A Developmental Model of Syntax Acquisition in the Construction Grammar Framework with Cross-Linguistic Validation in English and Japanese

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Developmental Model of Syntax Acquisition in the Construction Grammar Framework with Cross-Linguistic Validation in English and Japanese A Developmental Model of Syntax Acquisition in the Construction Grammar Framework with Cross-Linguistic Validation in English and Japanese Peter Ford Dominey Toshio Inui Sequential Cognition and Language Group Graduate School of Informatics, Institut des Sciences Cognitives, CNRS Kyoto University, 69675 Bron CEDES, France Yoshida-honmachi, Sakyo-ku, 606-8501, [email protected] Kyoto, Japan [email protected] constructions in a generalized, generative manner. Abstract An alternative functionalist perspective holds The current research demonstrates a system that learning plays a much more central role in inspired by cognitive neuroscience and language acquisition. The infant develops an developmental psychology that learns to inventory of grammatical constructions as construct mappings between the grammatical mappings from form to meaning (Goldberg 1995). structure of sentences and the structure of their These constructions are initially rather fixed and meaning representations. Sentence to meaning specific, and later become generalized into a more mappings are learned and stored as abstract compositional form employed by the adult grammatical constructions. These are stored (Tomasello 1999, 2003). In this context, and retrieved from a construction inventory construction of the relation between perceptual and based on the constellation of closed class cognitive representations and grammatical form items uniquely identifying each construction. plays a central role in learning language (e.g. These learned mappings allow the system to Feldman et al. 1990, 1996; Langacker 1991; processes natural language sentences in order Mandler 1999; Talmy 1998). to reconstruct complex internal representations These issues of learnability and innateness have of the meanings these sentences describe. The provided a rich motivation for simulation studies system demonstrates error free performance that have taken a number of different forms. and systematic generalization for a rich subset Elman (1990) demonstrated that recurrent of English constructions that includes complex networks are sensitive to predictable structure in hierarchical grammatical structure, and grammatical sequences. Subsequent studies of generalizes systematically to new sentences of grammar induction demonstrate how syntactic the learned construction categories. Further testing demonstrates (1) the capability to structure can be recovered from sentences (e.g. accommodate a significantly extended set of Stolcke & Omohundro 1994). From the constructions, and (2) extension to Japanese, a “grounding of language in meaning” perspective free word order language that is structurally (e.g. Feldman et al. 1990, 1996; Langacker 1991; quite different from English, thus Goldberg 1995) Chang & Maia (2001) exploited demonstrating the extensibility of the structure the relations between action representation and mapping model. simple verb frames in a construction grammar approach. In effort to consider more complex 1 Introduction grammatical forms, Miikkulainen (1996) demonstrated a system that learned the mapping The nativist perspective on the problem of between relative phrase constructions and multiple language acquisition holds that the <sentence, event representations, based on the use of a stack meaning> data to which the child is exposed is for maintaining state information during the highly indeterminate, and underspecifies the processing of the next embedded clause in a mapping to be learned. This “poverty of the recursive manner. stimulus” is a central argument for the existence of In a more generalized approach, Dominey a genetically specified universal grammar, such (2000) exploited the regularity that sentence to that language acquisition consists of configuring meaning mapping is encoded in all languages by the UG for the appropriate target language word order and grammatical marking (bound or (Chomsky 1995). In this framework, once a given free) (Bates et al. 1982). That model was based on parameter is set, its use should apply to new 33 the functional neurophysiology of cognitive “gugle” to the object of push. sequence and language processing and an associated neural network model that has been WordToReferent(i,j) = WordToReferent(i,j) + demonstrated to simulate interesting aspects of OCA(k,i) * SEA(m,j) * infant (Dominey & Ramus 2000) and adult max(a, SentenceToScene(m,k)) (1) language processing (Dominey et al. 2003). 2.2 Open vs Closed Class Word Categories 2 Structure mapping for language learning Our approach is based on the cross-linguistic The mapping of sentence form onto meaning observation that open class words (e.g. nouns, (Goldberg 1995) takes place at two distinct levels verbs, adjectives and adverbs) are assigned to their in the current model: Words are associated with thematic roles based on word order and/or individual components of event descriptions, and grammatical function words or morphemes (Bates grammatical structure is associated with functional et al. 1982). Newborn infants are sensitive to the roles within scene events. The first level has been perceptual properties that distinguish these two addressed by Siskind (1996), Roy & Pentland categories (Shi et al. 1999), and in adults, these categories are processed by dissociable (2002) and Steels (2001) and we treat it here in a neurophysiological systems (Brown et al. 1999). relatively simple but effective manner. Our Similarly, artificial neural networks can also learn principle interest lies more in the second level of to make this function/content distinction (Morgan mapping between scene and sentence structure. et al. 1996). Thus, for the speech input that is Equations 1-7 implement the model depicted in provided to the learning model open and closed Figure 1, and are derived from a class words are directed to separate processing neurophysiologically motivated model of streams that preserve their order and identity, as sensorimotor sequence learning (Dominey et al. indicated in Figure 2. 2003). 2.1 Word Meaning Equation (1) describes the associative memory, WordToReferent, that links word vectors in the OpenClassArray (OCA) with their referent vectors in the SceneEventArray (SEA)1. In the initial learning phases there is no influence of syntactic knowledge and the word-referent associations are stored in the WordToReferent matrix (Eqn 1) by associating every word with every referent in the current scene (a = 1), exploiting the cross- situational regularity (Siskind 1996) that a given word will have a higher coincidence with referent to which it refers than with other referents. This initial word learning contributes to learning the mapping between sentence and scene structure (Eqn. 4, 5 & 6 below). Then, knowledge of the syntactic structure, encoded in SentenceToScene Figure 1. Structure-Mapping Architecture. 1. Lexical categorization. can be used to identify the appropriate referent (in 2. Open class words in Open Class Array are translated to Predicted the SEA) for a given word (in the OCA), Referents in the PRA via the WordtoReferent mapping. 3. PRA elements are mapped onto their roles in the SceneEventArray by the corresponding to a zero value of a in Eqn. 1. In SentenceToScene mapping, specific to each sentence type. 4. This this “syntactic bootstrapping” for the new word mapping is retrieved from Construction Inventory, via the ConstructionIndex that encodes the closed class words that “gugle,” for example, syntactic knowledge of characterize each grammatical construction type. Agent-Event-Object structure of the sentence 2.3 Mapping Sentence to Meaning “John pushed the gugle” can be used to assign Meanings are encoded in an event predicate, argument representation corresponding to the 1 In Eqn 1, the index k = 1 to 6, corresponding to the maximum SceneEventArray in Figure 1 (e.g. push(Block, number of words in the open class array (OCA). Index m = 1 to 6, corresponding to the maximum number of elements in the scene event triangle) for “The triangle pushed the block”). array (SEA). Indices i and j = 1 to 25, corresponding to the word and There, the sentence to meaning mapping can be scene item vector sizes, respectively. 34 characterized in the following successive steps. referents to scene elements (in SEA). The First, words in the Open Class Array are decoded resulting, SentenceToSceneCurrent encodes the into their corresponding scene referents (via the correspondence between word order (that is WordToReferent mapping) to yield the Predicted preserved in the PRA Eqn 2) and thematic roles in Referents Array that contains the translated words the SEA. Note that the quality of while preserving their original order from the OCA SentenceToSceneCurrent will depend on the (Eqn 2) 2. quality of acquired word meanings in WordToReferent. Thus, syntactic learning n requires a minimum baseline of semantic PRA(k,j) = OCA(k,i) * WordToReferent(i,j) (2) knowledge. i= 1 Next, each sentence type will correspond to a SentenceToSceneCurrent(m,k) = specific form to meaning mapping between the n (4) PRA and the SEA. encoded in the PRA(k,i)*SEA(m,i) SentenceToScene array. The problem will be to i=1 retrieve for each sentence type, the appropriate corresponding SentenceToScene mapping. To Given the SentenceToSceneCurrent mapping solve this problem, we recall that each sentence for the current sentence, we can now associate it in type will have a unique constellation of closed the ConstructionInventory with the corresponding class words and/or bound morphemes (Bates et al. function word configuration or
Recommended publications
  • Regular and Irregular Aspects of Grammatical Constructions Konrad Szcześniak, Palacký University, Olomouc, Czech Republic University of Silesia, Sosnowiec, Poland
    Regular and Irregular Aspects of Grammatical Constructions Konrad Szcześniak, Palacký University, Olomouc, Czech Republic University of Silesia, Sosnowiec, Poland This study focuses on two grammatical forms, the Incredulity Response Construction and the je vidět / vidieť construction. By analyzing their form and usage, I intend to weigh in on the debate concerning the importance of regularity and unpredictability inherent in grammatical constructions. The debate, initiated by Chomsky’s dismissal of idiosyncratic forms as peripheral, has gained momentum especially in recent years, with cognitive linguists uncovering more unpredictable aspects of language forms and claiming that language is primarily idiosyncratic, not regular. As a result, current cognitive linguistic descriptions of constructions highlight what is unpredictable and all but dismiss the regular aspects of constructions. The present study argues that the two sides—the predictable and the form-specific—coexist and thus have obvious implications for models of mental representations of language forms. By extrapolation, a recognition of a balance of the regular and irregular properties is relevant to our theorizing on how speakers learn and use constructions: just as the presence of idiosyncratic properties makes it necessary to memorize special cases of forms, the presence of regular properties can be assumed to ease the learning burden. An accurate description of any construction should attempt to detail both those aspects that can be predicted by rule and those that are endemic to a given construction and must be memorized. Keywords: grammatical construction, idiosyncrasy, regularity, IRC, je vidět / vidieť construction 1. Introduction The notions of irregularity, idiosyncrasy and unpredictability have received widely different treatments in major theoretical frameworks.
    [Show full text]
  • Sign-Based Construction Grammar: an Informal Synopsis IVA N A
    September 4, 2012 3 Sign-Based Construction Grammar: An Informal Synopsis IVA N A. SAG 1 Introduction This chapter1 is intended as an introduction to some of the central notions of Sign-Based Construction Grammar (SBCG).2 For a more general discussion of SBCG, including an informal, high-level summary of the framework, its historical development, motivation, and relation to other approaches to gram- mar, the reader is referred to Sag et al. this volume. 1.1 Preliminaries SBCG is a framework blending ideas developed over a quarter century of re- search in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG3) with those pre- 1Above all, I’d like to thank Paul Kay for hours of discussion and useful feedback at every stage in the seemingly endless process of writing this chapter. I’m also grateful to Chuck Fillmore and Laura Michaelis for many useful discussions of both content and exposition and for com- ments on an earlier draft, which was also been improved by the comments of Rui Chaves, Adele Goldberg, Bill Croft, Adam Przepiórkowski, Russell Lee-Goldman, and Hans Boas. In addition, discussions with the following people have been quite helpful: Farrell Ackerman, Emily Bender, Dan Flickinger, Mark Gawron, Andreas Kathol, Beth Levin, Bob Levine, Stefan Müller, Frank Van Eynde, Tom Wasow, Gert Webelhuth, and Steve Wechsler. None of the views expressed here should be attributed to any of these individuals; nor should they be held responsible for any remaining errors. 2SBCG owes a considerable debt to the implementation work carried out within CSLI’s LinGO Lab and the DELPHIN consortium, whose grammar development efforts have proceeded in paral- lel with the evolution of SBCG.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Cognitive Grammar
    COGNITIVE SCIENCE 10, l-40 (1986) An Introduction to Cognitive Grammar RONALD W. LANGACKER University of California, San Diego Cognitive grammar takes a nonstandard view of linguistic semantics and grammatical structure. Meaning is equated with conceptualization. Semantic structures are characterized relative to cognitive domains, and derive their value by construing the content of these domains in a specific fashion. Gram- mar is not a distinct level of linguistic representation, but reduces instead to the structuring and symbolization of conceptual content. All grammatical units are symbolic: Basic categories (e.g., noun ond verb) are held to be notionally definable, and grammatical rules are analyzed as symbolic units that are both complex ond schematic. These concepts permit a revealing account of gram- maticol composition with notable descriptive advantages. Despite the diversity of contemporary linguistic theory, certain fundamental views enjoy a rough consensus and are widely accepted without serious question. Points of general agreement include the following: (a) language is a self-contained system amenable to algorithmic characterization, with suf- ficient autonomy to be studied in essential isolation from broader cognitive concerns; (b) grammar (syntax in particular) is an independent aspect of lin- guistic structure distinct from both lexicon and semantics; and (c) if mean- ing falls within the purview of linguistic analysis, it is properly described by some type of formal logic based on truth conditions. Individual theorists would doubtlessly qualify their assent in various ways, but (a)-(c) certainly come much closer than their denials to representing majority opinion. What follows is a minority report. Since 1976, I have been developing a linguistic theory that departs quite radically from the assumptions of the currently predominant paradigm.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructions: a New Theoretical Approach to Language
    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.5 May 2003 219 Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language Adele E. Goldberg Linguistics Department, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801-0168, USA A new theoretical approach to language has emerged in the ‘core’ of language. Mainstream generative theory the past 10–15 years that allows linguistic observations argues further that the complexity of core language cannot about form–meaning pairings, known as ‘construc- be learned inductively by general cognitive mechanisms tions’, to be stated directly. Constructionist approaches and therefore learners must be hard-wired with principles aim to account for the full range of facts about that are specific to language (‘universal grammar’). language, without assuming that a particular subset of the data is part of a privileged ‘core’. Researchers in this Tenets of constructionist approaches field argue that unusual constructions shed light on Each basic tenet outlined below is shared by most more general issues, and can illuminate what is constructionist approaches. Each represents a major required for a complete account of language. divergence from the mainstream generative approach and, in many ways, a return to a more traditional view of Constructions – form and meaning pairings – have been language. the basis of major advances in the study of grammar since Tenet 1. All levels of description are understood to the days of Aristotle. Observations about specific linguistic involve pairings of form with semantic or discourse constructions have shaped our understanding of both function, including morphemes or words, idioms, particular languages and the nature of language itself. But partially lexically filled and fully abstract phrasal only recently has a new theoretical approach emerged that patterns.
    [Show full text]
  • A Neurolinguistic Model of Grammatical Construction Processing
    A Neurolinguistic Model of Grammatical Construction Processing Peter Ford Dominey1, Michel Hoen1, and Toshio Inui2 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/18/12/2088/1756017/jocn.2006.18.12.2088.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & One of the functions of everyday human language is to com- construction processing based on principles of psycholinguis- municate meaning. Thus, when one hears or reads the sen- tics, (2) develop a model of how these functions can be imple- tence, ‘‘John gave a book to Mary,’’ some aspect of an event mented in human neurophysiology, and then (3) demonstrate concerning the transfer of possession of a book from John to the feasibility of the resulting model in processing languages of Mary is (hopefully) transmitted. One theoretical approach to typologically diverse natures, that is, English, French, and Japa- language referred to as construction grammar emphasizes this nese. In this context, particular interest will be directed toward link between sentence structure and meaning in the form of the processing of novel compositional structure of relative grammatical constructions. The objective of the current re- phrases. The simulation results are discussed in the context of search is to (1) outline a functional description of grammatical recent neurophysiological studies of language processing. & INTRODUCTION ditransitive (e.g., Mary gave my mom a new recipe) One of the long-term quests of cognitive neuroscience constructions (Goldberg, 1995). has been to link functional aspects of language process- In this context, the ‘‘usage-based’’ perspective holds ing to its underlying neurophysiology, that is, to under- that the infant begins language acquisition by learning stand how neural mechanisms allow the mapping of the very simple constructions in a progressive development surface structure of a sentence onto a conceptual repre- of processing complexity, with a substantial amount of sentation of its meaning.
    [Show full text]
  • Introducing Sign-Based Construction Grammar IVA N A
    September 4, 2012 1 Introducing Sign-Based Construction Grammar IVA N A. SAG,HANS C. BOAS, AND PAUL KAY 1 Background Modern grammatical research,1 at least in the realms of morphosyntax, in- cludes a number of largely nonoverlapping communities that have surpris- ingly little to do with one another. One – the Universal Grammar (UG) camp – is mainly concerned with a particular view of human languages as instantia- tions of a single grammar that is fixed in its general shape. UG researchers put forth highly abstract hypotheses making use of a complex system of repre- sentations, operations, and constraints that are offered as a theory of the rich biological capacity that humans have for language.2 This community eschews writing explicit grammars of individual languages in favor of offering conjec- tures about the ‘parameters of variation’ that modulate the general grammat- ical scheme. These proposals are motivated by small data sets from a variety of languages. A second community, which we will refer to as the Typological (TYP) camp, is concerned with descriptive observations of individual languages, with particular concern for idiosyncrasies and complexities. Many TYP re- searchers eschew formal models (or leave their development to others), while others in this community refer to the theory they embrace as ‘Construction Grammar’ (CxG). 1For comments and valuable discussions, we are grateful to Bill Croft, Chuck Fillmore, Adele Goldberg, Stefan Müller, and Steve Wechsler. We also thank the people mentioned in footnote 8 below. 2The nature of these representations has changed considerably over the years. Seminal works include Chomsky 1965, 1973, 1977, 1981, and 1995.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Grammar with Reference to Passive Construction in Arabic
    International Journal of Linguistics, Literature and Culture (LLC) March 2017 edition Vol.4 No.1 ISSN 2410-6577 Cognitive Grammar with Reference to Passive Construction in Arabic Dr. Bahaa-eddin Hassan Faculty of Arts, Sohag University, Egypt Abstract The purpose of this study is to examine Cognitive Grammar (CG) theory with reference to the active and the passive voice construction in Arabic. It shows how the cognitive approach to linguistics and construction grammar work together to explain motivation beyond the use of this syntactic construction; therefore, formal structures of language are combined with the cognitive dimension. CG characterizations reveal the limitations of the view that grammatical constructions are autonomous categories. They are interrelated with conceptualization in the framework of cognitive grammar. The study also accounts for the verbs which have active form and passive meaning, and the verbs which have passive form and active meaning. Keywords: Cognitive Grammar, Voice, Arabic, Theme-oriented. 1. Introduction In his Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Chomsky represented each sentence in a language as having a surface structure and a deep structure. The deep structure represents the semantic component and the surface structure represents the phonological approach. He explained that languages’ deep structures share universal properties which surface structures do not reveal. He proposed transformational grammar to map the deep structure (semantic relations of a sentence) on to the surface structure. Individual languages use different grammatical patterns for their particular set of expressed meanings. Chomsky, then, in Language and Mind, proposed that language relates to mind. Later, in an interview, he emphasized that language represents a state of mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Classes in Radical Construction Grammar
    Word classes in Radical Construction Grammar William Croft, University of New Mexico 1. Introduction Radical Construction Grammar (Croft 2001, 2005a, 2013) is a construction-based theory of grammar that allows for the representation of both cross-linguistic and language- internal grammatical diversity. Radical Construction Grammar analyses conform to the following three basic principles: (1) Word classes and other syntactic structures are language-specific and construction-specific. What is universal is patterns of variation in the verbalization of experience, represented for example in conceptual space. (2) The internal structure of the morphosyntactic form of constructions consists solely of the part-whole relation between construction roles (“slots”) and the whole construction. The complexity of constructions rests in the symbolic relations between the roles and their meanings/functions, and the rich semantic/functional structure expressed by the construction and its parts. (3) The morphosyntactic forms of constructions are language-specific, that is, there is potentially gradient variation of constructional form across and within languages. Radical Construction Grammar (Croft 2001) and Morphosyntax: Constructions of the World’s Languages (Croft, in prep.) propose many specific analyses of constructions and universals of constructional variation. While those analyses may and likely will need revision as knowledge of the constructions of the world’s languages increases, any analysis that conforms to the principles in (1)-(3) can be considered
    [Show full text]
  • Generalizing Beyond the Input: the Functions of the Constructions Matter ⇑ Florent Perek , Adele E
    Journal of Memory and Language 84 (2015) 108–127 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Generalizing beyond the input: The functions of the constructions matter ⇑ Florent Perek , Adele E. Goldberg Princeton University, Department of Psychology, Peretsman-Scully Hall, Princeton, NJ 08540, United States article info abstract Article history: A growing emphasis on statistics in language learning raises the question of whether and Received 4 June 2014 when speakers use language in ways that go beyond the statistical regularities in the input. revision received 27 April 2015 In this study, two groups were exposed to six novel verbs and two novel word order constructions that differed in function: one construction but not the other was exclusively used with pronoun undergoers. The distributional structure of the input was manipulated Keywords: between groups according to whether each verb was used exclusively in one or the other Language acquisition construction (the lexicalist condition), or whether a minority of verbs was witnessed in Artificial language learning both constructions (the alternating condition). Production and judgments results Novel construction learning Statistical learning demonstrate that participants tended to generalize the constructions for use in appropriate Argument structure discourse contexts, ignoring evidence of verb-specific behavior, especially in the alternating Generalization condition. Our results suggest that construction learning
    [Show full text]
  • Retrieving Occurrences of Grammatical Constructions
    Retrieving Occurrences of Grammatical Constructions Anna Ehrlemark and Richard Johansson and Benjamin Lyngfelt University of Gothenburg [email protected], [email protected] [email protected] Abstract Finding authentic examples of grammatical constructions is central in constructionist approaches to linguistics, language processing, and second language learning. In this paper, we address this problem as an information retrieval (IR) task. To facilitate research in this area, we built a benchmark collection by annotating the occurrences of six constructions in a Swedish corpus. Furthermore, we implemented a simple and flexible retrieval system for finding construction occurrences, in which the user specifies a ranking function using lexical-semantic similarities (lexicon-based or distributional). The system was evaluated using standard IR metrics on the new benchmark, and we saw that lexical-semantical rerankers improve significantly over a purely surface-oriented system, but must be carefully tailored for each individual construction. 1 Introduction Linguistic theories based on the notion of constructions are a recent development in linguistics, which has revitalized the discussion in fields such as syntax, lexicography, and argument structure theory. In particular, it is useful for describing partially schematic constructions: templatic patterns that exhibit lexical as well as syntactic properties, which are too specific to be referred to general grammar rules, but too general to be attributed to specific lexical units, which is a reason why they historically have been overlooked (Fillmore et al., 1988). These constructions are very frequent, and they are not only theoretically interesting but also important in language teaching (De Knoop and Gilquin, 2016), since they are challenging for second language learners (Prentice and Skoldberg,¨ 2011).
    [Show full text]
  • Argument Structure Constructions by Adele Eva Goldberg BA
    Argument Structure Constructions by Adele Eva Goldberg B.A. (University of Pennsylvania) 1985 M .A (University of California at Berkeley) 1989 DISSERTATION Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA at BERKELEY Approved: Chain v w A ****************************** Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. To the memory of m y father, Mel Goldberg, fo r showing me by example what it was to love to learn, and for better or worse, to ignore the odds, And to my mom, Ann Goldberg, for her easy laughter, her wisdom and her ele­ gance, and for being a pillar of love and support from day one. ii Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. C ontents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 A Brief Introduction to Construction G ram m ar ..................................... 6 l.*2 On the Need to Recognize Constructions ................................................... 9 1.2.1 To Preserve C om p o sitio n ality .......................................................... 9 1.2.2 Subtle Semantic Differences Between Constructions .................. 16 1.2.3 Circularity is Avoided ....................................................................... 21 2 The Interaction Between Verbs Sc Constructions 24 2.1 Frame-semantics ............................................................................................... 26 2.2 The
    [Show full text]
  • A Construction Grammar Approach * Yoon-Suk Chung
    Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach * Yoon-Suk Chung B.A. (Seoul National University) 1985 M.A. (Seoul National University) 1988 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 1992 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Paul Kay, Chair Professor Charles J. Fillmore Professor Yoko Hasegawa Spring 2001 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach Copyright 2001 by Yoon-Suk Chung Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1 Abstract Tough construction in English: a Construction Grammar approach by Yoon-Suk Chung Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Paul Kay, Chair This study proposes a new analysis of the Tough construction (TC) in the English language. This analysis is couched within the non-modular, non- derivational, unification and inheritance-based framework of Construction Grammar (CG). There are five major theoretical issues around which the study of the TC has revolved: (1) the identification of the TC in generative syntax as a species of either extraction or control; (2) the characterization of the tough predicate as either a raising or a control predicate; (3) the analysis of the/or-phrase as either a PP or a complementizer + lower subject; and the classification of the tough infinitival phrase as (4) a VP, an IP, or a CP, and as (5) either a complement or an adjunct.
    [Show full text]