How Incidental Sequence Learning Creates Reportable Knowledge: the Role of Unexpected Events

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Incidental Sequence Learning Creates Reportable Knowledge: the Role of Unexpected Events Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0012942 2008, Vol. 34, No. 5, 1011–1026 How Incidental Sequence Learning Creates Reportable Knowledge: The Role of Unexpected Events Dennis Ru¨nger Peter A. Frensch Berlin–Brandenburg Academy of Sciences and Humanities Humboldt-Universita¨t zu Berlin Research on incidental sequence learning typically is concerned with the characteristics of implicit or nonconscious learning. In this article, the authors aim to elucidate the cognitive mechanisms that contribute to the generation of explicit, reportable sequence knowledge. According to the unexpected- event hypothesis (P. A. Frensch, H. Haider, D. Ru¨nger, U. Neugebauer, S. Voigt, & J. Werg, 2003), individuals acquire reportable knowledge when they search for the cause of an experienced deviation from the expected task performance. The authors experimentally induced unexpected events by disrupt- ing the sequence learning process with a modified serial reaction time task and found that, unlike random transfer sequences, a systematic transfer sequence increased the availability of reportable sequence knowledge. The lack of a facilitative effect of random sequences is explained by the detrimental effect of random events on the presumed search process that generates reportable knowledge. This view is corroborated in a final experiment in which the facilitative effect of systematic transfer blocks is offset by a concurrent secondary task that was introduced to interfere with the search process during transfer. Keywords: sequence learning, serial reaction time task, reportable knowledge, explicit knowledge, unexpected events Individuals can learn about the sequential structure of environ- able to provide a verbal description of the sequential regularity mental events incidentally, that is, without prior intention to ap- after the training phase. This and related findings beget the ques- prehend a sequential regularity. For the most part, empirical re- tion of which cognitive mechanisms might be responsible for the search on sequence learning has been concerned with the generation of explicit knowledge during incidental sequence learn- characteristics of implicit learning. Sequence learning is said to be ing. However, researchers have only recently begun to approach implicit when it occurs in the absence of conscious or explicit this particular issue theoretically; pertinent empirical research is knowledge about the sequential regularity (cf. Erdelyi, 2004). still largely absent from the literature. Little theoretical value has been attached to the ubiquitous finding The purpose of this report is to test a central prediction of a that incidental sequence learning also creates explicit knowl- theoretical framework advanced by Frensch et al. (2003) to explain edge—at least in some participants. Moreover, a review of the the acquisition of explicit, reportable knowledge in incidental literature suggests that the generation of explicit sequence knowl- learning situations. The framework was dubbed the unexpected- edge varies systematically across experimental conditions (Fren- event hypothesis in reference to the central theoretical notion that sch et al., 2003). For example, Frensch, Lin, and Buchner (1998; unexpected events can trigger the generation of reportable knowl- Experiments 2a and 2b) manipulated the amount of training, the edge. Before we delineate the hypothesis in greater detail, we type of training (i.e., single or dual task training), and the type of review some alternative theoretical accounts that have been of- sequence to be learned with the serial reaction time (SRT) task fered in the literature. (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987). On each trial with the task, a partic- It is possible to identify two broad classes of theories on the ipant responds to a target that appears in one of several screen generation of explicit sequence knowledge—single-system and positions by pressing a spatially compatible response key. Re- multiple-system accounts. According to the single-system view, sponse locations on consecutive trials conform to a fixed sequence implicit and explicit knowledge are rooted in the same set of that is continuously recycled throughout the training phase. All learning mechanisms (e.g., Cleeremans, 2006; Cleeremans & three factors were found to influence whether participants were Jime´nez, 2002; Kinder & Shanks, 2003; Perruchet & Vinter, 2002; Shanks, Wilkinson, & Channon, 2003). In its most stringent form, the single-system view rejects the notion of separable knowledge bases altogether, that is, the distinction between implicit and Dennis Ru¨nger, Berlin–Brandenburg Academy of Sciences and Human- explicit knowledge. It is assumed that all markers of learning, be ities, Berlin, Germany; Peter A. Frensch, Department of Psychology, it faster responses to sequentially structured stimuli or verbal Humboldt-Universita¨t zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany. descriptions of the sequential regularity, provide different expres- This research was supported by Federal Ministry of Education and Research Grant 01GWS061. sions of the same underlying memory representations. Learning Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dennis increases the quality of representations, which, in turn, leads to Ru¨nger, Berlin–Brandenburg Academy of Sciences and Humanities, Inter- improved performance in all available measures of learning (e.g., disciplinary Research Group “Functions of Consciousness,” Ja¨gerstr. 22/ Perruchet & Amorim, 1992; Perruchet, Bigand, & Benoit-Gonin, 23, D-10117 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: [email protected] 1997). 1011 1012 RU¨ NGER AND FRENSCH Weaker versions of the single-system view allow for a partial then might, or might not, lead to discovery and, subsequently, to decoupling of alternative measures of sequence learning. For ex- verbal report of the regularity (cf. Dienes & Perner, 1999). ample, according to Shanks and collaborators (Shanks, 2005; According to the unexpected-event hypothesis, the search for Shanks & Perruchet, 2002; Shanks et al., 2003), response time the cause of an observed unexpected event will likely lead to (RT) priming and recognition, two indicators of sequence learning, discovery of the regularity when the observed unexpected event is reflect different transformation processes of the same underlying related to the incidentally experienced regularity and when no knowledge representations. The transformation of memory repre- other immediately attributable causes for the unexpected event sentations into manual responses might be affected less by random exist. Consider, for example, a typical experiment with the SRT noise, for instance, than the transformation of the same represen- task. At some point during the training phase, the regular response tations into recognition judgments (e.g., Shanks et al., 2003). sequence is briefly replaced by random sequences, so that se- Therefore, it is conceivable that experimental manipulations that quence learning can be assessed by contrasting average RTs to affect the quality of memory representations influence different regular and random response locations. When viewed in light of measures of learning to different extents. In this way, empirical the unexpected-event hypothesis, this standard indirect test of dissociations may emerge that are typically interpreted as evidence sequence learning takes on a new significance: Over the course for the existence of separable implicit and explicit knowledge of training, implicit sequence learning improves task performance bases. by reducing both average RT and error rate. When the participant According to Cleeremans (2006; Cleeremans & Jime´nez, 2002) is transferred to random response locations, responses are likely to sequence learning is a mandatory consequence of performing a become slower and more error-prone due to the unpredictability of sequentially structured task such as the SRT task. Learning pro- target locations. We surmise that this sudden deterioration in task duces, over time, increasingly strong, stable, and distinct repre- performance, if registered by the participant, might constitute an sentations of the underlying sequential regularity. However, ensu- unexpected event that can trigger the generation of explicit se- ing high quality of a representation is not a sufficient condition for quence knowledge. conscious awareness of its content. For knowledge to be explicit, If interpolated random sequences can indeed function as a it has to be re-represented in a metarepresentation (Cleeremans, trigger for the generation of explicit knowledge, one should expect to find more explicit knowledge in this situation compared with an 2006; cf. Dienes & Perner, 1999). Cleeremans’ framework thus experimental condition in which sequence learning proceeds with- demarcates explicit and implicit knowledge according to the pres- out disruption. Preliminary support for this hypothesis comes from ence or absence of relevant metaknowledge. Nevertheless, the a study by Buchner, Steffens, Erdfelder, and Rothkegel (1997). framework qualifies as a single-system account of sequence learn- Buchner and colleagues used an auditory version of the SRT task ing because metarepresentations are produced by the same learn- in which participants discriminated between four different tones ing mechanisms in the same representational systems as their instead of target locations. Sequences of tones (and their
Recommended publications
  • The Unexpected Animorphs #44 K.A
    The Unexpected Animorphs #44 K.A. Applegate Table of Contents Chapter 1 Chapter 2 Chapter 3 Chapter 4 Chapter 5 Chapter 6 Chapter 7 Chapter 8 Chapter 9 Chapter 10 Chapter 11 Chapter 12 Chapter 13 Chapter 14 Chapter 15 Chapter 16 Chapter 17 Chapter 18 Chapter 19 Chapter 20 Chapter 21 Chapter 22 Chapter 23 Chapter 24 Chapter 1 I swooped low. This had to be it. Plane at the far gate. Two Marine guards, trying to look casual. Well, as casual as you can get wearing combat boots and a pistol strapped to your chest. <Jake, I think I found it. Jake?> I circled, flapped my wings to gain altitude. <Rachel? Tobias? Anybody?> An armored truck rumbled toward the plane. The driver stopped, showed one of the guards a clipboard, then backed up to the cargo hold. The rear of the truck opened. Two guys in hooded yellow coveralls climbed out. Pulled oxygen masks over their faces and unlatched the plane's cargo door. Okay. These guys definitely weren't unloading souvenirs from Disneyland. If somebody was transporting a chunk of Bug fighter wreckage, it had to be on this plane. I caught a thermal and rose above the airport. A baggage cart trundled across the tarmac. A jet screamed in for a landing. Guys in jumpsuits and headsets scrambled around, trying to keep the 747's from mowing down the commuter planes. And everywhere I looked - seagulls. On the roof, on the tarmac, against the fence. Seagulls are perfect cover. Part of the landscape, just like pigeons.
    [Show full text]
  • Green Acres School Reading Suggestions for 5Th Or 6Th Graders Updated June 2019
    Green Acres School Reading Suggestions for 5th or 6th Graders Updated June 2019 (The books recommended below are part of the Green Acres Library collection. Reading levels and interests vary greatly, so you may want to look also at Reading Suggestions for 4th Graders and Reading Suggestions for 7th/8th Graders.) This list includes: • Fiction • Poetry and Short Stories • Biography and Memoir • Other Nonfiction Graphic books are denoted with the symbol. Fiction Alice, Alex; transl. by Castle In the Stars: The Space Race of 1869 Anne Smith and Owen Smith. "In … this lavishly illustrated graphic novel, Alex Alice delivers a historical fantasy adventure set in a world where man journeyed into space in 1869, not 1969.” Graphic steampunk/Historical fantasy. (Publisher) Appelt, Kathi and Alison McGhee. Maybe a Fox “A fox kit born with a deep spiritual connection to a rural Vermont legend has a special bond with 11-year-old Jules.” Fantasy. (Kirkus Reviews) Avi. The Unexpected Life of Oliver Cromwell Pitts “A 12-year-old boy is left to fend for himself in 18th-century England following a terrible storm and the disappearance of his father… Impossible to put down.” Historical fiction. (Kirkus Reviews) Bauer, Joan. Soar "Sports, friendship, tragedy, and a love connection are all wrapped up in one heartwarming, page-turning story. …This coming-of-age tale features a boy who is courageous and witty; readers—baseball fans or otherwise—will cheer on Jeremiah and this team. The latest middle grade novel from this award-winning author is triumphant and moving." Fiction. (School Library Journal) Beckhorn, Susan.
    [Show full text]
  • The Unexpected Discovery of a Novel Low-Oxygen-Activated Locus for the Anoxic Persistence of Burkholderia Cenocepacia
    The ISME Journal (2013), 1–14 OPEN & 2013 International Society for Microbial Ecology All rights reserved 1751-7362/13 www.nature.com/ismej ORIGINAL ARTICLE The unexpected discovery of a novel low-oxygen-activated locus for the anoxic persistence of Burkholderia cenocepacia Andrea M Sass1,2, Crystal Schmerk3, Kirsty Agnoli4, Phillip J Norville1, Leo Eberl4, Miguel A Valvano3,5 and Eshwar Mahenthiralingam1 1Organisms and Environment Division, Cardiff School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales, UK; 2Laboratorium voor Farmaceutische Microbiologie, Universiteit Gent, Gent, Belgium; 3Centre for Human Immunology, Department of Microbiology and Immunology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada; 4Department of Microbiology, Institute of Plant Biology, University of Zu¨rich, Zu¨rich, Switzerland and 5Centre for Infection and Immunity, Queen’s University of Belfast, Belfast, Ireland, UK Burkholderia cenocepacia is a Gram-negative aerobic bacterium that belongs to a group of opportunistic pathogens displaying diverse environmental and pathogenic lifestyles. B. cenocepa- cia is known for its ability to cause lung infections in people with cystic fibrosis and it possesses a large 8 Mb multireplicon genome encoding a wide array of pathogenicity and fitness genes. Transcriptomic profiling across nine growth conditions was performed to identify the global gene expression changes made when B. cenocepacia changes niches from an environmental lifestyle to infection. In comparison to exponential growth, the results demonstrated that B. cenocepacia changes expression of over one-quarter of its genome during conditions of growth arrest, stationary phase and surprisingly, under reduced oxygen concentrations (6% instead of 20.9% normal atmospheric conditions). Multiple virulence factors are upregulated during these growth arrest conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music
    THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Isabelle Peretz Robert J. Zatorre Editors OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page i THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC This page intentionally left blank THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Edited by ISABELLE PERETZ Départment de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, C.P. 6128, Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec, H3C 3J7, Canada and ROBERT J. ZATORRE Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada 1 Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page iv 1 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide in Oxford New York Auckland Bangkok Buenos Aires Cape Town Chennai Dar es Salaam Delhi Hong Kong Istanbul Karachi Kolkata Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Mumbai Nairobi São Paulo Shanghai Taipei Tokyo Toronto Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries Published in the United States by Oxford University Press Inc., New York © The New York Academy of Sciences, Chapters 1–7, 9–20, and 22–8, and Oxford University Press, Chapters 8 and 21. Most of the materials in this book originally appeared in The Biological Foundations of Music, published as Volume 930 of the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, June 2001 (ISBN 1-57331-306-8). This book is an expanded version of the original Annals volume. The moral rights of the author have been asserted Database right Oxford University Press (maker) First published 2003 All rights reserved.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Digit Span Measures Long-Term Associative Learning
    Cognition 144 (2015) 1–13 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning ⇑ Gary Jones a, , Bill Macken b a Nottingham Trent University, UK b Cardiff University, UK article info abstract Article history: Traditional accounts of verbal short-term memory explain differences in performance for different types Received 23 December 2014 of verbal material by reference to inherent characteristics of the verbal items making up memory Revised 13 July 2015 sequences. The role of previous experience with sequences of different types is ostensibly controlled Accepted 15 July 2015 for either by deliberate exclusion or by presenting multiple trials constructed from different random per- mutations. We cast doubt on this general approach in a detailed analysis of the basis for the robust find- ing that short-term memory for digit sequences is superior to that for other sequences of verbal material. Keywords: Specifically, we show across four experiments that this advantage is not due to inherent characteristics of Digit span digits as verbal items, nor are individual digits within sequences better remembered than other types of Associative learning Short-term memory individual verbal items. Rather, the advantage for digit sequences stems from the increased frequency, Long-term memory compared to other verbal material, with which digits appear in random sequences in natural language, Sequence learning and furthermore, relatively frequent digit sequences support better short-term serial recall than less fre- Computational modelling quent ones. We also provide corpus-based computational support for the argument that performance in a short-term memory setting is a function of basic associative learning processes operating on the linguis- tic experience of the rememberer.
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship Between Mood and Implicit Learning
    UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones May 2017 The Relationship Between Mood and Implicit Learning Kathleen Larson University of Nevada, Las Vegas Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons, and the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons Repository Citation Larson, Kathleen, "The Relationship Between Mood and Implicit Learning" (2017). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 3003. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/10985984 This Dissertation is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Dissertation in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/or on the work itself. This Dissertation has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MOOD AND IMPLICIT LEARNING By Kathleen G. Larson Bachelor of Arts – Psychology Indiana University of Pennsylvania 2011 Master of Arts – Psychology University of Nevada, Las Vegas 2014 A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of
    [Show full text]
  • New Learning of Music After Bilateral Medial Temporal Lobe Damage: Evidence from an Amnesic Patient
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 03 September 2014 HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00694 New learning of music after bilateral medial temporal lobe damage: evidence from an amnesic patient Jussi Valtonen1*, Emma Gregory 2, Barbara Landau 2 and Michael McCloskey 2 1 Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland 2 Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA Edited by: Damage to the hippocampus impairs the ability to acquire new declarative memories, but Isabelle Peretz, Université de not the ability to learn simple motor tasks. An unresolved question is whether hippocampal Montréal, Canada damage affects learning for music performance, which requires motor processes, but in a Reviewed by: cognitively complex context. We studied learning of novel musical pieces by sight-reading Séverine Samson, Université de Lille, France in a newly identified amnesic, LSJ, who was a skilled amateur violist prior to contract- Aline Moussard, Rotman Research ing herpes simplex encephalitis. LSJ has suffered virtually complete destruction of the Institute, Canada hippocampus bilaterally, as well as extensive damage to other medial temporal lobe struc- *Correspondence: tures and the left anterior temporal lobe. Because of LSJ’s rare combination of musical Jussi Valtonen, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of training and near-complete hippocampal destruction, her case provides a unique oppor- Helsinki, P.O. Box 9, Helsinki tunity to investigate the role of the hippocampus for complex motor learning processes FI-00014, Finland specifically related to music performance. Three novel pieces of viola music were com- e-mail: jussi.valtonen@helsinki.fi posed and closely matched for factors contributing to a piece’s musical complexity.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociologists of the Unexpected: Edward A
    Sociologists of the Unexpected: Edward A. Ross and Georg Simmel on the Unintended Consequences of Modernity Author(s): Matthias Gross Source: The American Sociologist, Vol. 34, No. 4 (Winter, 2003), pp. 40-58 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/27700365 Accessed: 26-04-2016 16:33 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. American Sociological Association, Springer are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The American Sociologist This content downloaded from 128.135.12.127 on Tue, 26 Apr 2016 16:33:34 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Sociologists of the Unexpected: Edward A. Ross and Georg Simmel on the Unintended Consequences of Modernity Matthias Gross* The modern increase in opportunities for social activities also brings with it unintended side effects posed by the liberating potential and the acceleration of modern life. In this paper it is argued that the views reflected in Georg SimmePs formal approach and in American sociologist Edward A. Ross' reformative sociology are (1) complementary and (2) offer fresh insights for our current sociological understanding of unexpected consequences in contemporary "high moder nity" or knowledge societies.
    [Show full text]
  • Differentiating Visual from Response Sequencing During Long-Term Skill Learning
    Differentiating Visual from Response Sequencing during Long-term Skill Learning Brighid Lynch1, Patrick Beukema2, and Timothy Verstynen1 Abstract ■ The dual-system model of sequence learning posits that dur- order of key presses across training days), Combined (same ing early learning there is an advantage for encoding sequences serial order of cues and responses on all training days), and a in sensory frames; however, it remains unclear whether this ad- Control group (a novel sequence each training day). Across 5 days vantage extends to long-term consolidation. Using the serial RT of training, sequence-specific measures of response speed and task, we set out to distinguish the dynamics of learning sequen- accuracy improved faster in the Visual group than any of the tial orders of visual cues from learning sequential responses. On other three groups, despite no group differences in explicit each day, most participants learned a new mapping between awareness of the sequence. The two groups that were exposed a set of symbolic cues and responses made with one of four to the same visual sequence across days showed a marginal im- fingers, after which they were exposed to trial blocks of either provement in response binding that was not found in the other randomly ordered cues or deterministic ordered cues (12-item groups. These results indicate that there is an advantage, in sequence). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four terms of rate of consolidation across multiple days of training, groups (n = 15 per group): Visual sequences (same sequence for learning sequences of actions in a sensory representational of visual cues across training days), Response sequences (same space, rather than as motoric representations.
    [Show full text]
  • Grade 5 Supplemental Literature List
    Clovis Unified School District Supplemental Literature for Grade 5 Book Title Author Summary th 13 ​ Floor Fleischman, Sid When his older sister disappears, twelve-year-old Buddy Stebbins follows her back in time and finds ​ himself aboard a seventeenth-century pirate ship captained by a distant relative. Abraham Lincoln The story of President Lincoln, from his birth in Kentucky in 1809 to his life in Washington and the end of the Civil War. Across Five Aprils Hunt, Irene The effects of the Civil War on the Creighton family, farmers in Southern Illinois, are depicted as the older men are pulled one by one into the war, leaving the youngest, Jethro, to keep their farm going. The novel is based on the author's family records, letters, and grandfather's stories. Adventures of Blue Avenger, The Howe, Norma On his sixteenth birthday, still trying to cope with the unexpected death of his father, David Schumacher decides--or does he--to change his name to Blue Avenger, hoping to find a way to make a difference in his Oakland neighborhood and in the world. Afternoon of the Elves Lisle, Janet Taylor As Hillary works in the miniature village, allegedly built by elves, in Sara-Kate's backyard, she becomes more and more curious about Sara-Kate's real life inside her big, gloomy house with her mysterious, silent mother. Al Capone does my Shirts Choldenko, Gennifer A twelve-year-old boy named Moose moves to Alcatraz Island in 1935 when guards' families were housed there, and has to contend with his extraordinary new environment in addition to life with his autistic sister Natalie.
    [Show full text]
  • The Value of Surprise in Science* Steven French, University of Leeds Alice Murphy, LMU Munich Forthcoming in Erkenntnis March 2021 Version Abstract
    The Value of Surprise in Science* Steven French, University of Leeds Alice Murphy, LMU Munich Forthcoming in Erkenntnis March 2021 version Abstract Scientific results are often presented as ‘surprising’ as if that is a good thing. Is it? And if so, why? What is the value of surprise in science? Discussions of surprise in science have been limited, but surprise has been used as a way of defending the epistemic privilege of experiments over simulations. The argument is that while experiments can ‘confound’, simulations can merely surprise (Morgan 2005). Our aim in this paper is to show that the discussion of surprise can be usefully extended to thought experiments and theoretical derivations. We argue that in focusing on these features of scientific practice, we can see that the surprise-confoundment distinction does not fully capture surprise in science. We set out how thought experiments and theoretical derivations can bring about surprises that can be disruptive in a productive way, and we end by exploring how this linKs with their future fertility. Introduction Scientific results are often presented as ‘surprising’, as if that is a good thing. Is it? And if so, why? What is the value of surprise in science? In addressing such questions discussions have tended to focus on one of two features of scientific practice: novel predictions and their role in the realism debate (see Hitchcock and Sober 2004); and novel or surprising phenomena. In the former case, the surprise associated with the novelty is definitely a good thing as far as the scientific realist is concerned, indicative as it is of the ‘mind independent’ nature of the relevant theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Attention and Structure in Sequence Learning
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/90/$00.75 1990, Vol. 16, No. 1, 17-30 Attention and Structure in Sequence Learning Asher Cohen, Richard I. Ivry, and Steven W. Keele University of Oregon In this study we investigated the role of attention, sequence structure, and effector specificity in learning a structured sequence of actions. Experiment 1 demonstrated that simple structured sequences can be learned in the presence of attentional distraction. The learning is unaffected by variation in distractor task difficulty, and subjects appear unaware of the structure. The structured sequence knowledge transfers from finger production to arm production {Experiment 2), sug- gesting that sequence specification resides in an effector-independent system. Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrated that only structures with at least some unique associations (e.g., any association in Structure 15243... or 4 to 3 in Structure 143132...) can be learned under attentional distraction. Structures with all items repeated in different orders in different parts of the structure (e.g., Sequence 132312...) require attention for learning. Such structures may require hierarchic representation, the construction of which takes attention. One of the remarkable capabilities of humans is their ability four keys in response to an asterisk at one of four spatial to learn a variety of novel tasks involving complex motor positions. In one condition, the signals came on in a particular sequences. They learn to play the violin, knit, serve tennis sequence of 10 events, with the same order repeating cyclically balls, and perform a variety of language tasks such as speaking, (hereafter, structured sequence).
    [Show full text]