A Computational Model of the Emergence of Early Constructions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Computational Model of the Emergence of Early Constructions Constructing grammar: A computational model of the emergence of early constructions Nancy Chih-Lin Chang Electrical Engineering and Computer Sciences University of California at Berkeley Technical Report No. UCB/EECS-2009-24 http://www.eecs.berkeley.edu/Pubs/TechRpts/2009/EECS-2009-24.html February 5, 2009 Copyright 2009, by the author(s). All rights reserved. Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission. Constructing grammar: A computational model of the emergence of early constructions by Nancy Chih-lin Chang A.B. (Harvard University) 1994 M.Phil. (Cambridge University) 1995 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Computer Science in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Jerome A. Feldman, Chair Professor Nelson Morgan Professor Daniel I. Slobin Fall 2008 The dissertation of Nancy Chih-lin Chang is approved. Chair Date Date Date University of California, Berkeley Fall 2008 Constructing grammar: A computational model of the emergence of early constructions Copyright c 2008 by Nancy Chih-lin Chang Abstract Constructing grammar: A computational model of the emergence of early constructions by Nancy Chih-lin Chang Doctor of Philosophy in Computer Science University of California, Berkeley Professor Jerome A. Feldman, Chair In this thesis I explore and formalize the view that grammar learning is driven by meaningful lan- guage use in context. On this view, the goal of a first language learner is to become a better language user — in particular, by acquiring linguistic constructions (structured mappings between form and meaning) that facilitate successful communication. I present a computational model in which all aspects of the language learning problem are reformulated in line with these assumptions. The representational basis of the model is a construction grammar formalism that captures constituent structure and relational constraints, both within and across the domains of form and meaning. This formalism plays a central role in two processes: language understanding, which uses constuctions to interpret utterances in context; and language learning, which seeks to improve comprehension by making judicious changes to the current set of constructions. The resulting integrated model of language structure, use and acquisition provides a cogni- tively motivated and computationally precise account of how children acquire their earliest multi- word constructions. I define a set of operations for proposing new constructions, either to capture contextually available mappings not predicted by the current grammar, or to reorganize existing constructions. Candidate constructions are evaluated using a minimum description length criterion that balances a structural bias toward simpler grammars against a data-driven bias toward more specific grammars. When trained with a corpus of child-directed utterances annotated with situ- ation descriptions, the model gradually acquires the concrete word combinations and item-based constructions that constitute the first steps toward adult language. Professor Jerome A. Feldman Dissertation Committee Chair 1 for my parents, I-Cheng and Phoebe Chang A word after a word after a word is power. — Margaret Atwood, from “Spelling” We are all talkers It is true, but underneath the talk lies The moving and not wanting to be moved, the loose Meaning, untidy and simple like a threshing floor. — John Ashbery, from “Soonest Mended” i Contents Dedication i Contents ii List of Figures v List of Tables vii Acknowledgments viii 1 Beyond single words 1 1.1 Meaningful language learning: the model in brief .................... 3 1.2 Goals in context ........................................ 6 1.2.1 Grammaticality and its discontents ......................... 7 1.2.2 Embodied models of language acquisition and use ............... 11 1.2.3 Contributions ..................................... 14 1.3 Road maps ........................................... 16 2 Orientations 18 2.1 Preliminaries .......................................... 19 2.1.1 Gold’s legacy ..................................... 19 2.1.2 Theoretical frameworks ............................... 22 2.1.3 The course of acquisition .............................. 26 2.2 Foundations .......................................... 32 2.2.1 Constructions ..................................... 32 2.2.2 Embodiment ..................................... 35 2.2.3 Usage .......................................... 39 2.3 Formalisms ........................................... 42 2.3.1 Unification-based grammar ............................. 43 2.3.2 Extending parsing to constructions ......................... 44 2.3.3 Embodiment and simulation ............................ 45 2.3.4 Learning as optimization .............................. 47 3 Embodied Construction Grammar 50 3.1 Overview ............................................ 51 3.1.1 From sentence to simulation ............................ 53 3.1.2 Running example in ECG .............................. 55 3.1.3 Beyond Harry ran home ................................ 59 3.1.4 The formalism at a glance .............................. 61 3.2 Schemas ............................................ 64 3.2.1 Form .......................................... 64 3.2.2 Meaning ........................................ 65 3.2.3 Function ........................................ 68 3.3 Constructions ......................................... 69 ii 3.3.1 Cross-domain mapping types ............................ 70 3.3.2 Reference and predication .............................. 71 3.3.3 Other constructions .................................. 76 3.4 Summary ............................................ 79 4 Using embodied constructions 80 4.1 Definitions ........................................... 82 4.1.1 Schemas and constructions ............................. 82 4.1.2 Utterances and contexts ............................... 86 4.1.3 Analysis and resolution ............................... 88 4.2 A simple analyzer-resolver .................................. 91 4.2.1 Constructional analysis ............................... 93 4.2.2 Contextual resolution ................................ 95 4.3 Evaluation criteria ....................................... 99 4.3.1 Scoring construct graphs ............................... 100 4.3.2 Scoring form and meaning coverage ........................ 102 4.3.3 Scoring (resolved) analyses ............................. 104 5 Learning embodied constructions 111 5.1 The child’s learning task: a review ............................. 111 5.1.1 Representational requirements ........................... 112 5.1.2 Comprehension-based constraints ......................... 113 5.1.3 Developmental desiderata .............................. 113 5.2 Problem statement(s) ..................................... 115 5.2.1 Construction learning defined ........................... 115 5.2.2 Hypothesis space ................................... 117 5.2.3 Prior knowledge ................................... 118 5.2.4 Input data ....................................... 120 5.2.5 Performance criteria ................................. 121 5.2.6 Summary: relational construction learning .................... 122 5.3 Approaches to learning .................................... 123 5.3.1 Bayesian inference .................................. 124 5.3.2 Bayesian model merging ............................... 125 5.3.3 Model merging for embodied constructions .................... 128 5.4 Usage-based construction learning: overview ....................... 130 6 Usage-based learning operations 134 6.1 Overview of operations ................................... 135 6.2 Context-driven mapping operations ............................ 136 6.2.1 Simple mapping ................................... 137 6.2.2 Relational mapping .................................. 138 6.3 Reorganization operations .................................. 143 6.3.1 Structural alignment ................................. 144 6.3.2 Merging ........................................ 148 6.3.3 Joining ......................................... 156 6.3.4 Splitting ........................................ 158 6.4 Summary ............................................ 160 7 Simplicity-based evaluation heuristics 163 7.1 Minimum description length ................................ 163 7.2 MDL for ECG ......................................... 165 7.2.1 Counts and weights ................................. 167 7.2.2 Grammar length ................................... 168 7.2.3 Data length ...................................... 169 7.3 Updating description length ................................. 172 iii 7.3.1 Updating grammar length .............................. 172 7.3.2 Updating data length ................................. 174 7.4 Incorporating reference costs ................................ 175 7.5 Discussion ........................................... 177 8 The model in action 179 8.1 Nomi’s world ......................................... 179 8.1.1 Input assumptions .................................. 180 8.1.2 Initial lexicon and schema set ...........................
Recommended publications
  • Sign-Based Construction Grammar: an Informal Synopsis IVA N A
    September 4, 2012 3 Sign-Based Construction Grammar: An Informal Synopsis IVA N A. SAG 1 Introduction This chapter1 is intended as an introduction to some of the central notions of Sign-Based Construction Grammar (SBCG).2 For a more general discussion of SBCG, including an informal, high-level summary of the framework, its historical development, motivation, and relation to other approaches to gram- mar, the reader is referred to Sag et al. this volume. 1.1 Preliminaries SBCG is a framework blending ideas developed over a quarter century of re- search in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG3) with those pre- 1Above all, I’d like to thank Paul Kay for hours of discussion and useful feedback at every stage in the seemingly endless process of writing this chapter. I’m also grateful to Chuck Fillmore and Laura Michaelis for many useful discussions of both content and exposition and for com- ments on an earlier draft, which was also been improved by the comments of Rui Chaves, Adele Goldberg, Bill Croft, Adam Przepiórkowski, Russell Lee-Goldman, and Hans Boas. In addition, discussions with the following people have been quite helpful: Farrell Ackerman, Emily Bender, Dan Flickinger, Mark Gawron, Andreas Kathol, Beth Levin, Bob Levine, Stefan Müller, Frank Van Eynde, Tom Wasow, Gert Webelhuth, and Steve Wechsler. None of the views expressed here should be attributed to any of these individuals; nor should they be held responsible for any remaining errors. 2SBCG owes a considerable debt to the implementation work carried out within CSLI’s LinGO Lab and the DELPHIN consortium, whose grammar development efforts have proceeded in paral- lel with the evolution of SBCG.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructions: a New Theoretical Approach to Language
    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.5 May 2003 219 Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language Adele E. Goldberg Linguistics Department, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801-0168, USA A new theoretical approach to language has emerged in the ‘core’ of language. Mainstream generative theory the past 10–15 years that allows linguistic observations argues further that the complexity of core language cannot about form–meaning pairings, known as ‘construc- be learned inductively by general cognitive mechanisms tions’, to be stated directly. Constructionist approaches and therefore learners must be hard-wired with principles aim to account for the full range of facts about that are specific to language (‘universal grammar’). language, without assuming that a particular subset of the data is part of a privileged ‘core’. Researchers in this Tenets of constructionist approaches field argue that unusual constructions shed light on Each basic tenet outlined below is shared by most more general issues, and can illuminate what is constructionist approaches. Each represents a major required for a complete account of language. divergence from the mainstream generative approach and, in many ways, a return to a more traditional view of Constructions – form and meaning pairings – have been language. the basis of major advances in the study of grammar since Tenet 1. All levels of description are understood to the days of Aristotle. Observations about specific linguistic involve pairings of form with semantic or discourse constructions have shaped our understanding of both function, including morphemes or words, idioms, particular languages and the nature of language itself. But partially lexically filled and fully abstract phrasal only recently has a new theoretical approach emerged that patterns.
    [Show full text]
  • Introducing Sign-Based Construction Grammar IVA N A
    September 4, 2012 1 Introducing Sign-Based Construction Grammar IVA N A. SAG,HANS C. BOAS, AND PAUL KAY 1 Background Modern grammatical research,1 at least in the realms of morphosyntax, in- cludes a number of largely nonoverlapping communities that have surpris- ingly little to do with one another. One – the Universal Grammar (UG) camp – is mainly concerned with a particular view of human languages as instantia- tions of a single grammar that is fixed in its general shape. UG researchers put forth highly abstract hypotheses making use of a complex system of repre- sentations, operations, and constraints that are offered as a theory of the rich biological capacity that humans have for language.2 This community eschews writing explicit grammars of individual languages in favor of offering conjec- tures about the ‘parameters of variation’ that modulate the general grammat- ical scheme. These proposals are motivated by small data sets from a variety of languages. A second community, which we will refer to as the Typological (TYP) camp, is concerned with descriptive observations of individual languages, with particular concern for idiosyncrasies and complexities. Many TYP re- searchers eschew formal models (or leave their development to others), while others in this community refer to the theory they embrace as ‘Construction Grammar’ (CxG). 1For comments and valuable discussions, we are grateful to Bill Croft, Chuck Fillmore, Adele Goldberg, Stefan Müller, and Steve Wechsler. We also thank the people mentioned in footnote 8 below. 2The nature of these representations has changed considerably over the years. Seminal works include Chomsky 1965, 1973, 1977, 1981, and 1995.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Classes in Radical Construction Grammar
    Word classes in Radical Construction Grammar William Croft, University of New Mexico 1. Introduction Radical Construction Grammar (Croft 2001, 2005a, 2013) is a construction-based theory of grammar that allows for the representation of both cross-linguistic and language- internal grammatical diversity. Radical Construction Grammar analyses conform to the following three basic principles: (1) Word classes and other syntactic structures are language-specific and construction-specific. What is universal is patterns of variation in the verbalization of experience, represented for example in conceptual space. (2) The internal structure of the morphosyntactic form of constructions consists solely of the part-whole relation between construction roles (“slots”) and the whole construction. The complexity of constructions rests in the symbolic relations between the roles and their meanings/functions, and the rich semantic/functional structure expressed by the construction and its parts. (3) The morphosyntactic forms of constructions are language-specific, that is, there is potentially gradient variation of constructional form across and within languages. Radical Construction Grammar (Croft 2001) and Morphosyntax: Constructions of the World’s Languages (Croft, in prep.) propose many specific analyses of constructions and universals of constructional variation. While those analyses may and likely will need revision as knowledge of the constructions of the world’s languages increases, any analysis that conforms to the principles in (1)-(3) can be considered
    [Show full text]
  • A Construction Grammar Approach * Yoon-Suk Chung
    Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach * Yoon-Suk Chung B.A. (Seoul National University) 1985 M.A. (Seoul National University) 1988 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 1992 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Paul Kay, Chair Professor Charles J. Fillmore Professor Yoko Hasegawa Spring 2001 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach Copyright 2001 by Yoon-Suk Chung Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1 Abstract Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach by Yoon-Suk Chung Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Paul Kay, Chair This study proposes a new analysis of the Tough construction (TC) in the English language. This analysis is couched within the non-modular, non- derivational, unification and inheritance-based framework of Construction Grammar (CG). There are five major theoretical issues around which the study of the TC has revolved: (1) the identification of the TC in generative syntax as a species of either extraction or control; (2) the characterization of the tough predicate as either a raising or a control predicate; (3) the analysis of the/or-phrase as either a PP or a complementizer + lower subject; and the classification of the tough infinitival phrase as (4) a VP, an IP, or a CP, and as (5) either a complement or an adjunct.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructing a Protolanguage: Reconstructing Prehistoric
    Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. article template Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. doi:10.1098/rstb.2020.0200 Constructing a protolanguage: Reconstructing prehistoric languages in a usage-based construction grammar framework Stefan Hartmann1 and Michael Pleyer2,3 1 Germanistische Sprachwissenschaft, University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1186-7182 2 Centre for Language Evolution Studies and 3 University Centre of Excellence IMSErt - Interacting Minds, Societies, Environments, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Toruń, Poland https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6685-391X Keywords: Protolanguage, construction grammar, usage-based linguistics, linguistic reconstruction, syntactic reconstruction This is the Author Accepted Manuscript of this publication, i.e. the final manuscript reflecting all changes that were made after peer-review. The final published version will be available at the publisher’s website once the article has been published. Summary Construction grammar is an approach to language which posits that units and structures in language can be exhaustively described as pairings between form and meaning. These pairings are called constructions and can have different degrees of abstraction, i.e. they span the entire range from very concrete (Armadillo, avocado) to very abstract constructions such as the ditransitive construction (I gave her a book). This approach has been applied to a wide variety of different areas of research in linguistics, such as how new constructions emerge and change historically. It has also been applied to investigate the evolutionary emergence of modern fully- fledged language, i.e. the question of how systems of constructions can arise out of prelinguistic communication. In this paper we review the contribution of usage-based Construction grammar approaches to language change and language evolution to the questions of a) the structure and nature of prehistoric languages and b) how constructions in prehistoric languages emerged out of non-linguistic or protolinguistic communication.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards Bidirectional Processing
    Towards Bidirectional Processing Models of Sign Language: A Constructional Approach in Fluid Construction Grammar Remi van Trijp ([email protected]) Sony Computer Science Laboratory Paris 6, Rue Amyot, 75005 Paris France Abstract This paper proposes that a construction grammar architec- ture (Fillmore, 1988; Goldberg, 1995; Croft, 2001; Ostman¨ & Sign languages (SL) require a fundamental rethinking of many basic assumptions about human language processing because Fried, 2004; Steels, 2011) offers a solution to these problems. instead of using linear speech, sign languages coarticulate fa- The most salient property of construction grammar is that it cial expressions, shoulder and hand movements, eye gaze and eliminates the need for separate layers of linguistic organiza- usage of a three-dimensional space. SL researchers have there- fore advocated SL-specific approaches that do not start from tion, such as the sharp distinction between lexicon and syn- the biases of models that were originally developed for vo- tax. Instead, constructions (i.e. conventionalized mappings cal languages. Unfortunately, there are currently no process- between meaning/function and form) are posited as the sole ing models that adequately achieve both language comprehen- sion and formulation, and the SL-specific developments run data structure for representing linguistic knowledge. Con- the risk of becoming alienated from other linguistic research. structions are thus able to simultaneously access and manipu- This paper explores the hypothesis that a construction gram- late multimodal sources of information, which offers unique mar architecture offers a solution to these problems because constructions are able to simultaneously access and manipu- opportunities for modeling sign languages. late information coming from many different sources.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 3 Embodied Construction Grammar
    Chapter 3 Embodied Construction Grammar 3.1 Overview .................................. 51 3.1.1 From sentence to simulation ................... 53 3.1.2 Running example in ECG ..................... 55 3.1.3 Beyond Harry ran home ...................... 59 3.1.4 The formalism at a glance .................... 61 3.2 Schemas .................................. 64 3.2.1 Form ................................ 64 3.2.2 Meaning .............................. 65 3.2.3 Function .............................. 68 3.3 Constructions ............................... 69 3.3.1 Cross-domain mapping types .................. 70 3.3.2 Reference and predication .................... 71 3.3.3 Other constructions ........................ 76 3.4 Summary .................................. 79 What is an idea? It is an image that paints itself in my brain. — Voltaire My very photogenic mother died in a freak accident (picnic, lightning)... — from Lolita, Vladimir Nabokov Our current enterprise requires a theory of language structure that is compatible with the con- straints set forth so far: it must be consistent with the notion of embodiment in language use; it must be formally described and it must be learnable. Embodied Construction Grammar (ECG) is a computational formalism intended to fill this tall order. This chapter provides a general intro- duction to the formalism, beginning with an overview in Section 3.1 of its role in supporting a broader simulation-based model of language understanding. Section 3.2 and Section 3.3 provide more detailed descriptions of the two basic ECG primitives, schemas and constructions. As a relative newcomer in the world of grammatical theories, the ECG formalism remains un- der active development. Since its original formulation (primarily in Bergen & Chang 2005, with a shorter description in Chang et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructional Meaning and Compositionality(Pdf)
    To appear in C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger and P. Portner, eds., Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning, HSK Handbooks of Linguistics and Communication Science Series: 23: Semantics and Computer Science. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Constructional Meaning and Compositionality 5 Paul Kay and Laura A. Michaelis 1. Constructions and compositionality It is sometimes supposed that constructional approaches are opposed to compositional semantics. This happens to be an incorrect supposition, but it is instructive to consider why it exists. A foundation of construction-based grammar 10 is the idea that rules of syntactic combination (In Sign Based Construction Grammar [SBCG] descriptions of local trees) are directly associated with interpretive and use conditions, in the form of semantic and pragmatic features that attach to the mother or daughter nodes in these descriptions (Sag 2007, 2008). This amounts to the claim that syntactic rules have directly associated 15 meanings. Meaning, of course, is generally viewed as something that only words can do, and in the prevailing view of meaning composition, syntactic rules do no more than determine what symbol sequences function as units for syntactic purposes. So while syntactic rules assemble words and their dependent elements into phrases, and the phrases denote complex concepts like predicates and 20 propositions, the rules cannot add conceptual content to that contributed by the words; nor can they alter the combinatoric properties of the words. On this view, which Jackendoff (1997: 48) describes as the “doctrine of syntactically transparent composition”, “[a]ll elements of content in the meaning of a sentence are found in the lexical conceptual structures […] of the lexical items composing the 25 sentence.” To embrace a construction-based model of semantic composition is not to reject the existence of syntactically transparent composition but instead to treat it as a “default in a wider array of options” (Jackendoff (1997: 49).
    [Show full text]
  • Construction Grammar 73
    Construction Grammar 73 Blakemore D (2000). ‘Indicators and procedures: neverthe- Lee H -K (2002). ‘Towards a new typology of connnectives less and but.’ Journal of Linguistics 36, 463–486. with special reference to conjunction in English and Blakemore D (2002). Relevance and linguistic meaning: Korean.’ Journal of Pragmatics 34, 851–866. The semantics and pragmatics of discourse markers. Levinson S C (1987). ‘Minimization and conversational Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. inference.’ In Verschueren J & Bertuccelli-Papi M (eds.) Blutner R (2000). ‘Some aspects of optimality in natural The pragmatic perspective: Selected papers from the language interpretation.’ Journal of Semantics 17, 1985 International Pragmatics Conference. Amsterdam: 189–216. J. Benjamins. 61–129. Blutner R & Zeevat H (eds.) (2003). Optimality theory and Levinson S C (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of pragmatics. London: Palgrave. generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, Horn L R (1984). ‘Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic Mass: MIT Press. inference: Q-based and R-based implicature.’ In Schiffrin Wilson D & Sperber D (1993). ‘Linguistic form and D (ed.) Georgetown University Round Table on relevance.’ Lingua 90, 1–25. Languages and Linguistics 1984. Washington, D.C.: Zeevat H (2000). ‘The asymmetry of optimality theoretic Georgetown University Press. 11–42. syntax and semantics.’ Journal of Semantics 17, 243–262. Construction Grammar L A Michaelis, University of Colorado at Boulder, of principles of [universal grammar], and traditional Boulder, CO, USA grammatical constructions are perhaps best regarded as taxonomic epiphenomena – collections of structures ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. with properties resulting from the interaction of fixed principles with parameters set one way or another Introduction (Chomsky, 1989: 43).
    [Show full text]
  • From Construction Grammar to Evolutionary Linguistics
    lecture 10 From Construction Grammar to Evolutionary Linguistics Thank you very much for coming. I know that a lot of you have been following the lectures since the three of us came to Beijing. And others of you are join- ing the lecture series for the first time, some of you who are students here at Beijing Foreign Studies University. The lectures that I have been giving have been on the topic of construction grammar. Construction grammar is a model of representing syntactic struc- ture in Cognitive Linguistics. I spent most of the lectures discussing a partic- ular version of construction grammar which I have proposed, called Radical Construction Grammar. In this model, I have focused my attention on how much diversity there is in grammatical constructions across the world’s lan- guages. In the last three talks that I have given yesterday and this morning, I focused on the relationship between form and meaning. And I argued that when we look at meaning, we also have to respect essentially the diversity of languages and how they pay attention to meaning. And that in fact, when we look at the analysis of the meaning of linguistic forms, we have to take an ex- tremely fine-grained perspective on that, to describe the range of very specific kinds of situations that human beings express in language. And I concluded by arguing for essentially a radical version of what’s called the usage-based model in Cognitive Linguistics, the idea that the linguistic knowledge that speakers have is derived from language use.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Introduction
    1 Introduction Language is a complex system that consists of atomic units called morph- emes. These units can be combined in the form of structured entities called sentences. There are two main sorts of words: singular terms and predic- ates. While the former refer to individual objects, the latter refer to sets thereof. Words mean something by virtue of referring. Sentences mean something by virtue of expressing truths and falsehoods. Something along these lines is the orthodox view in formal semantics and the mainstream opinion conveyed in contemporary philosophy of lan- guage textbooks. The main thesis of this book is that this view is essentially wrong-headed. The opposite view to the above approach is usage-based semantics. This book explains why current strands in empirical research undermine formal semantic theories. Classications always imply simplications. I take it that one quite plaus- ible divide in the area of philosophical approaches to meaning is along the lines of ‘truth-conditional vs usage-based’. In the following chapters, I shall therefore present the current debate in philosophy guided by this distinc- tion. Truth-conditional semantics is the orthodox view in philosophy in two distinct respects. There are many explicit adherents of this research tradition, most notably Emma Borg, Ernest Lepore, and Kirk Ludwig.1 But then there are even more people who are implicitly committed to this view or related views. Although it is hard to prove this point, usage-based se- mantics is probably the minority view in current philosophy of language. The currently by far most inuential usage-based philosopher is Paul Hor- wich.
    [Show full text]