă7 ཱིݡրࡓ֑ăέݑ 5 ཱིݡրქ֑ (ҋϤᏴፄॡĂ၆ˬ჌ֻྏ૙ (mung bean) (Vigna radiata (L.) Wilczekܧࡁտពϯᅬᖪд (͹ (ࡓ֑ăქ֑ٕโ֑) ̝யӉઐр֭൑ព඾ ̈́έݑ 3 ཱིݡրโ֑ (black soybean मளćдҋϤᏴፄॡĂݒົព඾ઐрࡓ֑׶โ (Glycine max)Ă֭ͽϹ੨࿅̝α৳֑෪ᅬᖪ јᖪഇ۞̙Т૙͹ ซҖயӉઐрྏរĄژĄЯѩĂࣃ଀ซ˘Վ̶֑ གྷរ၆யӉઐрᇆᜩ۞࠹၆ࢦࢋّĄώྏរ૟ ਠქ֑̂̈) ᄃ̂ქ Πȃ҂ᙫЅԙᙫԞ෈ငᡛᄇѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁ˘ٺ̈) ౅࿅೩ֻ̈ࡓ֑ ኇ៪ޟ ௡ЪֻᅬᖪซҖயӉᏴፄĂͽᗃ୻૙͹̝֑̂ ჌ᙷ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр۞࠹၆ࢦࢋ (˘)ρᖪ૙͹གྷរ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ̈́̈

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 13 ࠎᑭរα৳֑෪јᖪߏӎົઐрயӉд Hopkins principle)Ă̶Ҿͽࡓ֑ٕქ֑࿶ዳ Ϥٺࡗ 25 ͇Ăӈ࠹༊ޢࢴ࿅۞૙͹ (Hopkins’ host α৳֑෪ĂҋӉြ̼פρᖪഇ selection principle)Ă̶Ҿͽࡓ֑ٕქ֑࿶ዳ ρᖪഇซˢུഇॡĂͽྋ࣠˥࣠ฟ૙͹֑૟׎ ΍Ăͽͨඊ୻நᖪវ˯̝૙͹༤ढ̈́פᗓฟ૙͹֑ 12 ̈ॡ ଂ֑̰פޢα৳֑෪Ăܶ׎Ҁ̼ дۡश 9 cm ۩ૈዳϪă׍ࡓٸĂޢۏڴ෪јᖪĂ૟ᅬᖪăฯᖪ̶ฟĂͽ 5.5 cm ׎ଵ֑̝̰ ዳϪཉˢ 5Ƃ8 ੸јᖪ͞ёซҖ࿣ᗓ఍நĂ ֑ৰϐ̝ૈዳϪٕ׍ქ֑ৰϐ̝ૈዳϪඈᒖૈ ฟؕ߿જ̝јᖪග׎ 4 ̈ॡפ׎Ҁ̼Ăޞश 9 cm ဩۡ̚ٺГ૟ 5 ၆ᅬăฯᖪཉޢॡ̈ 24 Чӣ 50 ᔺࡓ֑ᄃٺ૟̝ཉޢϹԍ ҋϤϹ੨ĂᐌפĂޢዳϪ̚ 4 ̈ॡĂ΄׎ҋϤϹ੨ૈ۞ ҋϤ̈́ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ 50 ᔺქ֑̝ۡश 9 cm ૈዳϪ̚ 24 ̈ॡซҖܧԆБ̝ᅬᖪซҖ Ҿͧྵͽ̶ژͽᑭീயӉઐрĄ ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរĂͽត̶͞ ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ̶ࠎˬ௡Ă̶Ҿдۡ ࡓăქ֑࿶ዳ̙̈́ТҀ̼ᒖဩ఍நड़ᑕ̝ព඾ܧ श 9 cm ۞ૈዳϪ̚ཉˢ 100 ᔺࡓ֑ăქ֑ٕ ّĂͽα৳֑෪дࡓ֑˯̝யӉּͧซҖјᖪ โ֑Ă̶֭Ҿତˢ˘੸Ҁ̼ҋࡓ֑ٕქ֑۞α ѝഇགྷរ۞मளّͧྵ (SAS, 1990)Ą ੃ޢ෪ᅬᖪĂග̟ 24 ̈ॡឰ׎யӉĂ̝֑৳ Ч჌૙͹˯۞ᓁயӉᇴĄ έȃኇ៪౰֊୑ԁӰυᔮۡٺᐂ ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរϺ̶ࠎˬ௡఍நĂ఍ந (˘)૙͹ּͧ၆யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ ௡ A ߏдۡश 9 cm ۞ૈዳϪ̚ཉˢ 50 ᔺࡓ д࠹Тᇴณ̝̙Т૙͹֑ҋϤᏴፄயӉ ព۞૙͹ઐځᄃ 50 ᔺქ֑Ă఍ந௡ B ཉˢ 50 ᔺࡓ֑ᄃ ྏរ˭Ăα৳֑෪ਕܑன΍֑ ᔺโ֑Ă఍ந௡ C ཉˢ 50 ᔺโ֑ᄃ 50 ᔺ рĂពϯα৳֑෪ਕෞҤ૙͹ݡኳଂ҃ซҖ 50 ᆧΐઐрّҲ۞૙͹ּͧĂڍქ֑ĂГ̶Ҿତˢ˘੸Ҁ̼ҋࡓ֑ٕქ֑۞α யӉᏴፄĄт ૙ٺ੃ ᅬᖪдѩ૙͹˯யӉּͧ۞ᆧΐព඾੼ޢ෪ᅬᖪĂග̟ 24 ̈ॡឰ׎யӉĂ̝֑৳ Ч჌૙͹˯۞ᓁயӉᇴĄ ͹ּ̝ͧᆧΐĂ݋͚޺૙͹ּͧົᇆᜩ׎யٺᐂ ૙͹ઐрྏរͽயӉᇴࠎෞҤᇾ໤ĄЧ఍ Ӊઐр۞઄ᄲĄ̝ͅĂӈ̙͚޺ᅬᖪዎ࿃૙ ͹۞གྷរΞᇆᜩ׎யӉઐр۞઄ᄲĄѩྏរ 10 ٺந̝ࢦᖬᇴӮࠎ 20 ௡ͽ˯ĂᓁӉᇴҲ Ăഴ͌˭ڶߏӀϡдᓁ૙͹֑ᇴณ࠹Т۞ଐ ̶ొڍҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕܧᔺ݋̙̟੃ᐂĄ two-way ANOVA) ᑭീ̙Тρ α৳֑෪ઐр۞૙͹̝ᇴณĂᆧΐ̙ઐр۞) ژͽត̶͞ үϡ ૙͹̝ᇴณĂෞҤ၆׎૙͹ઐр۞ᇆᜩĂͽ̝ޘצᖪགྷរ̈́யӉ૙͹၆ᅬᖪ૙͹ତ ݋ ଣ੅α৳֑෪ਕӎЯᑕ૙͹̶οଐԛ҃አፋ̶ొڍSAS, 1990)ĄҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕ) ͽ t-test ̶ҾᑭീЧ఍ந̚Ă֑෪д׌჌̙ ׎யӉᏴፄĄ ͽࡓ֑࿶ዳĂֶ݈ࢗ఍நٙᒔ଀̏Ϲ੨פ Т૙͹˯யӉᇴ̝मளĄ֭ࢍზ׎д̙Т૙͹ ૈ யӉּͧĂͽ t-test ᑭീ̙Тρᖪགྷរ ̝ᅬᖪ̶ࠎˬ௡ྏរĂ఍ந௡ A д 9 cm̝˯ ၆̙Т఍ந̚ઐрّྵ੼̝૙͹˯யӉּͧ ዳϪ̚ཉˢ 20 ᔺࡓ֑ᄃ 80 ᔺქ֑ć఍ந௡ B үϡ (SAS, 1990)Ą ཉˢ 20 ᔺࡓ֑ᄃ 80 ᔺโ֑ć఍ந௡ C ཉˢ̝ јᖪѝഇགྷរ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ 20 ᔺโ֑ᄃ 80 ᔺქ֑Ă̶Ҿତˢ˘੸Ҁ̼ҋ(˟) ࡓ֑ٕქ֑۞α৳֑෪ᅬᖪĂග̟ 24 ̈ॡய ۞זҀ̼ॡٙତᛈٺࠎ˞ᑭរα৳֑෪ ᒖဩĂߏӎົᇆᜩ׎јᖪ૙͹ઐр (neo- ӉĂЧ఍ந̝ࢦᖬᇴӮࠎ 20 ௡Ă੃ᐂ׎дЧ

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 14 ݎ!!჌૙͹˯̝யӉּͧĂ֭ͽ Manly ̝ͩ α ๖ ޽ᇾ७ϒֻᑕ̙Т૙͹ּ֑̝ͧ (Manly et al., 1972)Ăͽࢍზдֻᑕ࠹Тᇴณ̝૙͹֑ Ιȃ҂ᙫЅԙᙫԞ෈ငᡛᄇѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁ ኇ៪ޟ ॡĂα৳֑෪ᅬᖪ၆̙Т૙͹֑̝ઐр޽ᇴ α)Ă (˘)ρᖪ૙͹གྷរ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ) 1 1. ܧҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ = ri α i ( ) ni ∑ r j n j Ҁ̼ҋࡓ֑۞α৳֑෪дࡓ֑ăქ֑ٕโ ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏܧ჌ֻྏ૙͹˯ซҖˬ֑ ၆૙͹ iޢ׎̚ αi ΃ܑགྷ Manly’s α ७ϒ ពϯјᖪдˬ჌૙͹̝யӉᇴม֭൑ڍរĂඕ ٕ) ૙͹̚ iצઐр޽ᇴĂri ( or r j ) ΃ܑତ۞ ព඾मள (F2,64 = 0.0, p = 0.995; ဦ˘)ĄҀ j ) ૙͹ٙҫּͧĂni (or nj ) ΃ܑٙѣ૙͹̚ ܧ჌ֻྏ૙͹ซҖˬٺi (ٕ j ) ૙͹ٙҫּͧĄГͽ t-test ̶Ҿᑭ ̼ҋქ֑۞α৳֑෪ ពϯјᖪдˬ჌ڍീֻᑕ̙Тּͧ૙͹ (ӈ 1:1 ̈́ 1:4) ఍நม ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរĂྏរඕ = ઐр޽ᇴ̝मள (SAS, 1990)Ą ૙͹֑̝யӉᇴม˵൑ព඾मள (F2,60 ૙͹̂̈၆யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ 0.95, p = 0.394; ဦ˘)ĄЯѩĂ̙ኢјᖪҀ̼(˟) Ă ҋࡓ֑ٕქ֑Ăֻᑕಏ჌૙͹֑ॡĂјᖪ၆ֻڍࡓ֑ᄃქ֑ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ۞ඕ Ăӈρᖪ૙͹གྷរ̙ܕ࠹ޘצྏ૙͹̝யӉତ ڍពϯα৳֑෪дქ֑˯ೀͼ̙யӉĂѩඕ Ąଘ༊ֻග׎ქ֑ޘצᇆᜩα৳֑෪۞૙͹ତ ٺពϯα৳֑෪׍ព඾۞૙͹ઐрّĄϤ ࡓăქ֑дγ៍˯׍ព඾̝̂̈मளĂϒ૱ யӉॡĂͽქ֑࿶ዳ۞α৳֑෪׎யӉᇴព඾ ;ͽࡓ֑࿶ዳ۰ (F1,42 = 5.41, p = 0.025ٺࡓ֑̝πӮࢦณ (168.4 ± 3.91 mg, n = ੼ ϒ૱ქ֑ (58.62 ± 1.08 mg, ဦ˘)ćֻ҃ගࡓ֑ٕโ֑ॡĂ࿶ዳ૙͹၆யӉٺព඾੼ (100 = n = 100) ̝πӮࢦณ (t = 27.17, p < ᇴ՟ѣព඾ᇆᜩ (ࡓ֑ĈF1,41 = 3.33, p Ăࠎᒢྋα৳֑෪̝૙͹ઐрߏӎϤ 0.08; โ֑ĈF1,41 = 0.5, p = 0.48; ဦ˘)Ą(0.0001 ពΞ֍۞̂̈मளٙ࡭ĂЯѩ 2. ҋϤᏴፄྏរځࡓăქ֑ٺ ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ̚Ăα৳֑෪дܧ߄Ᏼྵ̝̈ࡓ֑ (πӮࢦณࠎ 57.22 ± ᔵ൒ ĂҭߏТॡ೩ֻࡓăܕmg, n = 100) ᄃྵ̝̂ქ֑ (πӮࢦ ࡓ֑ᄃქ֑۞யӉᇴ࠹ 0.91 ณࠎ 93.15 ± 1.09 mg, n = 100) ֹࡓ֑̝ ქ֑ග̟ҋϤᏴፄॡĂ݋̙ኢҀ̼ҋࡓ֑ٕߏ ˯ქ֑̝πӮࢦณ (t = 25.3, p ქ֑Ăα৳֑෪ೀͼ૟ٙѣӉӮயдࡓ֑ٺπӮࢦณҲ ,ĂซҖҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរĄ (ࡓ֑Ĉt = 27.68, p < 0.001; ქ֑Ĉt = 21.64(0.0001 > ពгઐрࡓځઐрྏរͽயӉּͧࠎෞҤᇾ໤Ă߄Ᏼ̈ p < 0.001; ܑ˘)Ăពϯα৳֑෪ श 9 cm ۞ ֑ĄТॡֻගࡓăโ֑۞ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕۡٺࡓ֑ 50 ᔺᄃ̂ქ֑ 50 ᔺĂཉ ϺТ (ࡓ֑Ĉt = 13.32, p < 0.001; ქ֑Ĉtڍ ዳϪ̚Ăତˢͽࡓ֑࿶ዳă12 ̈ॡ̰Ҁ̼Ăૈ ࢗ఍நᒔ଀Ϲ੨ԆБ̝α৳֑෪ᅬᖪซ = 9.85, p < 0.001; ܑ˘)Ą҃Тॡֻගโăქֶ݈ Җ 24 ̈ॡ̝யӉྏរĂࢦᖬᇴࠎ 22 ௡Ąͽ ֑ซҖҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរॡĂҀ̼ҋࡓ֑̈́ქ = t-test ̶Ҿᑭീ֑෪д׌჌̙Т૙͹˯யӉᇴ ֑̝α৳֑෪Ӯઐрдโ֑˯யӉ (ࡓ֑Ĉt ܑ ;मள (SAS, 1990)Ą 5.29, p < 0.001; ქ֑Ĉt = 2.4, p = 0.024̝ ĄЯѩĂҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរពϯα৳֑෪၆(˘

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 15 ၐசкْΰϞߨՌҥᒵᐅ౰֊୑ԁ (mean ɲڧᆦْႷᎴϞѲ઴ْຫӵऔْȃᆦْᇄ༃ْ้έᆍܖყΙ! оऔْ ՌϚӣႷᎴசкϞ౰֊ኵԤᡗ๿৯౴ȄپS.E.)ȄϚӣԅҔфߒоശωᡗ๿৯౴Шၶ Fig. 1.! No-choice test of host acceptance by the seed beetle, reared on , mung bean or black soybean. Error bars represent standard errors. Different letters indicate a significant difference in number of eggs between individuals reared on different hosts by Least Significant Test.

ಢӫϞՌҥᒵᐅ౰֊ၐᡛϛϞ౰֊୑ԁڍऔْȃᆦْЅ༃ْӈټߒΙ! ႷᎴՌऔْЅᆦْϞѲ઴ْຫӵඪ Table 1.! Oviposition preference (mean ± S.E.) of Callosobruchus maculatus reared on adzuki or mung beans in free choice tests using combinations of adzuki beans, mung beans, and black soybeans Rearing Host Adzuki beans Mung beans No. eggs on No. eggs on Treatments (Host a, Host b) Host a Host b t p Host a Host b t p 1) A (Adzuki beans, Mung beans) 30.74 ± 1.08 0.32 ± 0.17 27.68 < 0.001 28.97 ± 1.31 0.55 ± 0.17 21.64 < 0.001 B (Adzuki beans, Black soybeans) 26.5 ± 1.34 5.68 ± 0.69 13.32 < 0.001 25.32 ± 1.56 4.86 ± 0.91 9.85 < 0.001 C (Black soybeans, Mung beans) 23.95 ± 1.74 8.68 ± 1.62 5.29 < 0.001 21.12 ± 1.72 13.08 ± 1.81 2.4 0.024 1) p < 0.05 indicates significant difference based on t-test.

ޞዳϪĂࡓ֑ৰٕქ֑ৰ̚ૈ۩ٺĂ̶Ҿཉޢ Т૙͹׍ព඾۞யӉઐрमளĂҭ̙Тᖪ໚̙ ৳൴னαڍ၆யӉઐр֭൑ព඾ᇆᜩ (t = 1.64, p = ׎Ҁ̼ГซҖ૙͹ઐр۞ീྏĂඕ ࡓ֑˯யӉĂͷҀ̼ᒖဩ఍ٺ0.106ăt = 0.96, p = 0.342ăt = 1.66, p = ֑෪Ӯព඾ઐр

ܑ ;Ăӈρᖪ૙͹གྷរ̙֭ᇆᜩα৳ நड़ᑕ̙ព඾ (F2,6 = 2.08, p = 0.206(˟ܑ ;0.103 ٺ෪۞૙͹ઐрّĄ ˬ)Ąͽქ֑࿶ዳ̝α৳֑෪ϺТᇹព඾ઐр֑

= јᖪѝഇགྷរ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐр۞ᇆᜩ ࡓ֑˯யӉĂҀ̼ᒖဩ఍நड़ᑕ̙ព඾ (F2,9(˟) ΍ 0.01, p = 0.987; ܑˬ)ĄЯѩĂ̙ኢߏͽࡓ֑פഇུٺࡓ֑۞α৳֑෪ٺ૟࿶ዳ

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 16 ಢӫϞՌҥᒵᐅ౰֊ၐᡛϛϞ౰֊୑ԁ৯౴ڍऔْȃᆦْЅ༃ْӈټᆦْϞѲ઴ْຫӵඪܖߒΠ! ႷᎴՌऔْ Table 2.! Oviposition preference (%) of Callosobruchus maculatus reared on adzuki beans or mung beans in free choice tests with combinations of adzuki beans, mung beans and black soybeans Rearing host Adzuki beans Mung beans Proportion of eggs on Proportion of eggs on Treatments (Host a, Host b) Host b Host b t p 1) A (Adzuki beans, Mung beans) 0.99 ± 0.52 1.77 ± 0.54 1.64 0.106 B (Adzuki beans, Black soybeans) 17.53 ± 1.97 15.91 ± 2.86 0.96 0.342 C (Black soybeans, Mung beans) 26.11 ± 4.06 37.38 ± 4.73 1.66 0.103 1) p > 0.05 indicates that proportions of eggs on host b for the beetles that reared on adzuki and mung bean are not significantly different by t–test. Data were transformed to Sin–1 ( ) values prior to analysis.

ኇ៪ޟߒέ! ԙᙫԞ෈ငᡛᄇႷᎴՌϚӣசкѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁ Table 3.! Early adult experience on oviposition preference measured by proportion of eggs on adzuki beans by the seed beetle, Callosobruchus maculatus, reared on adzuki beans or mung beans Rearing host Adzuki beans Mung beans Treatments (mean ± S.E.) (mean ± S.E.) Control 100 ± 0 99.04 ± 0.96 Adzuki bean powder 98.37 ± 0.82 99.11 ± 0.89 Mung bean powder 97.02 ± 1.50 98.65 ± 1.35 p 1) 0.206 0.987 1) p > 0.05 indicates that proportions of eggs laid on adzuki bean by females with different early adult experiences are not significantly different by ANOVA. Data were transformed to Sin–1 ( ) values prior to analysis.

Ҁ̼ॡٙତᛈ ࡓăโ֑૙͹ּͧ 1:1 ̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរٺߏქ֑࿶ዳ۞α৳֑෪Ă׎ٕ ࡓăโ֑૙͹ּͧ 1:4ٺĂα৳֑෪ྵͧڍ྽Ӯ̙ົᇆᜩ׎யӉઐрĄϤͽ˯ྏរ ඕק۞ז ˵Ă൴னα৳֑෪јᖪ̙֭ົ၆׎ρഇഅགྷ ̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរ̚Ă၆โ֑۞ઐр޽ᇴڍඕ யϠઐ ѣព඾ᆧΐ (t = 2.88, p = 0.006; ܑα)Ąҭۏࢴ࿅ٕјᖪѝഇତᛈ࿅۞૙͹ങפ рĄ ߏдโăქ֑૙͹ּͧ 1:4 ̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉ ྏរ̚Ă၆ქ֑۞ઐр޽ᇴݒ՟ѣᆧΐ۞ଐԛ Ӱυ (t = 0.39, p = 0.7; ܑα)ĄЯѩĂᒖဩ̚۞૙ޟΠȃኇ៪౰֊୑ԁ ૙͹ּͧ၆யӉઐр̝ᇆᜩ ͹̶οּͧᆧΐॡົᆧΐα৳֑෪дѩ૙͹(˘) ࡓăქ֑૙͹ּͧ 1:4 ̝ҋ ֑˯۞யӉּͧĂҭͽઐр޽ᇴࠎ޽ᇾΐͽͧٺα৳֑෪ ࡓ֑૙͹˯ய ྵॡĂពϯઐрّ۞Լត̙֭ߏ˘࡭۞ன෪ĄٺϤᏴፄயӉྏរ̚Ăព඾ઐр Ӊ (t = 10.68, p < 0.0001; ܑα)Ăᄃࡓăქ (˟)૙͹̂̈၆யӉઐр̝ᇆᜩ ૱༊α৳֑෪ዎ࿃̈ࡓ֑ᄃ̂ქ֑Ă׶ϒ ͧڍ૙͹ּͧ 1:1 ̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕ֑ Ă൴னྵͧڍĂα৳֑෪၆ქ֑̝ઐр޽ᇴົᐌქ֑ᆧΐ ࡓăქ֑̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕྵ ព඾ᆧΐ (t = 3.9, p < 0.001; ܑα)Ąᄃ α৳֑෪၆ქ֑̝யӉઐрົព඾೩੼ (t҃

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 17 ኇ៪ޟᄇѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁٽߒѲ! சкШ Table 4.! Effect of different ratios of adzuki beans, mung beans or black soybeans on oviposition preference of the seed beetle, Callosobruchus maculatus Host ratio (host a : host b) 1 : 1 1 : 4 Preference index of Preference index of Treatments (Host a, Host b) Host b Host b t p 1) A (Adzuki beans, Mung beans) 0.01 ± 0.01 0.05 ± 0.01 3.9 < 0.001 B (Adzuki beans, Black soybeans) 0.18 ± 0.02 0.28 ± 0.04 2.88 0.006 C (Black soybeans, Mung beans) 0.26 ± 0.04 0.24 ± 0.04 0.39 0.7 1) p < 0.05 indicates significant difference based on t-test.

ყΠ! சкσωᄇѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁϞհҢ (mean ɲ S.E.)Ȅ(A) ӵғலऔْᇄғலᆦْΰϞ౰֊ԻϷШ (n = 31)ȇ Ԥᡗ๿৯౴ȄϚӣԅҔфߒٽசкْΰϞ౰֊ШڍB) ӵωऔْᇄσᆦْΰϞ౰֊ԻϷШ (n = 22)Ȅ࢑ဴфߒܻ) Ԥᡗ๿৯౴Ȅٽоശωᡗ๿৯౴ШၶӨ೎౩ಢܻᆦْϞ౰֊Ш Fig. 2.! The effect of host size on proportion of eggs laid by the seed beetle, C. maculatus. (A) host preference on normal size of adzuki bean and mung bean; (B) host preference on small adzuki bean and big mung bean. Error bars represent standard errors. Asterisks above the bars indicate significant differences between the proportions of eggs laid on adzuki and mung beans. Different letters indicate a significant difference between the proportions of eggs laid on mung beans in two treatments.

ქ֑ (t = 8.48, p < 0.0001)Ăᙋٺp < 0.001; ဦ˟)Ăពϯ૙͹̂̈۞ቁ р̪ព඾੼ ,10.9= ૙͹჌ᙷ̪ߏᇆᜩயӉઐрྵࢦࢋ۞Я̄Ąځ ᇆᜩα৳֑෪̝யӉઐрĄҭߏд̈ࡓ֑ᄃົ Ă၆ࡓ֑̝ઐڍქ֑̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរඕ̂

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 18 ᒖဩĂ̖ߏᇆᜩјᖪ૙͹Ᏼፄ۞ՙؠЯ̄۞ז ଆ!!፣ (Smith and Cornell, 1979; Jaisson, 1980; ;ଣ੅α৳֑෪јᖪ݈གྷរ (Β Jaenike, 1983, 1988; Hoffmann, 1988ޘώྏរࢵ ߁ρᖪഇ̈́јᖪѝഇགྷរ) ၆α৳֑෪யӉઐ Caubet and Jaisson, 1991; Kester and ;൴ன׎၆ქ֑ᄃโ֑۞யӉ Barbosa, 1991; van Emden et al., 1996ޘр̝үϡć˵ࢵ ពࢫҲ۞ன Bjorksten and Hoffmann, 1998; Breed etځઐрдҋϤᏴፄྏរ̚ົѣ ෪ĂЯѩซ˘Վͧྵགྷ።̙Т૙͹གྷរ (ּ al., 1998; Barron and Corbet, 1999; Rojas ৳тĈ૙͹჌ᙷăּͧ̈́૙͹̂̈) ၆Լតα৳ and Wyatt, 1999)ĄҭϤјᖪѝഇགྷរ၆α ෪யӉઐр̝࠹၆ࢦࢋّĄ ֑෪யӉઐр̝ྏរពϯĂ̙ኢֽҋࡓ֑ٕქ֑ Ăˬ჌ུഇ఍ந̝ड़ᑕӮ̙ព඾ (ܑˬ)ĄЯ֑ Ιȃ҂ᙫЅԙᙫԞ෈ငᡛᄇѲ઴ْຫ౰֊୑ԁ ѩĂጐგѣ఺ᆃкࡁտ͚޺ρᖪགྷរٕјᖪ݈ ኇ៪ གྷរົᇆᜩјᖪ૙͹Ᏼፄ۞ઐрĂტЪώྏរޟ ൴னĂ̙ኢߏρᖪགྷរᇆᜩјᖪઐр۞ڍᔵ൒ѣధкࡁտ൴னĂρᖪགྷរົᇆᜩј ඕ ᖪ̝யӉઐр (Hopkins, 1917; Thrope, Hopkins’ host-selection principlećٕ ߏ Ϥ ј Dethier, 1954; Manning, 1967; ᖪҀ̼ॡٙᒔ଀۞གྷរ҃Լត૙͹ઐр۞ ;1930 α৳֑෪Ӯ՟ѣ൴ٺPhillips, 1977; Corbet, 1985; Szentesi and neo-Hopkins principleĂ Jermy, 1990; Rietdorf and Steidle, 2002)Ą ன఺჌ன෪Ą ᖪ఼૱ົઐ Ω˘͞ࢬĂͽქ֑࿶ዳ۞α৳֑෪дˬ჌ٿॡĂۏࢴٕயӉ۞ങפӈ༊Ᏼፄ ͽࡓ֑࿶ዳ۞ٺ჌ᙷ ֻྏ૙͹֑˯̝யӉᇴ࠰੼ۏࢴ࿅۞ങפρഇॡഅགྷٺр ҋϤᏴፄ α৳֑෪̝யӉᇴ (ဦ˘)Ă҃α৳֑෪дქ֑ܧSchoonhoven et al., 1998)ĄҭϤ) தӮྵࡓ֑˯ࠎָܜă൴ֈ̈́୉ཏϠܜĂ̙͚֭޺఺࣎நኢ ˯̝ϠڍᄃҋϤᏴፄྏរ̝ඕ ҋϤᏴፄ (Lin, 2004)Ăͷࡓ֑ᄃქ֑ࠎТᛳĂᏐቡᙯܼܧဦ˘ăܑ˘)Ąдֻᑕಏ჌૙͹̝) Hu et al., 2000)Ă၆α৳֑෪ֽᄲ) ܕᒖဩ˭Ăα৳֑෪၆ಏ፾хд̝૙͹֑д ࠹༊ତטࢨ ዋЪ۞૙͹ĂҭߏдҋϤᏴ૱ܧ൑मள (ဦ˘)ĂଯീΪࢋᒖဩ̚х ქ֑˵ߏ˘֭࣎˯ޘצତ дΞӀϡ̝૙͹Ăα৳֑෪ಶΞдѩ૙͹˯ய ፄயӉྏរ˭Ăα৳֑෪၆ࡓ֑۞ઐрព඾੼ ᄃ Chiu and Messinaڍქ֑ (ܑ˘)Ăѩඕٺ ৳ӉćϺѣΞਕߏࠎ˞ྋੵྶӉᑅ˧Ăֹ଀α ࠹ڍѩ૙͹Ąҭ༊α৳֑෪Тॡዎ (1994) ̈́ Cheng et al. (2003) ̝ඕצ෪̙଀̙ତ֑ ă൴ֈ͞ࢬֽ࠻ĂᏴፄქ֑யӉܜ࿃̙Т૙͹֑ॡĂα৳֑෪ົܑன၆̙Т૙͹ ҬĄࡶଂϠ ઐр (ܑ˘)Ăពϯ׎ਕТॡ၆̙Т૙͹֑ซ ၆׎̄΃ዋхࣃՀࠎѣӀĂα৳֑෪̙ᑕтѩ۞ ࡓ֑˯யӉĂҭ҂ᇋࡓ֑ᄃქ֑̝ٺҖݡኳෞҤĄ̙҃ኢߏͽࡓ֑ٕქ֑࿶ዳ̝α ព඾ઐр ࡓ֑˯யӉٺ෪ĂдТॡዎ࿃̙Т૙͹֑ॡĂ၆ࡓ֑ᄃ ม̂̈۞मளĂ݋α৳֑෪Ᏼፄ֑৳ ֑৳ქ֑ (ܑ˘)Ăព ϯ ρ ᖪ ӈΞਕࠎѣӀ۞ҖࠎĄ̏ѣಡጱ޽΍Ăαٺโ֑۞ઐр࠰ព඾੼ ૙͹ய̂ྵٺࢴགྷរ၆α৳֑෪јᖪ̝யӉઐр֭൑ព ෪ਕૉᏰҾ૙͹۞֭̂̈ઐрפ ඾ᇆᜩĄ Ӊ (Mitchell, 1990; Hu et al., 1995; Yang ;ᖪଂུҀ̼Ҍјᖪ۞ and Horng, 2002; Cheng et al., 2003ٿΩѣֱࡁտᄮࠎĂ ఺̈˘߱Ⴭࠎјᖪѝഇགྷរ۞ॡม̰ٙତᛈ Cope and Fox, 2003)Ą҃ Yang and Horng

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 19 ҋϤᏴፄᄃҋϤᏴፄயܧγĂώࡁտϺ൴னд ྵٺᄮࠎĂਕ̶Ᏸ૙͹̂̈҃૟Ӊய (2002) ૙͹˯۞Җࠎдዋᑕ˯׍ѣࢦ̂۞ຍཌྷĄд Ӊྏរ̚Ăα৳֑෪дˬ჌૙͹֑˯̝யӉሀ̂ צҋϤᏴፄ˭̝૙͹ତܧԆБ઀ஐ۞૙ ёϺѣٙमளĂពϯזዎ࿃ٽҋ൒ᒖဩ̚Ă֑෪̙ट ΞਕטᄃҋϤᏴፄ˭̝૙͹ઐрĂ׎үϡ፟ޘ ۞ז͹ (Fox and Mousseau, 1995)Ă׎ዎ࿃ ૙͹̂к̏జӀϡ࿅ٕߏ̏ѣ׎΁ρᖪ૙Ϡ ̙ТĂࣃ଀Հซ˘Վଣ੅Ą ᆧΐĂρᖪ۞ޘ׎̚Ă҃ᐌ඾૙͹֑̰ρᖪ૜ ȃσωᄇٽӰυȈசкШޟх߿தົᐌ̝ࢫҲ (Mitchell, 1990; Πȃኇ៪౰֊୑ԁ Mitchell and Thanthianga, 1990)Ąдѣρ ౰֊୑ԁϞኇ៪ Ăྵ̂۞૙͹ٙਕ೩ֻ۞ྤ໚ ͽ̙Тּ̝ͧࡓăქ֑஄Ъ೩ֻα৳֑෪˭ڶଐ۞ۋᖪᚮ ࡓ֑۞ּͧĂᐌ೩ֻࡓٺ૙͹۞х߿தྵ੼ யӉĂ൴ன֑෪யӉ̂ٺ࠹၆ྵкĂЯ҃ρᖪ Mitchell, 1975; Visser, 1994; Hu et al., ּ֑ͧᆧΐ҃ᆧΐ (Shiau et al., 1994)Ą༊) Ăα৳֑෪ޢHoffmeister et al., 1999)ĄЯѩĂᔵ൒ ೩੼ࣧώྵ̙ઐр૙͹֑۞ּͧ ;1995 ქ֑ćҭͅ៍ٺத дࡓ֑˯̝யӉᇴ̪ព඾੼ܜă൴ֈ̈́୉ཏϠܜα৳֑෪дქ֑˯̝Ϡ ࡓ֑Ăҭ׌۰࠰ࠎዋЪα৳֑෪۞૙ ࡓăโ֑ᄃโăქ֑௡Ъ̝ҋϤᏴፄݒѣ̙ТٺӮᐹ α)Ă༊೩੼ࣧώྵ̙ઐр۞โܑ֑̝) ڍᑅ˧Ă೩̿׎̄΃х ۞ඕۋ͹Ă҃ࠎ˞ࢫҲρᖪᚮ Ăα৳֑෪дโ֑˯̝ᓁயӉᇴӈព඾ޢּͧ ߿தĂઐр૟Ӊயдྵ̂۞ࡓ֑ߏྵѣӀ۞Ᏼ ࡓ֑Ă҃дโăქ֑఍ந௡݋Ξ൴ன׌۰ٺፄĄ ੼ Cheng et al. (2003) ᄮࠎα৳֑෪ᅬᖪ ˯̝யӉᇴ൑ព඾मளĄͽ Manley’s α ޽ᇴ Ăͧྵдֻᑕ࠹Тᇴณ૙ޢТ૙͹̝ઐрّᄃ׎̄΃х߿த̝ ७ϒֻᑕ૙͹ּ̙ͧٺ၆ โ֑˯̝ ͹ॡĂα৳֑෪၆૙͹֑̝࠹၆ઐр޽ᇴĂඕٺมĂ׍ѣព඾۞࠹ᙯّĄα৳֑෪ Ăα৳֑෪ޢ൴னྵ̙ઐр۞૙͹ּͧ೩੼ڍ ĂҀ̼̝ј(͇ 60 ٺ̂) ܜх߿தҲͷ൴ֈഇ mg)Ă҃ٙய ၆׎ઐрّ˵ົᐌ̝ᆧΐ (ܑα)Ăѩઐрّᆧ 300 ٺሴ̈ (ᅬјᖪࡗҲ૱ܧᖪ ૙͹ٺ၆α৳֑෪ߏѣӀ۞ĄϤܫϒ૱̂̈ (Ϗ൴ܑྤफ़)Ă૟ Ҁ ΐ̝ன෪࠹ٺӉϺព඾̝̈ ˯ҋโ֑̝α৳֑෪ᄃҀ̼ҋࡓ֑ăქ֑̝α ּͧᆧΐĂα৳֑෪ዎ࿃ѩᙷ૙͹̝፟த̼ ᒖဩ̚ߏྵᖳಱ̝ྤ໚Ăٺ෪ͧྵ׎൴ֈഇăӉ̂̈ (Lin, 2004)Ă ̿Ăܑϯѩᙷ૙͹֑৳ ͽ̈́Ҁ̼јᖪࢦ (Ҁ̼ҋࡓ֑Ăฯᖪ 507.23 ± ҃ྵઐр۞૙͹ٙ೩ֻ۞ྤ໚ྵ͌ĂЯѩ૟Ӊ ྤٺᖳಱ̝૙͹ĂΞᔖҺ૟Ӊะ̚யྵٺmgĂᅬ ᖪ 786.69 ± 14.20 mgĒҀ̼ҋ ̶੨ 13.69 Ąҭ೩੼૙͹ͧۋქ ֑Ăฯ ᖪ 477.76 ± 7.32 mgĂᅬ ᖪ 766.88 ± ໚ณ͌۞૙͹҃೩੼ρᖪᚮ ĂϤܑ̚Ξ൴னα৳֑෪ޘโ֑˯ܑ̝னӮ ּΪਕ೩੼ઐр඀ٺmg)Ă൴னα৳֑෪ 10.23 ព඾ઐр̪˭ڶქ֑ٕโּ֑ͧྵ੼۞ଐٺ ࢴࡓ֑ٕქ֑̝α৳֑෪ĂЯѩΞۢโפт̙ ̝ޘዋЪα ࡓ֑ćͷЯ೩੼૙͹ּͧ҃ᆧΐ׎ઐр඀ܧࡓ֑ᄃქ֑Ăҭ֭ٺࢦณᔵ൒̝֑̂ ࡓăโ֑ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏ ன෪̙֭˘࡭Ăдโăქ֑ҋϤᏴፄĂ༊ქ֑ٺ෪̝૙͹Ą֑৳ Ăα৳֑෪၆ქ֑̝ઐр֭Ϗᐌ̝ޢរĂα৳֑෪ព඾ઐрࡓ֑̝ன෪࠻ֽ (ܑ ּͧ೩੼ Ăα৳֑෪̝૙͹ઐрҬͼᄃ׎̄΃х߿த ᆧΐ (ܑα)Ą(˘ โăქ֑ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរĂα৳ ೩ֻࡓăქ֑̟α৳֑෪ซҖҋϤᏴፄயٺ࠹ᙯćҭ ዋЪ̝โ֑˯யӉĄѩ ӉྏរॡĂα৳֑෪дࡓ֑˯̝யӉᇴព඾੼̙ྵٺ෪ݒព඾ઐр֑

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 20 ქ֑Ąᔵ൒ࡓ֑჌̂ٺព̝யӉઐр ֑̝யӉּ̪ͧព඾੼ځࡓ֑ѣٺქ֑Ăពϯ׎၆ٺ ኳ̏ѣಡጱ (Gokhale etۏ፬וĄα৳֑෪д૙͹Ᏸᙊ࿅඀̚Ăਕૉჟ ϩ׍ѣயӉ(˘ܑ) ኳߏ֑෪ጯۏቁгᏰҾ૙͹֑̝ม۞̂̈मள (Avidov et al., 1990)ĂҭᇆᜩயӉઐр۞ ซ˘Վ۞ᗃޞal., 1965)Ă҃ซҖயӉྏរॡĂα৳֑෪А ௫۞ᇾ۞Ă׌۰ߏӎ࠹Т݋̪ѣ ϏயӉ̝૙ ୻ĄإٺயӉ̝૙͹֑۞πӮࢦณ̂ ࡓ̂ٺ͹֑Ă֭ͷд̂ă̈ࡓ֑۞Ᏼፄ˯ઐр யӉ (Mitchell, 1975; Hu et al., 1994; ᇬ!!ᗂ˯֑ Yang and Horng, 2002; Cope and Fox, Ăពϯ૙͹̂̈၆α৳֑෪̝யӉઐр ώࡁտᄋ઼ࡊົ NSC 93-2313-B-002-(2003 ࡓ֑ᄃქ֑дγ៍˯׍ 003 གྷ෱ྃӄĂᖰ࡭ᔁԢĄٺ׍ѣ˘ؠ۞ᇆᜩĄϤ ѣព඾̝̂̈मளĂͷࡓ֑̝πӮࢦณព඾̂ ქ֑ĂЯѩᄮࠎα৳֑෪၆ࡓ֑׍ព඾̝ய ЕҢМᝦٺ ࡓ֑ᄃქ֑ࢦณमளٙ࡭ٺӉઐрΞਕߏϤ (Shiau et al., 1994; Cheng et al., 2003)Ąҭ Avidov, Z., M. J. Berlinger, and S. W. ព Appelbaum. 1965. Physiological aspectsڍϤ̈ࡓ֑ă̂ქ֑௡Ъ̝ҋϤᏴፄྏរඕ :ქ֑ of host specificity in the BruchidaêٺϯĂα৳֑෪Ϊѣ 23.5% ۞Ӊய Ă׎̂кᇴ۞Ӊ̪யд̈ࡓ֑˯ (ဦ˟)Ăព III. Effect of curvature and surface˯ α৳֑෪၆ࡓ֑ઐр۞ area on oviposition of Callosobruchusܧϯ૙͹̂̈मள֭ ͹ЯĂᇆᜩயӉઐрྵ̂۞Я̄ଯീߏ૙͹֑ chinensis L. Anim. Behav. 13: 178- .Ą 180קώ֗۞পّĂּт૙͹ٙ೸൴΍ֽ۞ঈ Thanthianga and Mitchell (1990) ˵൴ன Barron, A. B. 2001. The life and death of .α৳֑෪дซҖயӉՙඉॡĂົАֶ૙͹̝჌ Hopkins’ host-selection principle. J .ᙷᏴፄĂГᏴፄ૙͹֑̝̂̈ĄГ૟ѩྏរඕ Insect Behav. 14: 725-737 .ᄃϒ૱ࡓăქ֑̝ҋϤᏴፄயӉྏរү˘ͧ Barron, A. B., and S. A. Corbet. 1999ڍ ქ֑૙ Preimaginal conditioning in DrosophilaٺĂ൴னд఺׌࣎ྏរ̚Ăα৳֑෪ྵ .͹˯̝யӉּͧѣព඾मளĂдϒ૱ࡓăქ֑ revisited. Anim. Behav. 58: 621-628 .ქ֑૙ Barron, A. B., and S. A. Corbet. 2000ٺ੨၆̚Ăα৳֑෪Ϊѣ 1% ۞Ӊய :͹Ă҃̈ࡓ֑ă̂ქ֑੨၆̚ݒѣ 23.5% ۞ Behavioural induction in Drosophila .ქ֑૙͹˯ (ဦ˟)Ăពϯα৳֑෪၆̂ timing and specificity. Entomol. ExpٺӉய .ࡓ֑Ăݒ̏ព Appl. 94: 159-171̈ٺქ֑۞யӉઐрᔵ൒՟ѣ੼ .ΞۢĂ Bjorksten, T. A., and A. A. Hoffmannڍ඾ᆧΐ׎၆ქ֑۞யӉઐрĄϤѩඕ ࠎᇆ 1998. cues influence searchingܧ૙͹̂̈ᔵ൒ົᇆᜩயӉઐрĂҭ׎֭ ᜩயӉઐрԼត̝͹ࢋЯ̄ćдᇆᜩயӉઐр behaviour and parasitism in the egg .ኳΞਕ parasitoid Trichogramma nr. brassicaeۏЯ̄̚Ă̙Т૙͹˯ٙхд۞̼ጯ۞ .ߏᇆᜩ۞ᙯᔣĂ҃ଯീдࡓ֑૙͹˯Ξਕѣ Ecol. Entomol. 23: 355-362̖ ,α৳֑෪д̈ࡓ Breed, M. D., E. A. Leger, A. N. PearceٺኳĂͽ࡭ۏԼតயӉઐр̝

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 21 and Y. J. Wang. 1998. Comb wax Learning in host selection in effects on the ontogeny of honey bee Helicoverpa armigera (Hübner) (Lepidoptera: nestmate recognition. Anim. Behav. Noctuidae). Anim. Behav. 55: 227- 55: 13-20. 234. Caubet, Y., and P. Jaisson. 1991. A Dethier, V. G. 1954. Evolution of feeding post-eclosion early learning involved preferences in phytophagous insects. in host recognition by Dinarmus basalis Evolution 8: 33-54. Rondani (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). van Emden, H. F., B. Sponagl, E. Wagner, Anim. Behav. 42: 977-980. T. Baker, S. Ganguly, and S. Cheng, I. C., R. L. Yang, and S. B. Horng. Douloumpaka. 1996. Hopkins’ ‘host 2003. Interaction between host selection principle’, another nail in preference and offspring survivorship its coffin. Physiol. Entomol. 21: 325- of Callosobruchus maculatus (Fab.). 328. Formosan Entomol. 23: 291-303. (in Fox, C. W., and T. A. Mousseau. 1995. Chinese) Determinants of clutch size and seed Chiu, Y. J., and F. J. Messina. 1994. Effect preference in a seed beetle, Stator of experience on host preference in beali (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Environ. Callosobruchus maculatus (Coleoptera: Entomol. 24: 1557-1561. Bruchidae): variability among populations. Gokhale, V. G., H. Honda, and I. J. Insect Behav. 7: 503-515. Yamamoto. 1990. Role of physical Cooley, S. S., R. J. Prokopy, P. T. and chemical stimuli of host McDonald, and T. T. Y. Wong. 1986. seeds in comparative ovipositional Learning in oviposition site selection behaviour of Callosobruchus maculatus by Ceratitis capitata flies. Entomol. (Fab.) and C. chinensis (Linn.) Exp. Appl. 40: 47-51. (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). pp. 45-51. In: Cope, J. M., and C. W. Fox. 2003. K. Fujii, M. R. Gatehouse, C. D. Oviposition decisions in the seed Johnson, R. Mitchell, and T. Yoshida, beetle, Callosobruchus maculatus eds. Bruchids and : Economics, (Coleoptera: Bruchidae): effects of seed Ecology and Coevolution. Kluwer size on superparasitism. J. Stored Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Prod. Res. 39: 355-365. Netherlands. Corbet, S. A. 1985. Insect chemosensory Hoffmann, A. A. 1988. Early adult responses: a chemical legacy hypothesis. experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Ecol. Entomol. 10: 143-153. J. Insect Physiol. 34: 197-204. Cunningham, J. P., M. F. A. Jallow, D. J. Hoffmeister, T. S., R. F. Lachlan, and B. D. Wright, and M. P. Zalucki. 1998. Roitberg. 1999. Do larger fruits

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 22 provide a partial refuge for rose-hip Postemergence learning in the insect flies against parasitoids? J. Insect parasitoid, Cotesia congregata (Say) Behav. 12: 451-460. (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). J. Insect Hopkins, A. D. 1917. A discussion of H.G. Behav. 4: 727-742. Hewitt’s paper on ‘Insect Behaviour’. Landolt, P. J., and O. Molina. 1996. J. Econ. Entomol. 10: 92-93. Host-finding by cabbage lopper moths Hu, J. M., M. Lavin, M. F. Wojciechowski, (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae): learning of and M. J. Sanderson. 2000. Phylogenetic host odor upon contact with host systematic of the tribe Millettieae foliage. J. Insect Behav. 9: 899-908. (Leguminosae) based on chloroplast Lin, J. Y. 2004. Oviposition behavior and trnK/matK sequences and its its impact on interspecific competition implications for evolutionary patterns in (L.) and in Papilonoideae. Am. J. Bot. 87: C. maculatus (F.). Ph. D. dissertation. 418-430. National University, 145 pp. Hu, T. 1989. Effects of gamma radiation (in Chinese) on the cowpea weevil (Callosobruchus Manly, B. F. J., P. Miller, and L. M. Cool. maculatus (Fab.)). Ph. D. dissertation. 1972. Analysis of a selective predation National Taiwan University, 127 pp. experiment. Am. Nat. 106: 719-736. (in Chinese) Manning, A. 1967. Pre-imaginal conditioning Hu, W. T., Y. C. Lan, and S. B. Horng. 1995. in Drosophila. Nature 216: 338-340. Effects of bean size on larval Mitchell, R. 1975. The evolution of competition and oviposition preference oviposition tactics in the bean weevil, of Callosobruchus maculatus. Chinese Callosobruchus maculatus. Ecology J. Entomol. 15: 321-332. (in Chinese) 56: 696-702. Jaenike, J. 1983. Induction of host Mitchell, R. 1990. Behavioral ecology of preference in Drosophila melanogaster. Callosoruchus maculatus. pp. 317-330. Oecologia 58: 320-325. In: K. Fujii, M. R. Gatehouse, C. D. Jaenike, J. 1988. Effects of early adult Johnson, R. Mitchell, and T. Yoshida, experience on host selection in insects: eds. Bruchids and Legumes: Economics, some experimental and theoretical Ecology, and Coevolution. Kluwer results. J. Insect Behav. 1: 3-15. Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Jaisson, P. 1980. Environmental preference Netherlands. induced experimentally in ants Mitchell, R., and C. Thanthianga. 1990. (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Nature Are the oviposition traits of the 286: 388-389. South strain of Callosobruchus Kester, K. M., and P. Barbosa. 1991. maculatus maintained by natural

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 23 selection? Entomol. Exp. Appl. 57: Not Behave Normally. Chapman and 143-150. Hall, London. Papaj, D. R. 1986. Interpopulation Shiau, Y. S., L. Y. L. Lai, and S. B. Horng. differences in host preference and the 1994. Effects of oviposition behavior evolution of learning in the butterfly, on host preference of Callosobruchus Battus philenor. Evolution 40: 518- maculatus. Chinese J. Entomol. 14: 530. 245-253. (in Chinese) Phillips, W. M. 1977. Modification of Smith, M. A., and Cornell, H. V. 1979. feeding ‘preference’ in the flea beetle Hopkins host-selection in Nasonia Haltica lythri (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae). vitripennis and its implications for Entomol. Exp. Appl. 21: 71-80. sympatric speciation. Anim. Behav. Ray, S. 1999. Survival of olfactory 27: 365-370. memory through metamorphosis in Szentesi, A., and T. Jermy. 1990. The role the fly Musca domestica. Neurosci. of experience in host plant choice by Lett. 259: 37-40. phytophagous insects. pp. 39-74. In: Rietdorf, K., and J. L. M. Steidle. 2002. E. A. Bernays, ed. Insect-Plant Was Hopkins right? Influence of Interactions, Vol. II. CRC Press, Boca larval and early adult experience on Raton, Florida. the olfactory response in the granary Technau, G., and M. Heisenberg. 1982. weevil Sitophilus granaries (Coleoptera, Neural reorganisation during metamorphosis Curculionidae). Physiol. Entomol. 27: of the corpora pendunculata in 223-227. Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 295: Rojas, J. C., and T. D. Wyatt. 1999. The 405-407. role of pre- and post-imaginal experience Thanthianga, C., and R. Mitchell. 1990. in the host-finding and oviposition The fecundity and oviposition behavior behaviour of the cabbage moth. of south Indian strain Callosobruchus Physiol. Entomol. 24: 83-89. maculatus. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 57: SAS Institute. 1990. SAS/STAT User’s 133-142. Guide. Version 6, 4th edition. SAS Thrope, W. H. 1930. Biological races in Institute, Cary, NC, USA. insects and allied groups. Biol. Rev. 5: Schoonhoven, L. M., T. Jermy, and J. J. 177-212. van Loon. 1998. Insect–plant biology: Tissot, M., N. Gendre, A. Hawken, K. F. from physiology to evolution. pp. Störtkuhl, and R. F. Stoker. 1997. 195-225. In: L. M. Schoonhoven, T. Larval chemosensory projections and Jermy, and J. J. van Loon, eds. invasion of adult afferents in the Host-plant Selection: Why Insect Do antennal lobe of Drosophila. J.

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 24 Neurobiol. 32: 281-297. Visser, M. E. 1994. The importance of Tissot, M., and R. F. Stocker. 2000. being large: the relationship between Metamorphosis in Drosophila and size and fitness of the parasitoid other insects: the fate of neurons Aphaereta minuta (Hymenoptera: throughout the stages. Progr. Neurobiol. Braconidae). J. Anim. Ecol. 63: 963- 62: 89-111. 978. Truman, J. W. 1990. Metamorphosis of the Wiklund, C. 1973. Host plant suitability CNS of Drosophila. J. Neurobiol. 21: and the mechanism of host selection 1072-1084. in larvae of Papilio machaon. Tully, T., V. Cambiazo, and L. Kruse. 1994. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 16: 232-242. Memory through metamorphosis in Yang, R. L., and S. B. Horng. 2002. Host normal and mutant Drosophila. J. size discrimination and oviposition Neurosci. 14: 68-74. behavior of the seed beetle, Vet, L. E. M., W. J. Lewis, and R. T. Cardé. Callosobruchus maculatus (F.). Formosan 1995. Parasitoid foraging and learning. Entomol. 22: 343-357. (in Chinese) pp. 65-101. In: R. T. Cardé, ed. Chemical Ecology of Insects 2. ԝӇР෈Ȉ2005 ԑ 9 Т 27 Р Р෈Ȉ2006 ԑ 1 Т 18 РڧChapman and Hall, New York. ௥

̙Тᖪഇ૙͹གྷរ၆யӉઐр̝үϡ 25 Effects of Host Experience During Different Stages on Oviposition Preference of Callosobruchus maculatus (F.) (Coleoptera: Bruchidae)

Shiau-Min Dai, Rou-Ling Yang and Shwu-Bin Horng* Institute of Entomology, National Taiwan University, 1 Roosevelt Rd., Sec. 4, Taipei 10617, Taiwan

ABSTRACT

The seed beetle, Callosobruchus maculatus (F.) develops to the adult stage inside seeds before adult emergence, thus the larva or adult has an opportunity to obtain information about host traits, and this experience may influence the host preference of the adult. In this study, adults that had emerged from adzuki and mung beans were used to test whether larval or early adult experience effects host preference of the adult. In no-choice tests of host acceptance there was no significant difference in acceptance of adzuki bean, mung bean or black soybean by beetles reared on adzuki bean or mung bean. In free choice tests of host preference, adults exhibited significant preference for adzuki beans over mung beans, adzuki beans over black soybeans, and black soybeans over mung beans. However, host preferences did not differ significantly between adults with different experiences (e.g., reared on adzuki bean or mung bean, or emerged from adzuki and mung bean powder). These results indicate that larval or early adult host experience does not influence adult host preference. In addition, we provide evidence that the host ratio and the size of hosts affect host preference of the adult. When either big mung bean and small adzuki bean or normal sizes of each bean were provided in a free choice test, adults laid more eggs on adzuki bean than mung bean in both treatments, but the number of eggs laid on big mung bean was greater than on normal sized mung bean. These findings show that the host is a more important factor affecting host preference by the seed beetle than host size.

Key words: host experience, host size, oviposition preference, host proportion

ௐ˘ഇס̱˩˟ᖪௐٿέ៉ 26