THERAPY for WORD FINDING in APHASIA Effects on Picture Naming and Conversation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

THERAPY for WORD FINDING in APHASIA Effects on Picture Naming and Conversation THERAPY FOR WORD FINDING IN APHASIA Effects on picture naming and conversation Ruth Elizabeth Herbert University College London Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the degree of PhD UMI Number: U6026B6 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U602636 Published by ProQuest LLC 2014. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 2 ABSTRACT Therapy for word finding deficits in aphasia have taken two forms: semantic and phonological, with relatively more examples of the former in the literature. Criticisms levelled against such therapies focus on the fact that in most reported cases treatment effects are limited to treated items, and there is very little evidence of real functional change in terms of improvement in everyday speech for the person with aphasia. Behaviour in conversation can vary and for this reason it is important to establish reliability and stability of the aspects of conversation under scrutiny. This was carried out in the work reported here in order to identify aspects of conversation which might be used as outcome measures for therapy. The analysis of inter and intra-rater reliability and of test retest stability produced a measure which was used to identify the effects of two forms of therapy. The two forms of therapy were presented consecutively to three people with aphasia. In the first phase phonological and orthographic cues were used. In the second phase participants were encouraged to use the set of treatment words in speech situations, ranging from naming to definition to use in conversation. The effect of each form of therapy on picture naming and on conversation was measured. The results showed a positive effect of the phonological and orthographic cues for two of the participants in terms of gains in picture naming. For the third participant this therapy was ineffective. The second phase of therapy was effective for all three in terms of gains in items only treated in that phase of therapy. The analysis of the conversation data showed unstable baselines for a number of aspects for all three participants. Nevertheless there were some aspects which were stable for a given individual and some evidence of positive changes after therapy. 3 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank most particularly the three people with aphasia, and their significant others, who took part in the study with enthusiasm and interest. It was a real pleasure to work with them and to discuss aphasia and therapy with those who know most about it. In completing the thesis the advice, support, and wisdom of my two supervisors, Wendy Best and Shula Chiat, have been invaluable. Both Wendy and Shula read with enthusiasm the miles of script that came their way. The meetings we had throughout this time were always interesting and stimulating. I regret not insisting on more meals out but there is always tomorrow... While collecting the data for the study I was employed on a grant from the Tavistock Trust for Aphasia. During the data collection period I worked closely with the ideas of Felicity Osborne, and with the physical reality of Wendy Best and Julie Hickin. Those were very fruitful times which I look back on with fondness. Thanks to all three for those happy times. David Howard provided many of the test materials used here, computer programmes to ensure easy data management, and invaluable statistical advice throughout. Thank you. I would like to thank also staff at University College London for help and support. Jane Maxim, for enthusiastically embracing anything new and challenging, Chris Donlan for humour and advice on statistics, Katie Kay and Caroline Newton for being sane and friendly and keeping things in perspective. At Sheffield Martin Szcerbinski offered calm advice that was easy to follow. To all other friends and colleagues who have offered help when needed: Julie (always on the end of the phone when needed), Caroline, Jo, Sarah, Gill, Stephanie for being understanding about the time it has taken and for ongoing friendship. Finally I wish to thank the whole of my extensive and colourful family for being so encouraging throughout, in particular my mum and dad who are very special. 5 LIST OF CONTENTS CHAPTER ONE: SPOKEN WORD PRODUCTION IN APHASIA 21 1.0 Introduction 21 1.0.1 Aims and hypotheses of the research 21 1.0.1.1 Conversation and therapy 21 1.0.1.2 Therapy for word finding deficits 21 1.0.1.3 Relating deficit to therapy outcome 22 1.0.2 Overview of Chapter One 23 1.1 Anomia 23 1.1.1 Word finding in aphasia 23 1.1.2 Aphasia syndromes and anomia 23 1.1.3 Anomia: a cognitive approach to assessment 25 1.2 Methodological issues in aphasia research 26 1.2.1 Assessment methods 26 1.2.2 Issues of validity in testing word finding through picture naming 27 1.2.3 Reliability of methods and analyses in aphasia research 28 1.2.3.1 Age related controls 28 1.2.3.2 Error types and error classification 29 1.2.3.2.1 Whole word errors: semantic relationships 29 1.2.3.2.2 Whole word errors: syntactic relationships 29 1.2.3.2.3 Whole word errors: perseverations 30 1.2.3.2.4 Morphological errors 31 1.2.3.2.5 Formal or phonological errors 31 1.2.3.2.6 Mixed errors 32 1.2.3.2.7 Connected speech: circumlocutions 33 1.2.3.2.8 Summary 33 1.2.3.3 Reporting data 35 1.3 Anomia subtypes 37 1.3.1 Semantic deficits and semantic errors 37 1.3.1.1 Background 37 1.3.1.2 Semantic output errors in aphasia 39 1.3.1.3 Relationship between input and output semantic errors 39 1.3.1.4 Single cases with a lexical semantic deficit 40 1.3.1.5 Semantic errors without a lexical semantic deficit 42 1.3.1.6 Variables and semantic errors 42 1.3.1.7 Comparing targets and semantic errors 43 1.3.1.8 Summary 44 1.3.2 Pure anomia 45 1.3.2.1 Classical anomic aphasia 45 1.3.2.2 Pure anomia: a breakdown between semantics and 45 phonology 1.3.2.3 Tip of the tongue states in aphasia 46 1.3.3 Phonological deficits 47 1.3.3.1 Classical accounts of phonological impairment 47 1.3.3.2 Single case studies 47 1.3.4 Summary 49 1.4 Variables influencing naming success 50 1.4.1 Semantic variables 50 1.4.1.1 Imageability/Concreteness 50 1.4.1.3 Operativity 51 6 1.4.2 Lexical variables 52 1.4.2.1 Frequency 52 1.4.2.2 Age of acquisition 53 1.4.2.3 Familiarity 54 1.4.3 Variables acting upon phonological encoding 54 1.4.3.1 Syllable and phoneme length 54 1.4.3.2 Group studies and single case studies 55 1.4.4 Summary 56 1.5 Summary of the Chapter 56 CHAPTER TWO: MODELS OF SPOKEN WORD PRODUCTION 57 2.0 Introduction 57 2.1 Methodology 57 2.1.1 Speech errors 57 2.1.2 Reaction time experiments 58 2.1.3 Cognitive neuropsychology 58 2.1.4 Connectionism 58 2.2 Theories of spoken word production 59 2.2.1 Architecture: Boxes, arrows, units and interconnections 59 2.2.2 Architecture: Semantic and phonological levels 59 2.2.3 Evidence for independent levels: semantics and phonology 60 2.2.3.1 Speech errors in normal speakers 60 2.2.3.2 Tip of the tongue states 62 2.2.3.3 Reaction time experiments 62 2.2.3.4 Evidence from aphasia 63 2.2.4 One-step models 64 2.2.5 Two-step models 64 2.2.5.1 Lemma, or word level 64 2.2.5.2 The independence of syntax (lemma) and phonology: 65 tip of the tongue studies 2.2.5.3 The independence of syntax (lemma) and phonology: 66 Evidence from neuropsychological studies 2.2.5.4 The organisation of syntactic and phonological 67 knowledge 2.2.6 Activation within one and two step models 67 2.2.7 Four models of spoken word production 68 2.2.7.1 WEAVER ++ (Levelt, Roelofs and Meyer, 1999) 68 22.12 Dell, Schwartz et al (1997): Interactive two-step model 70 2.2.7.3 Rapp and Goldrick’s Restricted interactive activation 72 (RIA) model 2.2.7.4 Caramazza’s (1997) Independent Network model 73 2.3 Levels of representation 75 2.3.1 Semantics 75 2.3.1.1 Two systems or one? 75 2.3.1.2 Semantic representations 76 2.3.2 The lemma or word level 76 2.3.3 Morphological representations 77 2.4 Phonological encoding 77 2.4.1 Slot and filler model 77 2.4.2 Serial ordering in phonological encoding 78 2.4.3 Temporal aspects of phonological encoding 78 2.5 Models of spoken word production and aphasic naming errors 79 2.5.1 Background 79 2.5.2 Non-implemented explanations 79 2.5.2.1 Semantic errors in input and output 79 2.5.2.2 Output semantic errors without input semantic errors 80 2.5.2.3 Evidence for lemma level deficit 81 2.5.2.4 Form related errors 81 2.5.3 Computerised simulations of aphasicnaming 82 2.5.3.1 Dell et al (1997) 82 2.5.3.1.1 Problems with this simulation 84 2.5.3.2 Foygel & Dell’s (2000) semantic phonological model 86 2.6 Summary 87 CHAPTER THREE: SPOKEN WORD PRODUCTION 88 IN CONVERSATION 3.0 Introduction 88 3.1 Background 88 3.2 Conversation analysis 89 3.2.1 Sequentiality 90 3.2.2 Turn taking 91 3.2.2.1 Turn construction units and transition relevance points 91 3.2.2.2 Types of turns 93 3.2.3 Repair 94 33 Conversation analysis and aphasia 96 3.3.1 Turn taking in aphasia 96 3.3.2 Aphasia and repair 98 3.3.2.1 Causes of breakdown in aphasic conversation 100 3.3.2.2 The nature of repair in aphasic conversation 101 3.3.2.3 Influence of partner 102
Recommended publications
  • L1 Attrition and the Mental Lexicon Monika Schmid, Barbara Köpke
    L1 attrition and the mental lexicon Monika Schmid, Barbara Köpke To cite this version: Monika Schmid, Barbara Köpke. L1 attrition and the mental lexicon. Pavlenko, Aneta. The Bilin- gual Mental Lexicon. Interdisciplinary approaches, Multilingual Matters, pp.209-238, 2009, Bilingual Education & Bilingualism, 978-1-84769-124-8. hal-00981094 HAL Id: hal-00981094 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00981094 Submitted on 20 Apr 2014 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Chapter 9 L1 attrition and the mental lexicon Monika S. Schmid, Rijksuniversiteit, Groningen & Barbara Köpke, Université de Toulouse – Le Mirail Introduction The bilingual mental lexicon is one of the most thoroughly studied domains within investigations of bilingualism. Psycholinguistic research has focused mostly on its organization or functional architecture, as well as on lexical access or retrieval procedures (see also Meuter, this volume). The dynamics of the bilingual mental lexicon have been investigated mainly in the context of second language acquisition (SLA) and language pathology. Within SLA, an important body of research is devoted to vocabulary learning and teaching (e.g. Bogaards & Laufer, 2004; Ellis, 1994; Hulstijn & Laufer, 2001; Nation, 1990, 1993). In pathology, anomia (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 4 the Mental Lexicon of Multilingual Adult Learners of Italian L3: a Study of Word Association Behavior and Cross-Lingual Semantic Priming
    Chapter 4 The mental lexicon of multilingual adult learners of Italian L3: A study of word association behavior and cross-lingual semantic priming Anna Gudmundson Stockholm University This study, a partial replication of the study conducted by Fitzpatrick & Izura (2011) on bilingual speakers, investigates the structure and processing of the mental lex- icon of multilingual speakers of first language (L1) Swedish, second language (L2) English and third language (L3) Italian in order of acquisition. By way of word association tasks in all three languages, the effect of language status (L1, L2, L3) and association category (i.e., the different kinds of word association responses) on reaction time (RT) and association distribution (i.e., proportion of associations in different association categories) is measured. Results show a significant effect of language status on association distribution and on RT. The present study also investigates the effect of long-term cross-lingual semantic priming and lexical me- diation between L3 and L2 in a lexical decision task (LDT), i.e. if the activation of L3 conceptual information is mediated by the corresponding word form in the L2. The primes in this study are English words whose Italian translation equiva- lents were present in the prior L3 Italian word association task. The translation equivalents obtained shorter RTs compared to control words, which indicates that L2 English words were activated during the L3 Italian word association task. The kind of cross-lingual priming found in the multilinguals investigated in this study would imply that, besides the L1, also an L2 could mediate in lexical processing, and that the L1 does not have a privileged status in that respect.
    [Show full text]
  • Mental Lexicon: a Conceptual Framework
    International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 5, Issue 5, May 2015 1 ISSN 2250-3153 Mental lexicon: A conceptual framework Othman Aref Al-Dala’ien*, Badri Abdulhakim D.M. Mudhsh **, Ayman Hamid Al-Takhayinh ** Department of Linguistics Aligarh Muslim University AMU Aligarh, India Abstract- The paper discussed the concept of mental lexicon Australians, that is, they are stored as entries in the mental or mental dictionary and some other studies related to the area of lexicon, and therefore an Australian has access to mental lexicon. How lexical items are accessed, acquired and the meanings of these words and can consequently comprehend ordered in the mind is a controversial area for both linguists and the sentence. If one possessed no mental psychologists. The mental lexicon is a complicated structure lexicon, communication through language would be precluded." organized in many ways. This paper discusses the notion of (Marcus Taft, 1999). mental lexicon and what are the similarities and contrasts between mental lexicon and physical dictionary, methods and II. REVIW OF LITERATURE ways used for inquiry and the role of mental lexicon in case of In the early Generative Grammar (Chomsky, 1965) the idea learning and acquisition of language as well as an overview on about the lexicon was just that that it is a list of words on which the language and memory. The paper covers research findings in syntax, the central module, operated. But recently, the study of this area. the lexicon has been given more importance and it has received a The study has conducted on the qualitative method that based up reinforcement from the researches of cognitive psycholinguists on observations and collecting material besides the observations (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Lexical Access in Speech Production: the Bilingual Case
    Psicológica ISSN: 0211-2159 [email protected] Universitat de València España Costa, Albert; Caramazza, Alfonso Lexical access in speech production: the bilingual case Psicológica, vol. 21, núm. 2, 2000, pp. 403-437 Universitat de València Valencia, España Disponible en: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16921211 Cómo citar el artículo Número completo Sistema de Información Científica Más información del artículo Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina, el Caribe, España y Portugal Página de la revista en redalyc.org Proyecto académico sin fines de lucro, desarrollado bajo la iniciativa de acceso abierto Psicológica (2000), 21, 403-437 Lexical Access in Speech Production: The Bilingual Case Albert Costa*, Àngels Colomé**and Alfonso Caramazza* * Harvard University **Universitat de Barcelona In this paper we review models of lexical access in speech production in bilingual speakers. We focus on two major aspects of lexical access: a) how lexical selection is achieved, and b) whether lexical access involves cascaded or discrete stages of processing. We start by considering the major assumptions of how lexical access works in monolingual speakers, and then proceed to discuss those assumptions in the context of bilingual speakers. The main theoretical models and the most recent experimental evidence in their favor are described. Key words: Speech Production, Lexical Access, Bilingualism. Speaking involves translating concepts and ideas into patterns of sounds produced by our articulatory organs. During this "translation" process, speakers have to retrieve the appropriate words for conveying the intended message. Furthermore, they must combine these words according to the grammatical properties of the language being spoken. Finally, they have to retrieve information about how to articulate the selected words.
    [Show full text]
  • Words in the Mind: Exploring the Relationship Between Word Association and Lexical Development
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Ritsumeikan Research Repository Words in the Mind: Exploring the relationship between word association and lexical development Peter W. Roux Abstract Vocabulary acquisition in both L1 and L2 has long relied on the principle of associative language learning to help understand and explain the process of lexical acquisition in the mind of the language learner. To date, L2 studies in this area have however delivered somewhat contentious findings. This study seeks to revisit some of the relevant issues and gain some insight into how words are held in the mind – or, more precisely, to understand the relationship between lexical development and the process of word association (WA). To better understand the relationship between lexical semantics and the mental lexicon, this investigation explores and compares the word associations of a group of Japanese English speakers and a group of English native speakers. It attempts to understand how mental links are made between lexical items in the mental lexicon, a process commonly believed to partially support vocabulary acquisition. Initial findings support general trends in this research area, namely that the word-associations of native and foreign speakers differ in some respects. These however, do not occur to the extent that they may constitute a pattern that may distinguish native from foreign speakers. Findings further highlight the importance of lexical semantics and phonological aspects during word learning, while the importance of developing a robust methodology for the analysis of word association are briefly addressed. To conclude, implications for WA research, the mental lexicon and L2 language learning and teaching are discussed.
    [Show full text]
  • Aphasia Characterized by Grammatical Errors, Such As Using the Incorrect Verb Tense, Number Or Gender , Or Incorrect Word Order
    GLOSSARY Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) - Strategies used to assist or replace communication for individuals with communication disorders. Some examples include flash cards, gestures or applications for electronic devices. Agrammatism - A symptom of aphasia characterized by grammatical errors, such as using the incorrect verb tense, number or gender , or incorrect word order. Agnosia - Literally, a “loss of knowing.” For example, visual agnosia is the inability to recognize an object by sight. Akinesia - Very slow or rigid mobility. The loss or absence of voluntary movement. Alzheimer’s disease - A brain disease that primarily affects memory, however the disease progressively affects other cognitive abilities including language, behavior/personality, visual/spatial perception , motor abilities, and ability to carry out daily activities. This is the most common form of dementia in persons over age 65. Alzheimer’s is associated with the proteins tau and beta-amyloid. Allele - Refers to different versions of the same gene. Amyloid - A starch-like protein that in one configuration accumulates and is related to disease. Abnormal accumulations of amyloid in the brain are involved in Alzheimer’s disease, but not FTD. Identifying amyloid in brain imaging (amyloid PET imaging) can differentiate FTD from Alzheimer’s disease. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) - Also known as Lou Gehrig’s disease, is a disease of the nerve cells in the brain and spinal cord that control voluntary muscle movement. ALS is associated with abnormal accumulations of the protein TDP-43 and can be associated with symptoms of FTD. 10-15% of patients with FTD may also develop symptoms of ALS. C9orf 72 gene mutations can cause FTD and ALS.
    [Show full text]
  • The Multiplex Structure of the Mental Lexicon Influences Picture Naming In
    The multiplex structure of the mental lexicon influences picture naming in people with aphasia Nichol Castro1,+ and Massimo Stella2,3,+ 1School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, USA 2Institute for Complex Systems Simulation, University of Southampton, UK 3Complex Science Consulting, Italy *Corresponding authors: [email protected], [email protected] +These authors contributed equally to this work. October 17, 2018 Abstract An emerging area of research in cognitive science is the utilization of networks to model the structure and processes of the mental lexicon in healthy and clinical populations, like aphasia. Previous research has focused on only one type of word similarity at a time (e.g., semantic relationships), even though words are multi-faceted. Here, we investigate lexical retrieval in a picture naming task from people with Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia and healthy controls by utilizing a multiplex network structure that accounts for the interplay between multiple semantic and phonological relationships among words in the mental lexicon. Ex- tending upon previous work, we focused on the global network measure of closeness centrality which is known to capture spreading activation, an important process supporting lexical retrieval. We conducted a series of logistic regression models predicting the probability of correct picture naming. We tested whether multiplex closeness centrality was a better predictor of picture naming performance than single-layer closeness central- ities, other network measures assessing local and meso-scale structure, psycholinguistic variables, and group differences. We also examined production gaps, or the difference between the likelihood of producing a word with the lowest and highest closeness centralities.
    [Show full text]
  • An Overview on Primary Progressive Aphasia and Its Variants
    1 TITLE An Overview on Primary Progressive Aphasia and its variants Serena Amici a,d *M.D., Maria Luisa Gorno -Tempini a, M.D., PhD, Jennifer M. Ogar a,b , M.S., Nina F. Dronkers a,b,c , PhD, Bruce L. Miller a, M.D. aMemory and Aging Center, Department of Ne urology , University of California, San Francisco, USA bVA Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA, USA cUniversity of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA dDepartment of Neurosciences, University of Perugia, Perugia, ITALY *Corresponding author: Serena Amici 350 Parnassus Avenue, San Francisco, CA 94143 -1207 (415) 476 3572 Keywords : primary progressive aphasia, nonfluent progressive aphasia, semantic dementia, logopenic progressive aphasia, neuroimaging studies, language symptoms Words: 415 6 Tables : 2 Figures: 1 Reference s: 64 2 Abstract We present a review of the literature on Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA) together with the analysis of neuropschychological and neuroradiologic profile s of 42 PPA patients. Mesulam originally defined PPA as a progressive degenerative disorder characterized by isolated language impairment for at least two years. The most common variants of PPA are: 1) Progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA), 2) semantic dementia (SD), 3) logopenic progressive aphasia (LPA). PNFA is characterized by labored speech, agrammatism in production, and/or comprehension . In some cases the syndrome begins with isolated deficits in speech. SD patients typically present with loss of word and object meaning and surface dyslexia. LPA patients have word -finding difficulties, syntactically simple but accurate language output and impaired sentence comprehension. The neuropsychological data demonstrated that SD patients show the most characteristic pattern of impairment, while PNFA and LPA overlap with in many cognitive domains.
    [Show full text]
  • An Overview on Primary Progressive Aphasia and Its Variants
    Behavioural Neurology 17 (2006) 77–87 77 IOS Press An overview on Primary Progressive Aphasia and its variants Serena Amicia,d,∗, Maria Luisa Gorno-Tempinia, Jennifer M. Ogara,b, Nina F. Dronkersa,b,c and Bruce L. Millera aMemory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, USA bVA Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA, USA cUniversity of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA dDepartment of Neurosciences, University of Perugia, Perugia, Italy Abstract. We present a review of the literature on Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA) together with the analysis of neurop- schychological and neuroradiologic profiles of 42 PPA patients. Mesulam originally defined PPA as a progressive degenerative disorder characterized by isolated language impairment for at least two years. The most common variants of PPA are: 1) Progres- sive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA), 2) semantic dementia (SD), 3) logopenic progressive aphasia (LPA). PNFA is characterized by labored speech, agrammatism in production, and/or comprehension. In some cases the syndrome begins with isolated deficits in speech. SD patients typically present with loss of word and object meaning and surface dyslexia. LPA patients have word-finding difficulties, syntactically simple but accurate language output and impaired sentence comprehension. The neuropsychological data demonstrated that SD patients show the most characteristic pattern of impairment, while PNFA and LPA overlap within many cognitive domains. The neuroimaging analysis showed left perisylvian region involvement. A comprehensive cognitive, neuroimaging and pathological approach is necessary to identify the clinical and pathogenetic features of different PPA variants. Keywords: Primary progressive aphasia, nonfluent progressive aphasia, semantic dementia, logopenic progressive aphasia, neuroimaging studies, language symptoms 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Words and Paradigms in the Mental Lexicon by Kaidi L˜Oo a Thesis
    Words and Paradigms in the Mental Lexicon by Kaidi Loo˜ A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Linguistics University of Alberta Examining Committee: Juhani Jarvikivi,¨ Supervisor R. Harald Baayen, Supervisor Antti Arppe, Supervisory Committee Herbert Colston, Examiner James P. Blevins, Examiner c Kaidi Loo,˜ 2018 Abstract This dissertation examines the comprehension and production of Estonian case- inflected nouns. Estonian is a morphologically complex Finno-Ugric language with 14 cases in both singular and plural for each noun. Because storing millions of forms in memory seems implausible, languages like Estonian are often taken to be prime candidates for rule-driven morpheme-based processing. However, not all Estonian nouns actually occur in all their 28 cases, but only in cases that make sense based on the meaning of the word. For instance, for jalg ‘foot/leg’, the nom- inative plural jalad ‘feet/legs’ is very common, whereas the essive singular case jalana ‘as a foot/leg’ rarely ever gets used. Furthermore, Estonian inflected forms cluster into inflectional paradigms, which typically come with only a few inflected variants from which other forms in the paradigm can be predicted. Hence, the number of forms that a speaker would need to memorize is much smaller than the number of forms that one can understand or produce. Based on these observations, we aimed to clarify lexical-distributional prop- erties that co-determine Estonian processing. Using a large number of items and generalized mixed effects modeling, we tested the influence of a number of lexical measures, such as lemma frequency, whole-word frequency, morphological family size, inflectional entropy, orthographic length and orthographic neighbourhood density (all calculated on the basis of a 15-million token Estonian corpus).
    [Show full text]
  • Analysis of Naming Errors in Healthy Aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’S Disease
    ANALYSIS OF NAMING ERRORS IN HC, MCI, AND AD Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology (CJSLPA) Analysis of Naming Errors in Healthy Aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s Disease Analyse des erreurs de dénomination dans le vieillissement normal, le trouble cognitif léger et la maladie d’Alzheimer KEYWORDS Mélanie Gallant ANOMIA Monica Lavoie MILD COGNITIVE Carol Hudon IMPAIRMENT Laura Monetta NAMING ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE COGNITIVE AGING SEMANTIC MEMORY Mélanie Gallant, Monica Lavoie, Carol Hudon, and Laura Monetta Université Laval, Québec, QC, CANADA CERVO, Brain Research Centre, Québec, QC, CANADA Abstract The aim of this study was to document the functional origin of anomia in mild cognitive impairment in comparison to Alzheimer’s disease and healthy cognitive aging. An oral naming task of 260 pictures was administered to 20 individuals with mild cognitive impairment, 5 with mild Alzheimer’s disease, and 15 healthy controls. The mean total number of errors and types of naming errors were compared across the groups. The effect of psycholinguistic parameters and the efficacy of semantic and phonological cueing were also analyzed. Results showed a significant difference among the three groups’ total number of naming errors (Alzheimer’s disease > mild cognitive impairment > healthy controls). Similar types of naming errors were found among the groups and mainly consisted of coordinate semantic paraphasias. Further, less familiar words were associated with greater error probability in all groups. Finally, based on error types, psycholinguistic parameters, and efficacy of cueing, the main origin of anomia was determined for each participant and different patterns were observed among the three groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Delirium in the Medical Hospital
    Delirium in the Medical Hospital By Yankel Girshman, DO, FAPA Consultation Liaison Psychiatry & Psychosomatic Medicine Delirium • Definition • Epidemiology • Risk Factors & Etiology • Neuropathogenesis • Financial Impact & Length of Stay • Morbidity & Mortality • Relationship to Dementia • Work-up & Treatment • Recovery Delirium • Also known as: – Altered Mental Status – **Acute Brain Failure** – Encephalopathy – Acute Confusional State – ICU Psychosis – Hepatic/Hypoxic/Uremic/etc. Encephalopathy – Toxic Psychosis – Posttraumatic Confusion Delirium in the DSM V A. Disturbance in Attention (reduced ability to direct, focus, sustain and shift attention) AND Disturbance in Awareness (reduced orientation to environment) B. Develops over hours to days, change from baseline and fluctuates during the day. C. Additional disturbance in cognition (memory, disorientation, language, visuo-spatial ability or perception) Delirium Definition • ‘A serious disturbance in mental abilities that results in confused thinking and reduced awareness of your environment’. (Mayo Clinic) • ‘An acute mental disturbance characterized by confused thinking and disrupted attention usually accompanied by disordered speech and hallucinations’. (Webster) • Acute & fluctuating disorder of Consciousness (attention, awareness of self/environment and wakefulness), resulting in neuropsychiatric symptoms. (Psychosomatic Medicine Textbook) Delirium • Acute decline or change in mental status Disorders of Consciousness Minimally Vegetative Coma Conscious Delirium Normal State State
    [Show full text]