Language Development Brian Macwhinney

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Language Development Brian Macwhinney P1: HEF/FGC P2: HEF/FGC QC: HEF/FGC T1: HEF 0521651174c42.xml CU1767B-Hopkins November 19, 2004 21:58 Language development brian macwhinney Introduction hard-wired instinct and the view of language as an emergent process, let us review a few basic facts about Almost every human child succeeds in learning the developmental course of language acquisition and language. As a result, people often tend to take the some of the methods used to study it. process of language learning for granted. To many, language seems like a basic instinct, as simple as breathing or blinking. But, in fact, language is the most Early auditory development complex ability that a human being will ever master. The fact that all people succeed in learning to use William James (1890) described the world of the language, whereas not all people learn to swim or do newborn as a “blooming, buzzing confusion.” However, calculus, demonstrates how fully language conforms we now know that, at the auditory level at least, the to our human nature. In a very real sense, language is newborn’s world is remarkably well structured. The the complete expression of what it means to be cochlea and auditory nerve provide extensive pre- human. processing of signals for frequency and intensity. In the Linguists in the Chomskyan tradition (Pinker, 1994) 1970s and 1980s, researchers discovered that human tend to think of language as having a universal core from infants were specifically adapted at birth to perceive which individual languages select out a particular contrasts such as that between /p/ and /b/, as in pit and configuration of optional features known technically as bit.Subsequent research showed that even chinchillas ‘parameters’ (Chomsky, 1982). As a result, they see are capable of making this distinction. This suggests that language as an instinct driven by specifically human much of the basic structure of the infant’s auditory evolutionary adaptations. In their view, language resides world can be attributed to fundamental processes in the in a unique mental organ that has been given as a Special mammalian ear and cochlear nucleus. Beyond this basic Gift to the human species. This mental organ contains level of auditory processing, it appears that infants have rules, constraints, and other structures that can be aremarkable capacity to record and store sequences of specified by linguistic analysis. Without guidance from auditory events. It is as if the infant has something akin this universal core, the child would be unable to piece toatape recorder in the auditory cortex that records together the intricate details of language structure. input sounds, replays them, and accustoms the ear to Many psychologists (Fletcher & MacWhinney, 1995) their patterns, well before learning the actual meanings and linguists who reject the Chomskyan approach view of these words (Fig. 1). language learning from a very different perspective. To One clever method for studying early audition relies the psychologist, language development is a window on on the fact that babies tend to habituate to repeated the operation of the human mind. The patterns of stimuli from the same perceptual class. If the perceptual language emerge not from a unique instinct, but from class of the stimulus suddenly changes, the baby will the operation of general processes of evolution, brighten up and turn to look at the new stimulus. If the cognition, social processes, and facts about the human experimenter constructs a set of words which share a body. For researchers who accept this emergentist certain property and then shifts to words that have a approach, the goal of language acquisition studies is to different property, the infant may demonstrate understand how regularities in linguistic form emerge awareness of the distinction through preferential looking from the operation of low-level physical, neural, and (Fig. 2). For example, a baby may slowly habituate to a social processes. Before considering the current state of long string of syllables that have the form ABA, such as the dialogue between the view of language as a badaba-nopano-rinori-punapu.Ifthe string then shifts 257 P1: HEF/FGC P2: HEF/FGC QC: HEF/FGC T1: HEF 0521651174c42.xml CU1767B-Hopkins November 19, 2004 21:58 258 Domains of development: from infancy to adolescence Pinna Semicircular canals Malleus Oval Incus window Cochelea Auditory nerve Stapes Eardrum Auditory canal Round window Figure 1. The human peripheral auditory apparatus. to an ABB structure, as in badada-rinono-satoto-punana, (Fig. 3). By 6 months, the infant is producing structured the infant will perk up and show increased attention to vocalizations, including a larger diversity of individual the new string. vowels and consonants, mostly structured into the shape Infants also demonstrate preferences for the language of the consonant-vowel (CV) syllables like ta or pe.The that resembles the speech of their mothers. Thus, a basic framework of early babbling is built on top of French infant will prefer to listen to French, whereas a patterns of noisy lip-smacking that are present in many Polish infant will prefer to listen to Polish. In addition, primates. These CV vocal gestures include some form they demonstrate a preference for their own mother’s of vocal closure followed by a release with vocalic voice, as opposed to that of other women. Together, resonance. these abilities and preferences suggest that, during the Until the sixth month, deaf infants babble much like first eight months, the child is remarkably attentive to hearing children. However, well before 9 months, deaf language. Although babies are not yet learning words, infants lose their interest in babbling. This suggests that they are acquiring the basic auditory and intonational their earlier babbling is sustained largely through patterns of their native language. As they sharpen their proprioceptive and somaesthetic feedback, as the baby ability to hear the contrasts of their native language, they explores the various ways in which she can play with her begin to lose the ability to hear contrasts not represented mouth. After 6 months, babbling relies increasingly on in that language. If the child is growing up in a bilingual auditory feedback. During this period, the infant tries to world, full perceptual flexibility is maintained. However, produce specific sounds to match up with specific when growing up as a monolingual, flexibility in auditory impressions. It is at this point that the deaf processing is gradually traded off for quickness and child no longer finds babbling entertaining, since it is automaticity. not linked to auditory feedback. These facts suggest that, from the infant’s point of view, babbling is essentially a process of exploring the coordinated use of the mouth, Early articulation lungs, and larynx. In the heyday of behaviorism, researchers viewed the During the first three months, babies produce cries that development of babbling in terms of reinforcement express hunger, distress, and sometimes pain. By theory. They thought that the reinforcing qualities of 3months, at the time of the first social smile, they begin language would lead a Chinese baby to babble the sounds to make the delightful little sounds that we call ‘cooing’ of Chinese, whereas a Quechua baby would babble the P1: HEF/FGC P2: HEF/FGC QC: HEF/FGC T1: HEF 0521651174c42.xml CU1767B-Hopkins November 19, 2004 21:58 Language development 259 sounds of Quechua. This was the theory of ‘babbling drift.’ However, closer observation has indicated that no drift toward the native language occurs until well after 9months. By 12 months, there is some slight drift in the direction of the native language, as the infant begins to acquire the first words. Proponents of universal phono- logy have sometimes suggested that all children engage in babbling all the sounds of all the world’s language. Here,again, the claim seems to be overstated. Although it is certainly true that some English-learning infants will produce Bantu clicks and Quechua implosives, not all children produce all of these sounds. The first words The emergence of the first word is based on three earlier developments. The first is the infant’s growing ability to record the sounds of words. The second is the develop- ment of an ability to control vocal productions that occurs in the late stages of babbling. The third is the general growth of the symbolic function, as represented in play, imitation, and object manipulation. Piaget (1954) characterized the infant’s cognitive development in terms of the growth of representation or the ‘object concept.’ In the first six months of life, the child is unable to think about objects that are not physically present. However, a 12-month-old will see a dog’s tail sticking out from behind a chair and realize that the rest of the dog is hiding behind the chair. This Figure 2. The preferential looking task in which changes in infant understanding of how parts relate to wholes supports gaze signal discrimination of differences in words. From J. Berko and the child’s first major use of the symbolic function. N. Bernstein Ratner, 1993. Psycholinguistics.Fort Worth: Harcourt When playing with toys, the 12-month-old will begin to Brace Jovanovich. produce sounds such as vroom or bam-bam that represent properties of these toys and actions. Often these phonologically consistent forms appear before the first real words. Because they have no clear conventional status, parents may tend to ignore these first symbolic attempts as nothing more than spurious productions or babbling. Even before producing the first conventional word, the 12-month-old has already acquired an ability to (a) comprehend as many as ten conventional forms. The infant learns these forms through frequent associations between actions, objects, and words. Parents often Frequency in cycles per second realize that the pre-linguistic infant is beginning to understand what they say.
Recommended publications
  • 19 First Language Acquisition
    466 Brian MacWhinney 19 First Language Acquisition BRIAN MACWHINNEY Nothing can make a parent prouder than a child’s first word. Whether it be “Mama,” “Papa,” or even “kitty,” we all recognize the first word as a major milestone in the child’s development – a clear token of the child’s entrance into a fuller membership in human society. We emphasize the child’s first word, because of the enormous importance that we place on language as a way of communicating with other people After all, we reason, the only species that uses language is the human species. Moreover, it is through language that we come to understand the deepest secrets of other people and come to appreciate the extent to which we share a common humanity. Fortunately, the ability to acquire language is present in almost every human child (Lenneberg 1967). Children who are born blind have few problems learning to speak, although they may occasionally be confused about words for colors or geographic locations. Children who are born deaf readily acquire a rich system of signs, as long as they are exposed to native sign language speakers. Even a child like Helen Keller, who has lost both hearing and sight, can still acquire language through symbols expressed in touch and motion. Children with neurological disorders, such as brain lesions or hydrocephalus, often acquire complete control over spoken language, despite a few months of early delay. Children with the most extreme forms of mental retardation are still able to acquire the basic units of human communication. Given the pervas- iveness and inevitability of first language acquisition, we often tend to take the process of language learning for granted.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction: Reconciling Approaches to Intra-Individual Variation in Psycholinguistics and Variationist Sociolinguistics
    Linguistics Vanguard 2021; 7(s2): 20200027 Lars Bülow* and Simone E. Pfenninger Introduction: Reconciling approaches to intra-individual variation in psycholinguistics and variationist sociolinguistics https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2020-0027 Abstract: The overall theme of this special issue is intra-individual variation, that is, the observable variation within individuals’ behaviour, which plays an important role in the humanities area as well as in the social sciences. While various fields have recognised the complexity and dynamism of human thought and behav- iour, intra-individual variation has received less attention in regard to language acquisition, use and change. Linguistic research so far lacks both empirical and theoretical work that provides detailed information on the occurrence of intra-individual variation, the reasons for its occurrence and its consequences for language development as well as for language variation and change. The current issue brings together two sub- disciplines – psycholinguistics and variationist sociolinguistics – in juxtaposing systematic and non- systematic intra-individual variation, thereby attempting to build a cross-fertilisation relationship between two disciplines that have had surprisingly little connection so far. In so doing, we address critical stock-taking, meaningful theorizing and methodological innovation. Keywords: psycholinguistics, variationist sociolinguistics, intra-individual variation, intra-speaker variation, SLA, language variation and change, language development 1 Intra-individual variation in psycholinguistics and variationist sociolinguistics The overall theme of this special issue is intra-individual variation, that is, the observable variation within individuals’ behaviour, which plays an important role in the humanities area as well as in the social sciences. While various fields have acknowledged the complexity and dynamism of human behaviour, intra-individual variation has received less attention in regard to language use.
    [Show full text]
  • Part I: Perspectives on Usage in L2 Learning and Teaching
    Part I: Perspectives on usage in L2 learning and teaching Brian MacWhinney Multidimensional SLA Complex natural phenomena, such as human language, are shaped by processes operating on very different scales in both time and space (MacWhinney, in press). Consider the case of timescales in Geology. When geologists study rock outcrops they need to consider the results of general processes such as vulcanism, orogeny, glaciation, continental drift, erosion, sedimentation, and metamorphism. Within each of these larger processes, such as vulcanism, there are many microprocesses operating across smaller timescales. For example, once the pressure in the magma chamber reaches a certain level, there can be a slow outpouring of lava or sudden explosions. Pressure can be released through steam vents with geysers operating at regular intervals. The lava may enter lakes or oceans forming pillows or it may rest in underground chambers forming columnar basalt. The variations in these volcanic processes and their interactions with each other and plate tectonics are extensive. The same is true of human language. Within human populations, the ability to articulate and process sounds has emerged across millennia of ongoing changes in physiology and neurology. Within particular language communities, ongoing change is driven by language contact, dialect shift, and group formation. Within individuals, language learning involves a continual adaptation for both first and second languages. Within individual conversations, all of these forces come together, as people work out their mutual plans, goals, and disagreements, using language. Each of these space-time frames interacts with the others at the actual moment of language use. To fully understand the process of second language acquisition (SLA), we must place it within this multidimensional context, both theoretically and prac- tically.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Learning Research at the Intersection of Experimental, Computational and Corpus-Based Approaches Patrick Rebuschat
    Language learning research at the intersection of experimental, computational and corpus-based approaches (1,2) (2,3) (1,4) Patrick Rebuschat ,​ Detmar Meurers ,​ and Tony McEnery ​ ​ ​ (1) (2) Department​ of Linguistics and English Language, Lancaster University; LEAD​ Graduate ​ (3) School and Research Network, University of Tübingen; Seminar​ für Sprachwissenschaft, ​ ​ ​ (4) University of Tübingen; ESRC​ Centre for Corpus Approaches to Social Science ​ Keywords: Psycholinguistics; corpus linguistics; computational linguistics; natural language processing; first language acquisition; second language acquisition; multimethod approaches; interdisciplinary research Acknowledgements: Our research was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council, UK (grant ES/K002155/1) and by the LEAD Graduate School & Research Network (grant DFG-GSC1028), a project of the Excellence Initiative of the German federal and state governments. Correspondence: Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patrick Rebuschat, Department of Linguistics and English Language, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YL, United Kingdom, E-mail: [email protected]. Language acquisition occupies a central place in the study of human cognition, and research on how we learn language can be found across many disciplines, from developmental psychology and linguistics to education, philosophy and neuroscience. It is a very challenging topic to investigate given that the learning target in first and second language acquisition is highly complex, and part of the challenge consists in identifying how different domains of language are acquired to form a fully functioning system of usage (Ellis, this volume). Correspondingly, the evidence about language use and language learning is generally shaped by many factors, including the characteristics of the task in which the language is produced (Alexopoulou et al, this volume).
    [Show full text]
  • Preprint Ellis, NC (2013). Second Language Acquisition. in Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar
    Second Language Acquisition Nick C. Ellis University of Michigan [email protected] preprint Ellis, N. C. (2013). Second language acquisition. In Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 365-378), G. Trousdale & T. Hoffmann (Eds.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 1. Introduction Usage-based approaches hold that we learn linguistic constructions while engaging in communication, the “interpersonal communicative and cognitive processes that everywhere and always shape language” (Slobin 1997). Constructions are form-meaning mappings, conventionalized in the speech community, and entrenched as language knowledge in the learner’s mind. They are the symbolic units of language relating the defining properties of their morphological, syntactic, and lexical form with particular semantic, pragmatic, and discourse functions (Bates and MacWhinney 1987; Lakoff 1987; Langacker 1987; Croft 2001; Croft and Cruse 2004; Goldberg 1995, 2003, 2006; Tomasello 2003; Robinson and Ellis 2008; Bybee 2008). Broadly, Construction Grammar argues that all grammatical phenomena can be understood as learned pairings of form (from morphemes, words, and idioms, to partially lexically filled and fully general phrasal patterns) and their associated semantic or discourse functions. Such beliefs, increasingly influential in the study of child language acquisition, have turned upside down generative assumptions of innate language acquisition devices, the continuity hypothesis, and top-down, rule-governed, processing, bringing back data-driven, emergent accounts of linguistic systematicities. Constructionist theories of child first language acquisition (L1A) use dense longitudinal corpora to chart the emergence of creative linguistic competence from children’s analyses of the utterances in their usage history and from their abstraction of regularities within them (Goldberg 1995, 2006, 2003; Diessel, this volume; Tomasello 1998; Tomasello 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Apt Ratner Cv May 2020.Pdf
    Curriculum Vitae Notarization. I have read the following and certify that this curriculum vitae is a current and accurate statement of my professional record. Signature____________________ Date: May 2020__________ Personal Information A. UID nratner, Ratner, Nan Bernstein 0141GG Lefrak Hall, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742 Mailing address: 0100 Lefrak Hall 301-405-4217, 301-314-2023 (fax) [email protected] https://hesp.umd.edu/facultyprofile/Bernstein%20Ratner/Nan ORCID iD 0000-0002-9947-0656; link to my public record: http://orcid.org/0000-0002- 9947-0656 Google Scholar profile: https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C21&q=Nan+Bernstein+Ratner& btnG= B. Academic Appointments at UMD Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Assistant Professor (1983-1989) Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Associate Professor (1989-1999) Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Professor (1999 – present) (1993 - 2014) C. Administrative Appointments at UMD Chairman, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences (1993-2014) D. Other employment 1982 - 1983 Howard University Visiting Assistant Professor, Department of Communication Sciences 1978 - 1982 Boston College Instructor, Department of Special Education 1978 - 1982 University of Massachusetts, Boston (formerly Boston State College) Instructor, Depts. of Psychology and Special Education 1980 - 1982 Bridgewater State College, MA Instructor, Department of Communication Disorders 1977 - 1981 Tufts University Director, Forensics Program Intercollegiate Varsity Debate Coach Instructor, Experimental College 1977 - 1978 Child Development Center, Brockton, MA Director, Speech and language services 1977 - 1978 Southeast Massachusetts Head Start, Brockton, MA Director, Speech and language services E. Educational Background Boston University 1982 Ed.D. Applied Psycholinguistics Temple University 1976 M.A.
    [Show full text]
  • Bilingual Sentence Production and Code-Switching: Neural Network Simulations
    Bilingual sentence production and code-switching: Neural network simulations Chara Tsoukala 556869-L-bw-Tsoukala Processed on: 9-3-2021 PDF page: 1 The educational component of the doctoral training was provided by the International Max Planck Research School (IMPRS) for Language Sciences. The graduate school is a joint initiative between the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics and two partner institutes at Radboud University – the Centre for Language Studies, and the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour. The IMPRS curriculum, which is funded by the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, ensures that each member receives interdisciplinary training in the language sciences and develops a well-rounded skill set in preparation for fulfilling careers in academia and beyond. More information can be found at www.mpi.nl/imprs ISBN: 978-94-92910-27-1 Cover design: Amalia Tsichla Printed by: Ipskamp printing Layout: This thesis was typeset with LATEX2ε. It uses the Clean Thesis style developed by Ricardo Langner. © Chara Tsoukala, 2021. All rights reserved. This thesis was funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) Gravitation Grant 024.001.006 to the Language in Interaction Consortium. 556869-L-bw-Tsoukala Processed on: 9-3-2021 PDF page: 2 Bilingual sentence production and code-switching: Neural network simulations Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. dr. J.H.J.M. van Krieken, volgens besluit van het college van decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op woensdag 21 april 2021 om 14.30 uur precies door Chara Tsoukala geboren op 18 april 1986 te Athene (Griekenland) 556869-L-bw-Tsoukala Processed on: 9-3-2021 PDF page: 3 Promotor Prof.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Linguistic Form in Time
    Connection Science Vol. 17, No. 3–4, September–December 2005, 191–211 The emergence of linguistic form in time BRIAN MACWHINNEY*† †Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA Linguistic forms are shaped by forces operating on vastly different time scales. Some of these forces operate directly at the moment of speaking, whereas others accumulate over time in personal and social memory. Our challenge is to understand how forces with very different time scales mesh together in the current moment to determine the emergence of linguistic form. Keywords: Emergence; Word learning; Syntax; Phylogeny; Ontogeny; Resonance 1. Introduction Where do linguistic forms come from? Researchers have proposed accounts based on rules, conventions, constraints, statistical regularities, discourse patterns, language contact, neuro- logy, physical dynamics and genes. What is common to all of these forces is that they must all make their final impact at the moment of speaking. Some of these forces operate within a narrow time frame. For example, when we run up a flight of stairs, there will be an immediate impact on vocal production leading to loss of weak syllables and changes in pitch. This type of exertion has a quick and immediate effect on prosodic and phonetic form. Other processes have a slower time fuse. For example, we may not have made reference to some concept, such as ‘apotheosis’ or ‘ligature’ for years. Yet, when we need to use these words, we can dredge them up out of our memories and apply them in the moment. The effects of Alzheimer’s disease on language can work across decades, leading eventually to an inability to remember even the most common words.
    [Show full text]
  • Bifurcations and the Emergence of L2 Syntactic Structures in a Complex Dynamic System
    University of Massachusetts Medical School eScholarship@UMMS Open Access Articles Open Access Publications by UMMS Authors 2020-10-29 Bifurcations and the Emergence of L2 Syntactic Structures in a Complex Dynamic System D. Reid Evans University of Massachusetts Medical School Et al. Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Follow this and additional works at: https://escholarship.umassmed.edu/oapubs Part of the First and Second Language Acquisition Commons, and the Psychology Commons Repository Citation Evans DR, Larsen-Freeman D. (2020). Bifurcations and the Emergence of L2 Syntactic Structures in a Complex Dynamic System. Open Access Articles. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574603. Retrieved from https://escholarship.umassmed.edu/oapubs/4408 Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License. This material is brought to you by eScholarship@UMMS. It has been accepted for inclusion in Open Access Articles by an authorized administrator of eScholarship@UMMS. For more information, please contact [email protected]. fpsyg-11-574603 October 23, 2020 Time: 19:2 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 29 October 2020 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574603 Bifurcations and the Emergence of L2 Syntactic Structures in a Complex Dynamic System D. Reid Evans1* and Diane Larsen-Freeman2 1 Office of Graduate Medical Education, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, MA, United States, 2 Department of Linguistics, School of Education, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States We report on a complex dynamic systems study of an untutored adult French learner’s development of English syntax, specifically two non-finite adverbial constructions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics
    The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics This outstanding multi‐volume series covers all the major subdisciplines within linguistics today and, when complete, will offer a comprehensive survey of linguistics as a whole. Recent Titles Include: The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics The Handbook of Korean Linguistics Edited by Manuel Díaz‐Campos Edited by Lucien Brown and Jaehoon Yeon The Handbook of Language Socialization The Handbook of Speech Production Edited by Alessandro Duranti, Elinor Ochs, Edited Melissa A. Redford and Bambi B. Schieffelin The Handbook of Contemporary Semantic Theory, The Handbook of Intercultural Discourse and Second Edition Communication Edited by Shalom Lappin and Chris Fox Edited by Christina Bratt Paulston, Scott F. The Handbook of Classroom Discourse and Kiesling, and Elizabeth S. Rangel Interaction The Handbook of Historical Sociolinguistics Edited by Numa Markee Edited by Juan Manuel Hernández‐Campoy The Handbook of Narrative Analysis and Juan Camilo Conde‐Silvestre Edited by Anna De Fina & Alexandra The Handbook of Hispanic Linguistics Georgakopoulou Edited by José Ignacio Hualde, Antxon The Handbook of English Pronounciation Olarrea, and Erin O’Rourke Edited byMarnie Reed and John M. Levis The Handbook of Conversation Analysis The Handbook of Discourse Analysis, 2nd edition, Edited by Jack Sidnell and Tanya Stivers Edited by Deborah Tannen, Heidi E. The Handbook of English for Specific Purposes Hamilton, & Deborah Schiffrin Edited by Brian Paltridge and Sue Starfield The Handbook of Bilingual and Multilingual The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Education Acquisition Edited by Wayne E. Wright, Sovicheth Boun, Edited by Kimberly L. Geeslin and Ofelia García The Handbook of Chinese Linguistics The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics Edited by C.‐T.
    [Show full text]
  • Transcribing Interlanguage: the Case of Verb-Final [E] in L2 French Pascale Leclercq Université Paul Valéry Montpellier 3
    Chapter 7 Transcribing interlanguage: The case of verb-final [e] in L2 French Pascale Leclercq Université Paul Valéry Montpellier 3 This chapter aims at shedding some light on the place of transcription in the data interpretation process. More specifically, it focuses on the example of verb-final [e] in oral second-language French, which causes interpretation problems when con- text does not provide disambiguation cues. Through an analysis of three studies on this phenomenon (Herschensohn 2001; Prévost 2007b,a; Granget 2015), displaying a variety of theoretical frameworks (generative versus functional) and transcrip- tion options (written- and spoken-centric approaches), I show that transcription choices, whether made intuitively or in a theory-constrained manner, are often problematic as they entail an early categorization of data, even before data coding and analysis, thereby introducing an interpretive bias (Mondada 2007). Finally, I draw conclusions and offer suggestions regarding best transcription practices. Keywords: Transcription, data interpretation, L2 French, verbal morphology, in- terlanguage 1 Introduction This chapter stems from the author’s questions and doubts while engaging in corpus-based second language acquisition (SLA) research, and more specifically when facing oral learner production that is ambiguous and requires the researcher to make conscious transcription decisions. I was particularly puzzled by the way some English-speaking learners of French use verb-final [e] as a generic verb end- ing, in spite of its highly polysemous value (e.g., tomber ‘fall’ infinitive, tombait ‘was falling’ imperfect, tombé ‘fallen’ past participle, etc.) and wondered how to transcribe and interpret such forms. This phenomenon has been pointed out reg- ularly over the last decades in the literature on the acquisition of morphosyntax Pascale Leclercq.
    [Show full text]
  • Gestures and Some Key Issues in the Study of Language Development Gullberg, Marianne; De Bot, Kees; Volterra, Virginia
    Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development Gullberg, Marianne; de Bot, Kees; Volterra, Virginia Published in: Gesture DOI: 10.1075/gest.8.2.03gul 2008 Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Gullberg, M., de Bot, K., & Volterra, V. (2008). Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development. Gesture, 8(2), 149-179. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.8.2.03gul Total number of authors: 3 General rights Unless other specific re-use rights are stated the following general rights apply: Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Read more about Creative commons licenses: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. LUND UNIVERSITY PO Box 117 221 00 Lund +46 46-222 00 00 Running title: Key issues in language development Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development Marianne Gullberg1, Kees de Bot2, & Virginia Volterra3 1 Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 2 Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, 3 Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, CNR In Gesture, 8(2), Special issue Gestures in language development, eds.
    [Show full text]