Behavioral Ecology Invited Commentaries

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Behavioral Ecology Invited Commentaries Behavioral The official journal of the Ecology ISBE International Society for Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2014), 25(3), 464–469. Invited Commentaries Confusion and illusion: understanding Merilaita 2009). If nothing has been detected because the visual visual traits and behavior. A comment on system fails to differentiate object versus background, then presum- Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/25/3/464/510898 by guest on 28 September 2021 Kelley and Kelley ably there is no perception of the shape of the object, because the viewer is unaware of that object’s existence. A further example Martin Stevens is distractive camouflage. Kelley and Kelley outline work testing Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn whether distractive markings draw the viewer’s eye to high-contrast Campus, Penryn TR10 9FE, UK nonmarginal markings, thus preventing the observer from detect- ing the body outline. However, as normally described, this does not Most of us have been fooled by visual illusions. Thousands exist to seem to constitute an illusion because it refocuses gaze or attention fool human perception, creating the impression of objects that do toward one object feature at the expense of another by manipulat- not exist, are distorted in size, shape, or color, or suggest movement ing attention, although Kelley and Kelley’s work may challenge us where none exists. Whether animals have specifically evolved illusion- to think more carefully about how distractive features may work. ary traits to exploit others is not a new suggestion, but Kelley and It is also not clear whether traits that manipulate receiver behav- Kelley’s (2014) paper is probably the first to systematically discuss ior or overstimulate/inactivate receiver responses should be exam- such a concept and to present a clear review of where illusions may ples of illusions. Clearly, there is likely to be a continuum and so exist in nature and how they might work. Their paper brings to the pigeon-holing can be artificial, but it is important to be clear in forefront this exciting and significantly understudied area of biology. order to carefully guide future work. For example, if the circular Here, I focus mostly on visual illusions and communication. eyespots found on many animals that Kelley and Kelley discuss Gregory (1998) discusses illusions in depth and describes them as do mimic eyes (although supporting evidence is currently lacking; occurring when perception departs from the reality of the external Stevens and Ruxton 2014), should they and other types of mim- world. Akin to Gregory, Kelley and Kelley define illusions as traits icry be considered as illusions? I argue that mimicry would usually that “…act to distort the perception of the viewer such as the size, not constitute an illusion because it involves a direct resemblance to shape, coloration or movement…” of individuals or parts of individ- another object, and this is not the same as distorting perspective or uals. How we define illusions is a fundamental factor in how future creating the impression of something that is not really there. work may proceed and how we may interpret many animal traits. The above debates matter because if we are to understand com- Most of the potential examples of illusions that Kelley and munication and signals we need to comprehend the mechanisms Kelley (2014) present, I am in agreement with. For example, they by which they work. The issue of what is an illusion is not simple discuss potential cases involving the perceived size or color of sig- because perception is something quite specific to the sensory sys- nals and individuals, including where well known illusions exist (e.g., tems and brains of each animal. At the moment, more behavioral Ebbinghaus illusion). Likewise, illusions of perspective, such as in experiments are needed to demonstrate that other species really do recent bowerbird courtship studies (Endler et al. 2010), or contour perceive illusions, especially of more naturalistic stimuli. Kelley and completion in animals, seem well explained as illusions. Kelley and Kelley’s (2014) definition seems sensible and a good start in deter- Kelley also discuss motion dazzle, whereby the presence of stripes or mining which traits are illusions. zigzag markings found on fish, snakes, and some other animals could Finally, as Kelley and Kelley (2014) point out, arrangements of distort the observer’s ability to judge the speed and direction of object color and brightness can affect the perception of color patches movement (e.g., Stevens et al. 2008). Along with deterring biting flies, adjacent to one another. We need to pay more attention to the motion dazzle is a leading contender to explain the illusive function of composite arrangement of animal and plant markings, rather zebra stripes, and recent modeling of motion vision shows that zebra than focusing on individual elements in isolation. Studies of com- stripes may interfere with motion assessment (How and Zanker 2013). munication and signaling commonly analyze the color of a single Despite Kelley and Kelley’s (2014) valuable attempts to define patch and correlate this with some aspect of behavior and then and conceptualize illusions and relate them to animal traits and infer what information the receiver may have extracted. Yet, if sig- behavior, conceptual refinement is still needed in terms of what nal color patches operate synergistically, then this approach could animal traits we consider as illusions. This is no bad thing because misinterpret the information available and underestimate the effi- it can lead to future discussion and study. For example, Kelley and cacy of the signal. In short, we need to consider the 2D and 3D Kelley suggest that disruptive coloration, which works by destroying properties of animal signals, and studies of the strategic aspects the outline of a body or body parts, could be viewed as an illusion of signaling need to more commonly consider issues of signal effi- of shape. Although I can see how this may be a possibility were dis- cacy and multicomponent communication. In conspicuous signals, ruptive coloration to work by preventing recognition, it is normally for example, we would expect adjacent color patches that stimu- regarded as a type of crypsis that prevents detection (Stevens and late different opponent color channels or sets of receptors (such as © The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] Stevens • Confusion and illusion 465 ultraviolet-blue patches found next to fluorescent yellow patches on receivers. Are they really illusions, effects that act to distort the perception some parrots; Pearn et al. 2001). In short, understanding the colors of the viewer as defined by Kelley and Kelley (2014), or more simply present in signals requires considering the coloration of the animal optimization of signal efficacy? as a whole (and the background). Clearly, there is much left to do to One may wonder if the concept of illusion is different from the understand visual traits. exploitation of perceptual biases developed by Schaefer and Ruxton (2009), which also considers sensory, neuronal, and higher cogni- tive processes involved in the evolution of traits of the sender that FUNDING exploit perceptual biases of the receiver? Maybe not, even if this M.S. was supported by a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences model has mainly been used to explain deception by mimicry, it Research Council (BBSRC) David Phillips Research Fellowship can also be applied to the examples of illusions advanced by Kelley (BB/G022887/1). and Kelley (2014). The concept of animal illusions is within the range of sensory exploitation, bias, and traps. However, those Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/25/3/464/510898 by guest on 28 September 2021 I thank G. Ruxton for helpful comments on a version of this paper. have mainly been used in the study of female preferences and mate choice, whereas illusions in their present broad sense also Address correspondence to M. Stevens. E-mail: [email protected]. encompass nonsexually selected traits like disruptive coloration, countershading, distractive markings, or motion dazzle. This con- Received 13 January 2014; revised 15 January 2014; accepted 15 January 2014; Advance Access publication 17 February 2014. cept, to my opinion, could even be extended to several other cases and sensory modalities, for example, from aggressive mimicry of doi:10.1093/beheco/aru013 bolas spiders which create the illusion of presence of female moths Forum editor: Sue Healy by mimicking their pheromones to prey on males (Stowe et al. 1987), to aggressive mimicry of katydids which create the illusion REFERENCES of receptive female presence by mimicking their wing sounds to capture males (Marshall and Hill 2009) or even to orchids which Endler JA, Endler LC, Doerr NR. 2010. Great bowerbirds create the- attract pollinators by visual and olfactory illusions of food, mates, aters with forced perspective when seen by their audience. Curr Biol. and oviposition sites (Jersakova et al. 2006; Gaskett 2011). 20:1679–1684. Gregory RL. 1998. Eye and brain: the psychology of seeing. Oxford: Using the term illusion brings a focus on the neuronal, psycho- Oxford University Press. logical, and cognitive processes implicated in visual perception of How MJ, Zanker JM. 2013. Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes. naturally and sexually selected traits, which are rarely considered Zoology. doi: 10.1016/j.zool.2013.10.004. in nonhuman communication. This approach will certainly con- Kelley LA, Kelley JL. 2014. Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. Behav Ecol. 25:450–463. tribute to significant advances in the field, like the recent work of Pearn SM, Bennett ATD, Cuthill IC. 2001. Ultraviolet vision, fluorescence motion dazzle studied on locust neurons by Santer (2013).
Recommended publications
  • Mimicry and Defense
    3/24/2015 Professor Donald McFarlane Mimicry and Defense Protective Strategies Camouflage (“Cryptic coloration”) Diverse Coloration Diversion Structures Startle Structures 2 1 3/24/2015 Camouflage (“Cryptic coloration”) Minimize 3d shape, e.g. flatfish Halibut (Hippoglossus hippoglossus) 3 4 2 3/24/2015 Counter‐Shading 5 Disruptive Coloration 6 3 3/24/2015 Polymorphism – Cepeae snails 7 Polymorphism – Oophaga granuliferus 8 4 3/24/2015 Polymorphism – 9 Polymorphism – Oophaga Geographic locations of study populations and their color patterns. (A) Map of the pacific coast of Colombia showing the three study localities: in blue Oophaga histrionica, in orange O. lehmanni, and in green the pHYB population. (B) Examples of color patterns of individuals from the pHYB population (1–4) and the pattern from a hybrid between Oophaga histrionica and O. lehmanni bred in the laboratory (H) 10 5 3/24/2015 Diversion Structures 11 Startle Structures 12 6 3/24/2015 Warning Coloration (Aposematic coloration) Advertise organism as distasteful, toxic or venomous Problem: Predators must learn by attacking prey; predator learning is costly to prey. Therefore strong selective pressure to STANDARDIZE on a few colors/patterns. This is MULLERIAN MIMICRY. Most common is yellow/black, or red/yellow/black 13 Warning Coloration (Aposematic coloration) Bumblebee (Bombus Black and yellow mangrove snake (Boiga sp.) Sand Wasp (bembix oculata) dendrophila) Yellow‐banded poison dart frog (Dendrobates leucomelas Fire salamander ( Salamandra salamandra) 14 7 3/24/2015 Warning Coloration (Aposematic coloration) coral snakes (Micrurus sp.) ~ 50 species in two families, all venomous 15 Batesian Mimicry 1862 –Henry Walter Bates; “A Naturalist on the River Amazons” 16 8 3/24/2015 Batesian Mimicry Batesian mimics “cheat” –they lack toxins, venom, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Predatory Behavior of Jumping Spiders
    Annual Reviews www.annualreviews.org/aronline Annu Rev. Entomol. 19%. 41:287-308 Copyrighl8 1996 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved PREDATORY BEHAVIOR OF JUMPING SPIDERS R. R. Jackson and S. D. Pollard Department of Zoology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand KEY WORDS: salticids, salticid eyes, Portia, predatory versatility, aggressive mimicry ABSTRACT Salticids, the largest family of spiders, have unique eyes, acute vision, and elaborate vision-mediated predatory behavior, which is more pronounced than in any other spider group. Diverse predatory strategies have evolved, including araneophagy,aggressive mimicry, myrmicophagy ,and prey-specific preycatch- ing behavior. Salticids are also distinctive for development of behavioral flexi- bility, including conditional predatory strategies, the use of trial-and-error to solve predatory problems, and the undertaking of detours to reach prey. Predatory behavior of araneophagic salticids has undergone local adaptation to local prey, and there is evidence of predator-prey coevolution. Trade-offs between mating and predatory strategies appear to be important in ant-mimicking and araneo- phagic species. INTRODUCTION With over 4000 described species (1 l), jumping spiders (Salticidae) compose by Fordham University on 04/13/13. For personal use only. the largest family of spiders. They are characterized as cursorial, diurnal predators with excellent eyesight. Although spider eyes usually lack the struc- tural complexity required for acute vision, salticids have unique, complex eyes with resolution abilities without known parallels in animals of comparable size Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1996.41:287-308. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org (98). Salticids are the end-product of an evolutionary process in which a small silk-producing animal with a simple nervous system acquires acute vision, resulting in a diverse array of complex predatory strategies.
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM
    Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM Mimicry David W. Kikuchi, David W. Pfennig Introduction Among nature’s most exquisite adaptations are examples in which natural selection has favored a species (the mimic) to resemble a second, often unrelated species (the model) because it confuses a third species (the receiver). For example, the individual members of a nontoxic species that happen to resemble a toxic species may dupe any predators by behaving as if they are also dangerous and should therefore be avoided. In this way, adaptive resemblances can evolve via natural selection. When this phenomenon—dubbed “mimicry”—was first outlined by Henry Walter Bates in the middle of the 19th century, its intuitive appeal was so great that Charles Darwin immediately seized upon it as one of the finest examples of evolution by means of natural selection. Even today, mimicry is often used as a prime example in textbooks and in the popular press as a superlative example of natural selection’s efficacy. Moreover, mimicry remains an active area of research, and studies of mimicry have helped illuminate such diverse topics as how novel, complex traits arise; how new species form; and how animals make complex decisions. General Overviews Since Henry Walter Bates first published his theories of mimicry in 1862 (see Bates 1862, cited under Historical Background), there have been periodic reviews of our knowledge in the subject area. Cott 1940 was mainly concerned with animal coloration. Subsequent reviews, such as Edmunds 1974 and Ruxton, et al. 2004, have focused on types of mimicry associated with defense from predators.
    [Show full text]
  • Motion Dazzle and the Effects of Target Patterning on Capture Success
    BMC Evolutionary Biology This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance. Fully formatted PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon. Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success BMC Evolutionary Biology 2014, 14:201 doi:10.1186/s12862-014-0201-4 Anna Hughes ([email protected]) Jolyon Troscianko ([email protected]) Martin Stevens ([email protected]) Sample ISSN 1471-2148 Article type Research article Submission date 5 June 2014 Acceptance date 9 September 2014 Article URL http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2148/14/201 Like all articles in BMC journals, this peer-reviewed article can be downloaded, printed and distributed freely for any purposes (see copyright notice below). Articles in BMC journals are listed in PubMed and archived at PubMed Central. For information about publishing your research in BMC journals or any BioMed Central journal, go to http://www.biomedcentral.com/info/authors/ © Hughes et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success
    [Show full text]
  • About the Book the Format Acknowledgments
    About the Book For more than ten years I have been working on a book on bryophyte ecology and was joined by Heinjo During, who has been very helpful in critiquing multiple versions of the chapters. But as the book progressed, the field of bryophyte ecology progressed faster. No chapter ever seemed to stay finished, hence the decision to publish online. Furthermore, rather than being a textbook, it is evolving into an encyclopedia that would be at least three volumes. Having reached the age when I could retire whenever I wanted to, I no longer needed be so concerned with the publish or perish paradigm. In keeping with the sharing nature of bryologists, and the need to educate the non-bryologists about the nature and role of bryophytes in the ecosystem, it seemed my personal goals could best be accomplished by publishing online. This has several advantages for me. I can choose the format I want, I can include lots of color images, and I can post chapters or parts of chapters as I complete them and update later if I find it important. Throughout the book I have posed questions. I have even attempt to offer hypotheses for many of these. It is my hope that these questions and hypotheses will inspire students of all ages to attempt to answer these. Some are simple and could even be done by elementary school children. Others are suitable for undergraduate projects. And some will take lifelong work or a large team of researchers around the world. Have fun with them! The Format The decision to publish Bryophyte Ecology as an ebook occurred after I had a publisher, and I am sure I have not thought of all the complexities of publishing as I complete things, rather than in the order of the planned organization.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage & Mimicry
    Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage & Mimicry OCN 201 Biology Lecture 11 http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/2005/03/24_octopus.shtml Symbiosis • Parasitism - negative effect on host • Commensalism - no effect on host • Mutualism - both parties benefit Often involves food but benefits may also include protection from predators, dispersal, or habitat Parasitism Leeches (Segmented Worms) Tongue Louse (Crustacean) Nematodes (Roundworms) Commensalism or Mutualism? Anemone shrimp http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ Anemone fish http://www.scuba-equipment-usa.com/marine/APR04/ Mutualism Cleaner Shrimp and Eel http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ Whale Barnacles & Lice What kinds of symbioses are these? Commensal Parasite Camouflage • Often important for predators and prey to avoid being seen • Predators to catch their prey and prey to hide from their predators • Camouflage: Passive or adaptive Passive Camouflage Countershading Sharks Birds Countershading coloration of the Caribbean reef shark © George Ryschkewitsch Fish JONATHAN CHESTER Mammals shiftingbaselines.org/blog/big_tuna.jpg http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/pr/images/cetaceans/orca_spyhopping-noaa.jpg Passive Camouflage http://www.cspangler.com/images/photos/aquarium/weedy-sea-dragon2.jpg Adaptive Camouflage Camouflage by Accessorizing Decorator crab Friday Harbor Marine Health Observatory http://www.projectnoah.org/ Camouflage by Mimicry http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/2005/03/24_octopus.shtml Mimicry • Animals can gain protection (or even access to prey) by looking
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry: Ecology, Evolution, and Development
    Editorial Mimicry: Ecology, evolution, and development David PFENNIG, Guest Editor Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Coker Hall, CB#3280, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 USA, [email protected] 1 Introduction 1879), multiple undesirable species (e.g., toxic species) converge on the same warning signal, thereby sharing Mimicry occurs when one species (the “mimic”) the cost of educating predators about their undesirabil- evolves to resemble a second species (the “model”) be- ity. cause of the selective benefits associated with confusing Mimicry is among the most active research areas in a third species (the “receiver”). For example, natural all of evolutionary biology, in part because of the highly selection can favor phenotypic convergence between integrative nature that the study of mimicry necessarily completely unrelated species when an edible species entails. Mimicry involves asking both functional ques- receives the benefit of reduced predation by resembling tions (it involves investigating, for example, the adap- an inedible species that predators avoid. tive significance of more versus less precise resem- Research into mimicry has a rich history that traces blance between models and mimics) and mechanistic back to the beginnings of modern evolutionary biology. ones (it also involves investigating, for example, how In 1862––a scant three years after Darwin had published mimetic phenotypes are produced). Thus, mimicry re- The Origin of Species––Henry Walter Bates (1862), an search draws on diverse fields, many of which are on English explorer and naturalist, first suggested that close the cutting edge of biological research. Indeed, as Bro- resemblances between unrelated species could evolve as die and Brodie (2004, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Deceptive Coloration - Natureworks 01/04/20, 11:58 AM
    Deceptive Coloration - NatureWorks 01/04/20, 11:58 AM Deceptive Coloration Deceptive coloration is when an organism's color Mimicry fools either its predators or its prey. There are two Some animals and plants look like other things -- types of deceptive coloration: camouflage and they mimic them. Mimicry is another type of mimicry. deceptive coloration. It can protect the mimic from Camouflage predators or hide the mimic from prey. If mimicry was a play, there would be three characters. The Model - the species or object that is copied. The Mimic - looks and acts like another species or object. The Dupe- the tricked predator or prey. The poisonous Camouflage helps an organism blend in with its coral snake surroundings. Camouflage can be colors or and the patterns or both. When organisms are harmless camouflaged, they are harder to find. This means king snake predators have to spend a longer time finding can look a them. That's a waste of energy! When a predator is lot alike. camouflaged, it makes it easier to sneak up on or Predators surprise its prey. will avoid Blending In: Stripes or Solids? the king snake because they think it is poisonous. This type There are of mimicry is called Batesian mimicry. In lots of Batesian mimicry a harmless species mimics a different toxic or dangerous species. examples of The viceroy butterfly and monarch butterfly were once thought to camouflage. Some colors and patterns help exhibit animals blend into areas with light and shadow. Batesian The tiger's stripes help it blend into tall grass. Its mimicry golden brown strips blend in with the grass and the where a dark brown and black stripes merge with darker harmless shadows.
    [Show full text]
  • Antipredator Deception in Terrestrial Vertebrates
    Current Zoology 60 (1): 16–25, 2014 Antipredator deception in terrestrial vertebrates Tim CARO* Department of Wildlife, Fish and Conservation Biology, and Center of Population Biology, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA Abstract Deceptive antipredator defense mechanisms fall into three categories: depriving predators of knowledge of prey’s presence, providing cues that deceive predators about prey handling, and dishonest signaling. Deceptive defenses in terrestrial vertebrates include aspects of crypsis such as background matching and countershading, visual and acoustic Batesian mimicry, active defenses that make animals seem more difficult to handle such as increase in apparent size and threats, feigning injury and death, distractive behaviours, and aspects of flight. After reviewing these defenses, I attempt a preliminary evaluation of which aspects of antipredator deception are most widespread in amphibians, reptiles, mammals and birds [Current Zoology 60 (1): 16 25, 2014]. Keywords Amphibians, Birds, Defenses, Dishonesty, Mammals, Prey, Reptiles 1 Introduction homeotherms may increase the distance between prey and the pursuing predator or dupe the predator about the In this paper I review forms of deceptive antipredator flight path trajectory, or both (FitzGibbon, 1990). defenses in terrestrial vertebrates, a topic that has been Last, an antipredator defense may be a dishonest largely ignored for 25 years (Pough, 1988). I limit my signal. Bradbury and Vehrencamp (2011) state that “true scope to terrestrial organisms because lighting condi- deception occurs when a sender produces a signal tions in water are different from those in the air and whose reception will benefit it at the expense of the antipredator strategies often differ in the two environ- receiver regardless of the condition with which the sig- ments.
    [Show full text]
  • A Specialised Hunting Strategy Used to Overcome Dangerous Spider Prey
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Nest usurpation: a specialised hunting strategy used to overcome dangerous spider prey Received: 18 January 2019 Ondřej Michálek 1, Yael Lubin 2 & Stano Pekár 1 Accepted: 14 March 2019 Hunting other predators is dangerous, as the tables can turn and the hunter may become the hunted. Published: xx xx xxxx Specialized araneophagic (spider eating) predators have evolved intriguing hunting strategies that allow them to invade spiders’ webs by adopting a stealthy approach or using aggressive mimicry. Here, we present a newly discovered, specialized hunting strategy of the araneophagic spider Poecilochroa senilis (Araneae: Gnaphosidae), which forces its way into the silk retreat of the potential spider prey and immobilizes it by swathing gluey silk onto its forelegs and mouthparts. Poecilochroa senilis has been reported from the nests of a several, often large, spider species in the Negev desert (Israel), suggesting specialization on spiders as prey. Nevertheless, in laboratory experiments, we found that P. senilis has a wider trophic niche, and fed readily on several small insect species. The specialized nest-invading attack was used more frequently with large spiders, and even small juvenile P. senilis were able to attack and subdue larger spiders. Our observations show that specifc hunting tactics, like nest usurpation, allow specialized predators to overcome defences of dangerous prey. Evolutionary arms races between prey and predators lead to the evolution of various defence mechanisms of the prey and counter-adaptations of predators to subdue such a prey1. Predator-prey arms races are ofen asym- metrical, as a prey organism is under stronger selection pressure2.
    [Show full text]
  • 7 X 11.5 Long Title.P65
    Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-15257-0 - Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function Edited by Martin Stevens and Sami Merilaita Excerpt More information 1 Animal camouflage Function and mechanisms Martin Stevens and Sami Merilaita 1.1 Introduction One cannot help being impressed by the near-perfect camouflage of a moth matching the colour and pattern of the tree on which it rests, or of the many examples in nature of animals resembling other objects in order to be hidden (Figure 1.1). The Nobel Prize winning ethologist Niko Tinbergen referred to such moths as ‘bark with wings’ (Tinbergen 1974), such was the impressiveness of their camouflage. On a basic level, camouflage can be thought of as the property of an object that renders it difficult to detect or recognise by virtue of its similarity to its environment (Stevens & Merilaita 2009a). The advantage of being concealed from predators (or sometimes from prey) is easy to understand, and camouflage has long been used as a classical example of natural selection. Perhaps for this reason, until recently, camouflage was subject to little rigorous experimentation – its function and value seemed obvious. However, like any theory, the possible advantages of camouflage, and how it works, need rigorous scientific testing. Furthermore, as we shall see below and in this book in general, the concept of concealment is much richer, more complex and interesting than scientists originally thought. The natural world is full of amazing examples of camouflage, with the strategies employed diverse and sometimes extraordinary (Figure 1.2). These include using mark- ings to match the colour and pattern of the background, as do various moths (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Textiles and Polymers
    Part A: English Edition Journal of Textiles and Polymers Vol. 9, No. 3, 21-28, July 2021 http://dx.doi.org/10.48302/jtp.2021.136075 ORIGINAL PAPER Multi-dimensional Analytical Base Method for Evaluating Camouflage Patterns Elaheh Daneshvar, Mohammad Amani Tehrani*, and Fatemeh Zeighami Received: 12 December 2020, Accepted: 17 April 2021 Abstract- Precise evaluation of camouflage patterns is very a camouflage effect to safeguard the army forces in battles. important to achieve an effective protective cover. Recently, Although the early military uniforms had no camouflage researchers have focused on proposing computational pattern, the recently produced uniforms contain camouflage methods for camouflage evaluation using algorithms of patterns due to the development of military tactics [1]. image feature extraction. Although the available indexes Therefore, the military forces attempt to produce more determine the similarity of camouflage patterns to the successful camouflage patterns concerning the strategic environmental background, they generally suffer from a regions. In this regard, the evaluation of camouflage lack of quantified camouflage principles. The main idea of this paper is to propose a new evaluation metric by defining patterns is an important step to produce an effective seven camouflage factors to evaluate camouflage patterns. camouflage pattern [2]. Researchers proposed different To this end, several conceptual factors of camouflage quantitative metrics based on image feature extraction to that are vital for a camouflage pattern were defined. evaluate camouflage patterns [2-8]. Accordingly, if a pattern does not contain the mentioned Indeed, all the available evaluation methods of camouflage factors, it cannot be considered as an effective camouflage patterns are computable approaches based protective pattern.
    [Show full text]