<<

Appendix: Flow Short Scale

Items of the Flow Short Scale by Rheinberg, Vollmeyer, and Engeser (2003; cf. Engeser & Rheinberg, 2008). Items 1–10 measure the components of fl ow experi- ence. The items 11, 12, and 13 measure the perceived importance or perceived out- come importance (cf. Abuhamdeh, Chap. 6 of this volume). The fl ow items could be separated into two factors: (1) fl uency of performance (items 2, 4, 5, 7, 8, 9) and (2) absorption by activity (items 1, 3, 6, 10). With the additional items, demand, skills, and the perceived fi t of demands and skills are measured (cf. Keller and Landhäußer, Chap. 3 of this volume).

Not at all Partly Very much 1. I feel just the right amount of challenge 2. My thoughts/activities run fl uidly and smoothly 3. I do not notice time passing 4. I have no diffi culty concentrating 5. My mind is completely clear 6. I am totally absorbed in what I am doing 7. The right thoughts/movements occur of their own accord 8. I know what I have to do each step of the way 9. I feel that I have everything under control 10. I am completely lost in thought 11. Something important to me is at stake here 12. I must not make any mistakes here 13. I am worried about failing

S. Engeser (ed.), Advances in Flow Research, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1, 201 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012 202 Appendix: Flow Short Scale

• Compared to all other activities which I partake easy difficult in, this one is …

low high • I think that my competence in this area is …

too just too low right high • For me personally, the current demands are … References

Abuhamdeh, S. (2008). What makes an intrinsically motivated activity intrinsically motivating? The role of challenge. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Abuhamdeh, S. (2011). The enjoyment of suspense by actors and observers. Manuscript in preparation. Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Intrinsic and extrinsic orientations in the competitive context: An examination of person-situation interactions. Journal of Personality, 77 , 1615–1635. Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2011a). The importance of challenge for the enjoyment of intrinsically motivated, goal-directed activities. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin [Epub ahead of print]. Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2011b). Attentional involvement and intrinsic motivation. Motivation and [Epub ahead of print]. Ackerman, J. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). Two to tango: Automatic social coordination and the role of felt effort. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless : A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 335–371). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Adlai-Gail, W. S. (1994). Exploring the autotelic personality . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Aellig, S. (2004). Über den Sinn des Unsinns: Flow-Erleben und Wohlbefi nden als Anreize für autotelische Tätigkeiten : Eine Untersuchung mit der Experience Sampling Method (ESM) am Beispiel des Felsklettern [On the sense of nonsense. Flow experiences and well-being as incen- tives of autotelic activities] . Münster: Waxmann. Allport, G. W. (1937). The functional autonomy of motives. American Journal of Psychology, 50 , 141–156. Anderson, E. J., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2008). Human intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and competi- tion between exogenous and endogenous saccade plans. Neuroim age, 40 , 838–851. Andrew, M. J., & Jackson, S. A. (2008). Brief approaches to assessing task absorption and enhanced subjective experience: Examining ‘short’ and ‘core’ fl ow in diverse performance domains. Motivation and Emotion, 32 , 141–157. APA. (2011). American Psychological Association . http://www.apa.org/ . Asakawa, K. (2004). Flow experience and autotelic personality in Japanese college students: How do they experience challenges in daily life? Journal of Studies, 5 , 123–154. Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal of Happiness Studies, 11 , 205–223. Ashby, G. F., & Casale, M. B. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of implicit category learning. In L. Jimènez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 109–141). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

203 204 References

Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64 , 359–372. Atkinson, J. W., Heyns, R. W., & Veroff, J. (1958). The effect of experimental of the affi li- ation motive on thematic apperception. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study (pp. 95–104). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. Austin, J.H. (2010). The thalamic gateway: How the meditative training of attention evolves toward selfl ess transformations of consciousness. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new per- spective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 373–407). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120 , 338–375. Avnet, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fi t: Value transfer from ‘how’ to ‘what’. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39 , 525–530. Bagchi, B. K., & Wenger, M. A. (1958). Simultaneous EEG and other recordings during some yogic practices. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10 , 193. Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). Theta activity and meditative states: Spectral changes during concentrative meditation. Cognitive Process, 11 , 31–38. Bakker, A. (2005). Flow among music teachers and their students: The crossover of peak experi- ences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66 , 26–44. Bakker, A. B. (2008). The work-related fl ow inventory: Construction and initial validation of the WOLF. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72 , 400–414. Baldwin, C. A. (2001). Achievement goals and exam performance: An exploration of the role of study strategies and anticipatory test . Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities & Social Sciences, 62 , 2-A. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations . New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Bass, B. M. (1997). Personal selling and transactional/transformational leadership. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 17 , 19–28. Bassi, M., Steca, P., Delle Fave, A., & Caprara, G. V. (2007). Academic self-effi cacy beliefs and quality of experience in learning. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36 , 301–312. Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent dis- crepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 , 781–799. Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2007). sensitivity and affect regulation in dealing with positive and negative affect. Journal of Research in Personality, 41 , 239–248. Baumann, N., Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2010). Implicit motives: A look from Personality Systems Interaction theory. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 375– 403). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Intuition, affect, and personality: Unconscious coherence judg- ments and self-regulation of negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 , 1213–1223. Baumann, N., & Scheffer, D. (2010). Seeing and mastering diffi culty: The role of affective change in achievement fl ow. Cognition and Emotion, 24 , 1304–1328. Baumann, N., & Scheffer, D. (2011). Seeking fl ow in the achievement domain: The fl ow motive behind fl ow experience. Motivation and Emotion, 35 , 267–284. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 , 1252–1265. Beard, K. W., & Wolf, E. M. (2001). Modifi cation in the proposed diagnostic criteria for Internet addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4 , 377–383. Beard, K., & Hoy, W. K. (2010). The nature, meaning, and measure of teacher fl ow in elementary schools: A test of rival hypotheses. Educational Administration Quarterly, 463, 426–458. References 205

Beeman, M., Friedman, R. B., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., & Lindsay, M. B. (1994). Summation priming and coarse semantic coding in the right hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6 , 26–45. Beer, N., & Moneta, G. B. (2010). Construct and concurrent validity of the positive metacognitions and positive meta- questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 49 , 977–982. Beh, H. C. (1990). Achievement motivation, performance and cardiovascular activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 10 , 39–45. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42 , 155–162. Benedict, C., Kern, W., Schmid, S. M., Schultes, B., Born, J., & Hallschmid, M. (2009). Early morning rise in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity: a role for maintaining the brain’s energy balance. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34 , 455–462. Benjamin, L., & Flynn, F. (2006). Leadership style and regulatory mode: Value from fi t? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100 , 216–230. Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Confl ict, arousal, and . New York: McGraw-Hill. Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Autonomic origins, physiological mechanisms, and psychophysiological implications. Psychophysiology, 30 , 183–196. Blanck, P. D., Reis, H. T., & Jackson, L. (1984). The effects of verbal reinforcements on intrinsic motivation for sex-linked tasks. Sex Roles, 10 , 369–387. Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wölk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 , 665–679. Boerner, S., & von Streit, C. F. (2006). Teamgeist im Orchester: Das Zusammenspiel zwischen Flow und Führung. Musicae Scientiae, 10 , 245–263. Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity . New York: Plenum. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory . London: Routledge. Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104 , 11483–11488. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford. Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology . New York: Cambridge University Press. Brunstein, J. C., & Heckhausen, H. (2008). Achievement motivation. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 137–183). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement motiva- tion with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38 , 536–555. Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2010). Assessing individual differences in achievement motiva- tion with the Implicit Association Test: Predictive validity of a chronometric measure of the self-concept “ME = Successful”. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 151–185). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well- being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 , 494–508. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit of personal goals: A moti- vational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 169–196). New York: Sage. 206 References

Bruya, B. (2010a). Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bruya, B. (2010b). Introduction. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention. A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 1–28). Cambridge: MIT Press. Bryk, A., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods . Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bühler, K. (1922). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes [The mental development of the child] . Jena, Germany: Fischer. Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (2000). Handbook of psychophysiology . New York: Cambridge University Press. Callois, R. (1958). Les jeux et les hommes . Paris: Gallimard. Caplan, S. E. (2002). Problematic internet use and psychosocial well-being: Development of a theory-based cognitive-behavioral measurement instrument. Computers in Human Behavior, 18 , 553–575. Caputo, P. (1977). A rumor of war . London: Macmillan. Carli, M., Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (1988). The quality of experience in the fl ow channels: Comparison of Italian and U.S. students. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in consciousness (pp. 266–306). New York: Cambridge University Press. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1978). Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 , 324–332. Ceja, L., & Navarro, J. (2009). Dynamics of fl ow: A nonlinear perspective. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10 , 665–684. Ceja, L., & Navarro, J. (2011). Dynamic patterns of fl ow in the workplace: Characterizing within- individual variability using a complexity science approach. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32 , 627–651. Cermakova, L., Moneta, G. B., & Spada, M. M. (2010). Dispositional fl ow as mediator of the relationship between and approaches to studying during academics exami- nation preparation. Educational Psychology, 30 , 495–511. Chartrand, T. L., van Baaren, R. B., & Bargh, J. A. (2006). Linking automatic evaluation to mood and information processing style: Consequences for experienced affect, impression formation, and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 135 , 70–77. Chen, H. (2006). Flow on the net – Detecting Web users’ positive affect and their fl ow states. Computers in Human Behavior, 22 , 221–233. Chen, H., Wigand, R. T., & Nilan, M. (2000). Exploring web users optimal fl ow experiences. Information Technology and People, 13 , 263–281. Chou, T.-J., & Ting, C.-C. (2003). The role of fl ow experience in cyber- addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 6 , 663–675. Clarke, S. G., & Haworth, J. T. (1994). “Flow” experience in the daily lives of sixth-form college students. British Journal of Psychology, 85 , 511–523. Conti, R. (2001). Time fl ies: Investigating the connection between intrinsic motivation and the experience of time. Journal of Personality, 69 , 1–26. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Craig, J. A., Koestner, R., & Zuroff, D. C. (1994). Implicit and self-attributed intimacy motivation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11 , 491–507. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond and anxiety: Experiencing fl ow in work and . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the holistic approach to behavior. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness (pp. 335–356). New York: Plenum. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Toward a psychology of optimal experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 13–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. References 207

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1992). A response to the Kimiecik & Stein and Jackson papers. Journal of Applied , 4 , 181–183. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. A psychology for the 3rd millennium. New York: Harper Collins. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity. Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention . New York: Harper Perennial. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding fl ow. The psychology of engagement with everyday life . New York: Basic Books. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy? American Psychologist, 54 , 821–827. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety: Experiencing fl ow in work and play (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2004). Good business: Leadership, fl ow, and the making of meaning . New York, NY: Penguin Books. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Abuhamdeh, S., & Nakamura, J. (2005). Flow. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 598–608). New York: Guilford. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Bennett, S. (1971). An exploratory model of play. American Anthropologist, 73 , 45–58. Csikszentmihalyi, I., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988a). Introduction to part III. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in consciousness (pp. 183–192). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (1988b). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in consciousness . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1991). Einführung in Teil IV [Introduction to part IV]. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Hrsg.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag. Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebnisses [Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in consciousness] (S. 275–290). Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Getzels, J. W. (1973). The personality of young artist: An empirical and theoretical exploration. British Journal of Psychology, 64 , 91–104. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. W. (1984). Being adolescent . New York: Basic Books. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the Experience Sampling Method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175 , 529–536. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6 , 281–294. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56 , 815–822. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1993). The measurement of fl ow in everyday life: Toward a theory of emergent motivation. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1992: Developmental perspectives on motivation, current theory and research in motivation, 1992 (Vol. 40, pp. 57–97). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: A longitudinal study of their development . New York: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1996). Talented teenagers: The roots of suc- cess and failure . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schiefele, U. (1994). and the quality of experience in classrooms. European Journal of Psychology of , 10 , 251–270. Cury, F., Elliot, A., Sarrazin, P., Da Fonseca, D., & Rufo, M. (2002). The trichotomous achieve- ment goal model and intrinsic motivation: A sequential mediational analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38 , 473–481. Das, N. N., & Gastaut, H. (1955). Variations de l’activité electrique du cerveau, du coeur et des muscles squelettiques au cours de la Méditation et de l’extase yogique [Variations in the elec- trical activity of the brain, heart, and skeletal muscles during yogic meditation and ]. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 6 , 211–219. 208 References

Davis, C. A., Levitan, R. D., Reid, C., Carter, J. C., Kaplan, A. S., Patte, K. A., et al. (2009). Dopamine for ‘wanting’ and opioids for ‘liking’: A comparison of obese adults with and with- out binge eating. Obesity, 17 , 1220–1225. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (2007). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 159–189). New York: Cambridge University Press. De Kloet, E. R., Oitzl, M. S., & Joel, M. (1999). Stress and cognition: Are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends in Neurosciences, 22 , 422–426. De Manzano, Ö., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullen, F. (2010). The psychophysiology of fl ow dur- ing piano playing. Emotion, 10 , 301–311. Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1983). Components of gender stereotypes. Psychological Documents, 13 , 25. DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation . New York: Academic. Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18 , 105–115. Deci, E. L. (1972). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic reinforcement, and inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22 , 113–120. Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation . New York: Plenum. Deci, E., Betley, G., Kahle, J., Abrams, L., & Porac, J. (1981). When trying to win: Competition and intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7 , 79–83. Deci, E., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B., & Leone, D. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-deter- mination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62 , 119–142. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 39–80). New York: Academic. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour . New York: Plenum. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11 , 227–268. Delle Fave, A., & Bassi, M. (2000). The quality of experience in adolescents’ daily life: Developmental perspectives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126 , 347–367. Delle Fave, A., & Bassi, M. (2009). Sharing optimal experiences and promoting good community life in a multicultural society. The Journal of , 4 , 280–289. Delle Fave, A., Bassi, M., & Massimini, F. (2003). Quality of experience and risk perception in high-altitude rock climbing. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15 , 82–98. Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2004). The cross-cultural investigation of optimal experience. Ricerche di Psicologia, 1 , 79–102. Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2005). The investigation of optimal experience and : Developmental and psychosocial implications. European Psychologist, 10 , 264–274. DeManzano, Ö., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullén, F. (2010). The psychophysiology of fl ow dur- ing piano playing. Emotion, 10 , 301–311. Demerouti, E. (2006). Job characteristics, fl ow and performance: The moderating role of consci- entiousness. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11 , 266–280. Deppe, R. K., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Self-handicapping and intrinsic motvation: Buffering intrinsic motivation from the threat of failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 , 868–876. Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 , 1128–1139. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 , 1253–1265. References 209

Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Pavot, W. G., & Fujita, F. (1992). Extraversion and subjective well-being in a US national probability sample. Journal of Research in Personality, 26 , 205–215. Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 5 , 735–808. Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness. The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12 , 231–256. Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of fl ow. Consciousness and Cognition, 13 , 746–761. Dietrich, A., & Stoll, O. (2010). Effortless attention, hypofrontality, and perfectionism. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention (pp. 159–178). Cambridge: MIT Press. Donner, E. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1992). Transforming stress to fl ow. Executive Excellence, 9 , 16–17. Doya, K. (2000). Reinforcement learning in continuous time and space. Neural Computation, 12 , 243–269. Drengner, J., Gaus, H., & Jahn, S. (2008). Does fl ow infl uence the brand image in event marketing? Journal of Advertising Research, 48 , 138–147. Dubinsky, A. J., Yammarino, F. J., Jolson, M. A., & Spangler, W. D. (1995). Transformational leadership: An initial investigation in sales management. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 15 , 17–31. Edwards, J. R., & Cooper, C. L. (1988). The impacts of positive psychological states on physical health: a review and theoretical framework. Social Science & Medicine, 27 , 1447–1459. Eisenberger, R., Jones, J. R., Stinglhamber, F., Shanock, L., & Randall, A. T. (2005). Flow experi- ences at work: For high need achievers alone? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26 , 755–775. Eklund, R. C. (1996). Preparing to compete: A season-long investigation with collegiate wrestlers. The Sport Psychologist, 10 , 111–131. Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34 , 169–189. Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic moti- vation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 , 968–980. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 × 2 achievement goal frame work. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). The need for competence. In E. Deci & R. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 361–387). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2004). The intergenerational transmission of of failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30 , 957–971. Ellis, G. D., Voelkl, J. E., & Morris, C. (1994). Measurements and analysis issues with explanation of variance in daily experience using the Flow model. Journal of Leisure Research, 26 , 337–356. Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, moderators of challenge-skill-balance and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 32 , 158–172. Engeser, S. (2009). Lernmotivation und volitionale Handlungssteuerung: eine Längsschnittstudie zur Motivation und Volition und dem Lernverhalten beim Statistik Lernen [Learning motivation and volitional action regulation: a longitudinal study on motivation and volition in learning elementary statistics]. Saarbrücken: Südwestdeuter Verlag für Hochschulschriften. Engeser, S., Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Bischoff, J. (2005). Motivation, Flow-Erleben und Lernleistung in universitären Lernsettings [Motivation, fl ow experience and performance in learning settings at university]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 19 , 159–172. Epstein, J. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1992). Winning is not enough: The effects of competition and achievement orientation on intrinsic interest. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18 , 128–138. Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 210 References

Fairclough, S. H. (2009). Fundamentals of physiological computing. Interacting with Computers, 21 , 133–145. Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116 , 429–456. Finch, J. F., & West, S. G. (1997). The investigation of personality structure: Statistical models. Journal of Research in Personality, 31 , 439–485. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive develop- mental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34 , 906–911. Fletcher, C., & Baldry, C. (1999). Multi-source feedback systems: A research perspective. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 149–193). NewYork: Wiley. Freitas, A. L., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Enjoying goal-directed action: The role of regulatory fi t. Psychological Science, 13 , 1–6. Fridlund, A. J., & Cacippo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23 , 567–589. Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., & Kirschbaum, C. (2008). The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72 , 67–73. Fritz, B. S., & Avsec, A. (2007). The experience of fl ow and subjective well-being of music stu- dents. Horizons of Psychology, 17 , 5–17. Froh, R. C., Menges, R. J., & Walker, C. J. (1993). Revitalizing faculty work through intrinsic rewards. New Directions for Higher Education, 81 , 87–95. Fullagar, C. J., & Kelloway, E. K. (2009). ‘Flow’ at work: An experience sampling approach. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 82 , 595–615. Fullagar, C. J., & Mills, M. (2008). Motivation and fl ow: Toward an understanding of the dynamics of the relation in architecture students. The Journal of Psychology, 142 , 533–553. Furlong, W. (1976). The fun in fun. Psychology Today, 10 (1), 35–38. Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., Brewer, L. E., & Scheichel, B. J. (2007). Self control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92 , 325–336. Gangster, D. C., & Schaubroeck, J. (1991). Work stress and employee health. Journal of Management, 17 , 235–271. Getzel, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1966). The study of creativity in future artists: the criterion problem. In O. J. Harvey (Ed.), Experience, structure, and adaptability . New York: Springer. Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactiva- tion during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50 , 329–339. Goldstein, H. (1995). Multilevel statistical models . New York: Halsted. Goleman, D. J. (1995). . New York: Bantam. Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19 , 1211–1226. Graef, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & McManama Gianinno, S. (1983). Measuring intrinsic motiva- tion in everyday life. Leisure Studies, 2 , 155–168. Graham, J. M. (2008). Self-expansion and fl ow in couples’ momentary experiences: An experience sampling study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 , 679–694. Grant, J. E., Potenza, M. N., Weinstein, A., & Gorelick, D. A. (2010). Introduction to behavioral addictions. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36 , 233–242. Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102 , 4–27. Grier, R. A., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Metthews, G., Galinsky, T. L., Szalma, J. L., et al. (2003). The vigilance decrement refl ects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Human Factors, 45 , 349–359. Groos, K. (1899). Die Spiele des Menschen [Human ] . Jena: Fischer. References 211

Gupta, B. S., & Nagpal, M. (1978). Impulsivity/sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 69 , 204–206. Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98 , 4259–4264. Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Cognitive representation of personality impressions: Organizational processes in fi rst impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 , 1050–1063. Hamilton, J. A., Haier, R. J., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–93. Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2003): Vagal infl uence on working memory and attention. In: International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48, S. 263–274. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Predicting success in col- lege: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94 , 562–575. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 , 904–915. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). The joint effects of target and purpose goals on intrin- sic motivation: A mediational analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 , 675–689. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from here. In M. L. Maehr & E. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 21–49). Greenwich: JAI. Harari, Y. N. (2008). Combat fl ow: military, political, and ethical dimensions of subjective well- being in war. Review of General Psychology, 12 , 253–264. Harter, S. (1978). derived from challenge and the effects of receiving grades on chidren’s diffi culty level choices. Child Development, 49 , 788–799. Hatfi eld, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). . New York: Cambridge University Press. Hausenblaus, H. A., & Downs, D. S. (2002). Exercise dependence: A systematic review. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3 , 89–123. Haworth, J., & Evans, S. (1995). Challenge, skill and positive subjective states in the daily life of a sample of YTS students. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 68 , 109–121. Haworth, J. T., & Hill, S. (1992). Work, leisure and psychological well-being in a sample of young adults. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 2 , 147–160. Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the CNS. Psychogical Review, 62 , 243–254. Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1 , 283–329. Heckhausen, H. (1989). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action] . Berlin: Springer. Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action . New York: Springer. Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.). (2008). Motivation and action . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Heckhausen, H., & Rheinberg, F. (1980). Lernmotivation im Unterricht, erneut betrachtet [Learning motivation in the classroom reconsidered]. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 8 , 7–47. Heim, C., Ehlert, U., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2000). The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 1–35. Hektner, J. (1997). Exploring optimal personality development: A longitudinal study of adoles- cents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Hektner, J. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). A longitudinal exploration of fl ow an intrinsic motivation in adolescents. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American education research association. New York: Alfred Sloan Foundation. Hektner, J., Schmidt, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling method. Measuring the quality of everyday life . Thousand Oaks: Sage. 212 References

Heo, J., Lee, Y., Pedersen, P. M., & McCormick, B. P. (2010). Flow experience in the daily lives of older adults: An analysis of the interaction between fl ow, individual differences, serious lei- sure, location, and social context. Canadian Journal on Aging, 29 , 411–423. Herzog, H., Lele, V. R., Kuwert, T., Langen, K.-J., Kops, E. R., & Feinendegen, L. E. (1990). Changed pattern of regional glucose metabolism during meditative . Neuropsychobiology, 23 , 182–187. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and . American Psychologist, 52 , 1280–1300. Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30 , 1–46. Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fi t. American Psychologist, 55 , 1217–1230. Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological Review, 113 , 439–460. Higgins, E. T., Cesario, J., Hagiwara, N., Spiegel, S., & Pittman, T. (2010). Increasing or decreas- ing interest in activities: The role of regulatory fi t. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 , 559–572. Higgins, E. T., & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for self- regulation: A motivated cognition perspective. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 171–187). New York, NY: Guilford. Hugdahl, K. (1995). Psychophysiology: The mind-body perspective . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10 , 722–126. Jackson, D. N. (1984a). Personality research form (3rd ed.). Port Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems. Jackson, D. N. (1984b). Personality research form manual . Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists. Jackson, S. A. (1995). Factors infl uencing the occurrence of fl ow state in elite athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7 , 138–166. Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports: The keys to optimal experiences and performances . Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing fl ow in physical activity: The fl ow state scale-2 and dispositional fl ow scale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24 , 133–150. Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2004a). Relationships between quality of experience and partici- pation in diverse performance settings. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56 (Suppl), 193. Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2004b). The fl ow scale manual. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Jackson, S. A., Kimiecik, J. C., Ford, S. K., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychological correlates of fl ow in sport. Journal of Sport & Experience Psychology, 20 , 358–378. Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The fl ow state scale. Journal of Sport & Experience Psychology, 18 , 17–35. Jackson, S. A., Martin, A. J., & Eklund, R. C. (2008). Long and short measures of fl ow: The con- struct validity of the FSS-2, DFS-2, and new brief counterparts. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30 , 561–587. Jackson, S. A., & Roberts, G. C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward a concep- tual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6 , 156–171. Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J. (2001). Relationship between fl ow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13 , 129–153. Jacobs, D. F. (1989). A general theory of addictions: Rational for and evidence supporting a new approach for understanding and treating addictive behaviours. In H. J. Shaffer, S. A. Stein, References 213

B. Gambino, & T. N. Cummings (Eds.), Compulsive gambling: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 35–64). Lexington, UK: Lexington Books. Jimènez, L. (2002). Attention and implicit learning . Amsterdam: Benjamins. Job, V., Oertig, D., Brandstätter, V., & Allemand, M. (2010). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit motivation and unhealthy eating behavior. Journal of Personality, 78 , 1209–1238. Jones, C. D., Hollenhorst, S. J., & Perna, F. (2003). An empirical comparison of the four quadrant fl ow model and adventure experience paradigm. Leisure Sciences, 25 , 17–31. Joseph, R. (1996). Neuropsychology, neuropsychiatry, and behavioral neurology . New York: Plenum. Jostmann, N. B., & Koole, S. L. (2006). On the waxing and waning of working memory: Action orientation moderates the impact of demanding relationships primes on working memory capacity. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 32 , 1716–1728. Jostmann, N. B., & Koole, S. L. (2007). On the regulation of cognitive control: Action orientation moderates the impact of high demands in Stroop interference tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 136 , 593–609. Jostmann, N. B., Koole, S. L., van der Wulp, N. Y., & Fockenberg, D. A. (2005). Sublimal affect regulation: The moderating role of action versus state orientation. European Psychologist, 10 , 209–217. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort . Englewood Ciffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306 , 1776–1780. Katkin, E. S. (1975). Electrodermal lability: A psychophysiological analysis of individual differ- ences in response to stress. In I. G. Sarason & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 141–176). Washington, DC: Aldine. Kawabata, M., Mallett, C. J., & Jackson, S. A. (2008). The fl ow state scale-2 and dispositional fl ow scale-2: Examination of factorial validity and reliability for Japanese adults. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9 , 465–485. Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achievement motivation: Volitional facilita- tion and inhibition as a function of affective contents of need-related stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 , 426–448. Kehr, H. M. (2004a). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among man- agers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30 , 315–327. Kehr, H. M. (2004b). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives and perceived abilities: The compensatory model of and volition. Academy of Management Review, 29 , 479–499. Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2006). Regulatory fi t and cognitive performance: The interactive effect of chronic and situationally induced self-regulatory mechanisms on test performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36 , 393–405. Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2008). Flow and regulatory compatibility: An experimental approach to the fl ow model of intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 , 196–209. Keller, J., & Blomann, F. (2008). Locus of control and the fl ow experience: An experimental analy- sis. European Journal of Personality, 22 , 589–607. Keller, J., & Landhäußer, A. (2011). Im Flow sein: Experimentelle Analysen des Zustands opti- maler Beanspruchung [Experiencing fl ow: Experimental analyses of the experiential state resulting from optimal task demands]. Psychologische Rundschau, 62 , 213–220. Keller, J., Bless, H., & Blomann, F. (2011a). Flow and the tunnel vision: Cognitive consequences of a skills-demands-compatibility . Manuscript in preparation. Keller, J., Bless, H., Blomann, F., & Kleinbohl, D. (2011b). Physiological aspects of fl ow experi- ences: Skills-demand-compatibility effects on heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47 , 849–852. Keller, J., Landhäußer, A., & Fischer, J. (2011c). Effects of a skills-demands-compatibility: Flow experiences and physiological reactions in an aversive activity. Manuscript in preparation. 214 References

Keller, J., Ringelhan, S., & Blomann, F. (2011d). Does skills-demands-compatibility result in intrinsic motivation? Experimental test of a basic notion proposed in the theory of fl ow-experi- ences. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 408–417. Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. Handbook of Social Psychology, 1 , 233–265. Kilpatrick, D. G. (1972). Differential responsiveness of two electrodermal indices to psychological stress and performance of a complex cognitive task. Psychophysiology, 9, 218–226. Kim, H. K., & Davis, K. E. (2009). Toward a comprehensive theory of problematic Internet use: Evaluating the role of self-esteem, anxiety, fl ow, and the self-rated importance of Internet aci- tivities. Computers in Human Behavior, 25 , 490–500. Kivikangas, J. M. (2006). Psychophysiology of fl ow experience: An explorative study . Master’s thesis, University of Helsinki. Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cunningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., et al. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 393 , 266–268. Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Koestner, J. (1987). Praise, involvement, and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 , 383–390. Koole, S. L., & Coenen, L. H. M. (2007). Implicit self and affect regulation: Effects of action orienta- tion and subliminal self priming in an affective priming task. Self and Identity, 6 , 118–136. Koole, S. L., & Jostmann, N. B. (2004). Getting a grip on your : Effects of action orienta- tion and external demands on intuitive affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87 , 974–990. Koole, S. L., Jostmann, N. B., & Kuhl, J. (2012). Individual differences in decisiveness: An inte- grative review. Manuscript submitted for publication. Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387–406. Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest, learning, and development. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kruglanski, A. W. (1975). The endogenous-exogenous partition in attribution theory. Psychological Review, 82 , 387–406. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases . New York: Plenum. Kruglanski, A. W., Riter, A., Amitai, A., Margolin, B.-S., Shabtai, L., & Zaksh, D. (1975a). Can money enhance intrinsic motivation? A test of the content-consequence hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31 , 744–750. Kruglanski, A. W., Riter, A., Arazi, D., Agasi, R., Montesquio, J., Peri, I., & Peretz, M. (1975b). Effects of task-intrinsic rewards upon extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31 , 699–705. Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash, M. N., Pierro, A., Shah, J. Y., & Spiegel, S. (2000). To ‘do the right thing’ or to ‘just do it’: Locomotion and assessment as distinct self- regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 , 793–815. Kuhl, J. (1978). Standard setting and risk preference: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation and an empirical test. Psychological Review, 85 , 239–248. Kuhl, J. (1994a). A theory of action and state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 9–46). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Kuhl, J. (1994b). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the action-control- scale (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 47–59). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and volition: The dynamics of person- ality systems interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Self-regulation: Directions and challenges for future research (pp. 111–169). New York: Academic. Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Interaktionen psychischer Systeme [Motivation and personality: Interactions of mental systems] . Göttingen: Hogrefe. Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994). Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation . Göttingen: Hogrefe. References 215

Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1997). Das Persönlichkeits-Stil-und-Störungs-Inventar (PSSI): Manual [The Personality-Styles-and-Disorder-Inventory (PSSI): Manual] . Göttingen: Hogrefe. Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of diffi cult intentions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128 , 382–399. Kuhl, J., & Koole, S. L. (2004). Workings of the will: A functional approach. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 411–430). New York: Guilford. Kuhl, J., & Koole, S. L. (2008). The functional architecture of approach and avoidance motivation. In A. J. Elliott (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 535–555). New York: Psychology. Kuhl, J., & Koole, S. L. (2012). The functional architecture of personality . Manuscript submitted for publication. Kuhl, J. & Scheffer, D. (1999). Auswertungsmanual für den Operativen Motiv Test (OMT) [Scoring system for the Operant Motive Test]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Osnabrück. Kuhl, J., Scheffer, D., & Eichstaedt, J. (2003). Der Operante Motiv-Test (OMT): Ein neuer Ansatz zur Messung impliziter Motive [The operant motive-test (OMT): A new approach to the assess- ment of implicit motives]. In F. Rheinberg & J. Stiensmeier-Pelster (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 129–149). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Lacey, J. I., Kagan, J., Lacey, B. C., & Moss, H. A. (1963). The visceral level: Situational determi- nants and behavioral correlates of autonomic response patterns. In P. H. Knapp (Ed.), Expression of the emotions in man (pp. 161–196). New York: International Universities Press. Landhäußer, A., Keller, J., & Frommherz, A. (2011). The paradox of work reconsidered: A system- atic test based on the Day Reconstruction Method . Manuscript in preparation. Langner, C. A., & Winter, D. G. (2001). The motivational basis of concessions and compromise: Archival and laboratory studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 711–727. Larsen, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Poehlmann, K. M., Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). The psy- chophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), The handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 180–195). New York: Guilford. Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. In H. T. Reis (Ed.), Naturalistic approaches to studying social interaction (pp. 41–56). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Larson, R., & Delespaul, P. A. E. G. (1992). Analysing experience sampling data: A guidebook for the perplexed. In M. de Vries (Ed.), The experience of psychopathology: Investigating mental disorders in their natural settings (pp. 58–78). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Laurenceau, J.-P., Troy, A. B., & Carver, C. S. (2005). Two distinct emotional experiences in romantic relationships: Effects of perceptions regarding approach of intimacy and avoidance of confl ict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 , 1234–1133. Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks. Annual Review of Psychology, 44 , 1–21. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping . New York: Springer. Lazarus, R. S., Kanner, A. D. & Folkman, S. (1980). Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. In: R. Plutchnik & H. Kellerman (Eds.). Emotion. Theory, research, and experience, Vol. 1, Theories of emotion (pp. 189–217). Academic: New York. Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. NeuroReport, 11, 1581–1585. Lee, E. (2005). The relationship of motivation and fl ow experience to academic procrastination in university students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166 , 5–14. Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 413–499). San Diego, CA: Academic. Lehéricy, S., Bardinet, E., Tremblay, L., Van de Moortele, P. F., Pochon, J. B., Dormont, D., Kim, D. S., Yelnik, J., & Ugurbil, K. (2006). Motor control in basal ganglia circuits using fMRI and brain atlas approaches. Cerebral Cortex, 16 , 149–161. 216 References

Lehrer, P. (2003). Applied psychophysiology: Beyond the boundaries of biofeedback. (Mending a wall, a brief history of our fi eld, and applications to control of the muscles and cardiorespira- tory systems). Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 28 , 291–304. Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and chance. In H. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct (Vol. 1, pp. 15–63). New York: Academic. Linsner, S. H. (2009). Transformational leadership and ‘fl ow’: The mediating effects of psychological climate. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering , 70(5-B) Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 , 663–676. Liu, S.-H., Liao, H.-L., & Pratt, J. A. (2009). Impact of media richness and fl ow on e-learning technology . Computers & Education, 52 , 599–607. Lovallo, W. R., & Thomas, T. L. (2000). Stress hormones in psychophysiological research. Emotional, behavioral, and cognitive implications. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lundberg, U. (2005). Stress hormones in health and illness: The roles of work and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30 , 1017–1021. Lynch, J. C. (1980). The functional organization of posterior parietal association cortex. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 3 , 485–499. MacDonald, R., Byrne, C., & Carlton, L. (2006). Creativity and fl ow in musical composition: An empirical investigation. Psychology of Music, 34 , 292–306. Magee, J. C., & Langner, C. A. (2008). How personalized and socialized power motivation facilitate antisocial and prosocial decision-making. Journal of Research in Personality, 42 , 1547–1559. Mannell, R. C., Zuzanek, J., & Larson, R. (1988). Leisure states and “fl ow” experience: Testing perceived freedom and intrinsic motivation hypotheses. Journal of Leisure Research, 20 , 289–304. Marr, A. J. (2001). In the zone: A biobehavioral theory of the fl ow experience. Athletic insight. The Online Journal of Sport Psychology. Elektronische Ressource: http://www.athleticinsight.com/ Vol3Iss1/Commentary.htm . Maslow, A. (1964). Religions, values and peak-experiences. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University Press. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being . New York: Van Nostrand. Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1988). The systematic assessment of fl ow in daily experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in consciousness (pp. 266–287). New York: Cambridge University Press. Massimini, F., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Carli, M. (1987). The monitoring of optimal experience: A tool for psychiatric rehabilitation. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 175 , 545–549. Massimini, F., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Delle Fave, A. (1988). Flow and biocultural evolution. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological stud- ies of fl ow in consciousness (pp. 60–81). New York: Cambridge University Press. Mathews, A., Yiend, J., & Lawrence, A. D. (2004). Individual differences in the modulation of fear-related brain activation by attentional control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16 , 1683–1694. McAdams, D. P. (1984). Human motives and personal relationships. In V. Derlega (Ed.), Communication, intimacy and close relationships (pp. 41–67). New York: Academic. McAdams, D. P., Healy, S., & Krause, S. (1984a). Social motives and patterns of friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47 , 828–838. McAdams, D. P., Jackson, R. J., & Kirshnit, C. A. (1984b). Looking, laughing and smiling in dyads as a function of intimacy motivation and reciprocity. Journal of Personality, 52 , 261–273. References 217

McAdams, D. P., Lester, R. M., Brand, P. A., McNamara, W. J., & Lensky, D. B. (1988). Sex and the TAT: Are women more intimate than men? Do men fear intimacy? Journal of Personality Assessment, 52 , 397–409. McClelland, D. C. (1970). The two faces of power. Journal of International Affairs, 24 , 29–47. McClelland, D. C. (1975). Power. The inner experience . New York, NY: Irvington. McClelland, D. (1980). Motive dispositions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41). Beverly Hills: Sage. McClelland, D. C. (1985a). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American Psychologist, 40 , 812–825. McClelland, D. C. (1985b). Human motivation . Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. McClelland, D. C. (1987). Biological aspects of human motivation. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 11–19). Berlin: Springer. McClelland, D. C. (1995). Achievement motivation in relation to achievement-related recall, per- formance, and urine fl ow, a marker associated with release of vasopressin. Motivation and Emotion, 19 , 59–76. McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive . New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). Leadership motive pattern and long-term success in management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67 , 737–743. McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical diffi culties of detecting interactions and mod- erator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114 , 376–390. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96 , 690–702. McClelland, D. C., & Watson, R. I. (1973). Power motivation and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Personality, 41 , 121–139. McClelland, D. C., & Wilsnack, S. C. (1972). The effects of drinking on thoughts about power and restraint. In D. C. McClelland, W. N. Davis, R. Kalin, & E. Wanner (Eds.), The drinking man (pp. 123–141). New York: Free. McEwen, B. S. & Seeman, T. (1999). Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. In N. E. Adler, M. Marmot, B. S. McEwen & J. Stewart (Eds. ), Socioeconomic Status and Health in Industrial Nations: Social, Psychological and Biological Pathways (896 p). New York: Annual New York Academy of Sciences. McGregor, H. A., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Achievement goals as predictors of achievement-relevant processes prior to task engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94 , 381–395. McInman, A. D., & Grove, J. R. (1991). Peak moments in sport: A literature review. Quest, 43 , 333–351. Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and emo- tional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In J. A. Simpson & W. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York: Guilford. Moller, A. C., Meier, B. P., & Wall, R. D. (2010). Developing an experimental induction of fl ow: Effortless action in the lab. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 191–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Moneta, G. B. (in press). Opportunity for creativity in the job as a moderator of the relationship between trait intrinsic motivation and fl ow in work. Motivation and Emotion. Moneta, G. B. (1990). A modeling approach to the study of subjective experience: Everyday life variations as a function of perceived challenges and skills in the activity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Moneta, G. B. (2004a). The fl ow experience across cultures. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5 , 115–121. Moneta, G. B. (2004b). The fl ow model of state intrinsic motivation in Chinese: Cultural and personal moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2 , 181–217. 218 References

Moneta, G. B. (2010). Flow in work as a function of trait intrinsic motivation, opportunity for creativity in the job, and work engagement. In S. Albrecht (Ed.), The Handbook of employee engagement: Perspectives, issues, research and practice (pp. 262–269). Cheltenham: Edward- Elgar Publishing House. Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64 , 275–310. Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Models of concentration in natural environments: A comparative approach based on streams of experiential data. Social Behavior and Personality, 27 , 603–637. Mugford, A. L. (2006). Flow in a team sport setting: Does cohesion matter? Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 66 , 4536. Mulder, G., Mulder, L. J. M., & Veldman, J. B. P. (1985). Mental tasks as stressors. In A. Steptoe, H. Riiddel, & H. Neus (Eds.), Clinical and methodological issues in cardiovascular psy- chophysiology (pp. 30–44). Berlin: Springer. Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic apperceptive test manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Nacke, L. & Lindley, C. (2009). Affective ludology, fl ow and immersion in a fi rst-person shooter: Measurement of player experience. Loading…, 3. Online resource available under http://jour- nals.sfu.ca/loading/index.php/loading/article/view/72 . Nakamura, Y. (1988). Optimal experience and the uses of talent. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in conscious- ness (pp. 319–326). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of fl ow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2005). The concept of fl ow. In C. R. Snyder & S. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York: Oxford University press. Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow theory and research. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 195–206). New York: Oxford University Press. Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., & d’Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood fl ow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging Section, 106 , 113–122. Nichols, S., & Newman, J. P. (1986). Effects of punishment on response latency in extraverts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 , 624–630. Nielsen, K., & Cleal, B. (2010). Predicting fl ow at work: Investigating the activities and job char- acteristics that predict fl ow states at work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15 , 180–190. Oettingen, G., Pak, H., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal-setting: Turning free fanta- sies about the future into binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 , 736–753. Ohse, A. (1997). Vom Streß zum Flow. Das Flowerlebens und Streßbewältungungstraining (FEST) [From stress to fl ow. Training of fl ow-experience and coping]. Marburg: Tectum. Oitzl, M. S., Champagne, D. L., van der Veen, R., & de Kloet, E. R. (2010). Brain development under stress: Hypotheses of glucocorticoid actions revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34 , 853–866. Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical of the septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47 , 419–427. Oud, J. H. L., & Delsing, M. J. M. H. (2010). Continuous time modeling of panel data by means of SEM. In K. van Montfort, J. H. L. Oud, & A. Satorra (Eds.), Longitudinal research with latent variables . New York: Springer. References 219

Pang, J. S. (2010). Content coding methods in implicit motive assessment: Standards of measure- ment and best practice for the picture story exercise. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 119–150). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85 , 280–294. Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1987). Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant’s integrative competence. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 669–720). New York: Wiley. Partington, S., Partington, E., & Olivier, S. (2009). The dark side of fl ow: A qualitative study of dependence in big wave surfi ng. The Sport Psychologist, 23 , 170–185. Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment, 5 , 164–172. Peifer, C., Schachinger, H., Baumann, N., Schulz, A. & Antoni, C. H. (2010, February). The rela- tion of the stress-hormone cortisol and the fl ow-phenomenon. Paper presented at the sympo- sium ‘The concept of Flow: Toward a Scientifi c Integration’, Braunschweig. Peifer, C. Schachinger, H. & Antoni, C. H. (2011, July). Cortisol and fl ow-experience – an experi- mental approach. Paper presented at the 2nd World Congress on Positive Psychology, Philadelphia. Perceived Competence Scales (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.psych.rochester.edu/SDT/mea- sures/PCS_description.php . Percival, G., Crous, F., & Schepers, J. M. (2003). Cognitive potential and job complexity as predic- tors of fl ow. Journal of Industrial Psychology, 29 , 60–71. Perry, S. K. (1999). Writing in fl ow . Cincinnati: Writer’s Digest Books. P fi ster, R. (2002). Flow im Alltag: Untersuchungen zum Quadrantenmodell des Flow-Erlebens und zum Konzept der autotelischen Perso ¨nlichkeit mit der Experience Sampling Method (ESM) [Flow in everyday life: Studies on the quadrant model of fl ow experiencing and on the concept of the autotelic personality with the experience sampling method (ESM)] . Bern: Peter Lang. Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood . New York: Norton. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review in behavioral research: A critical review of the litera- ture and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 , 879–903. Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19 , 225–233. Porges, S. W., & Byrne, E. A. (1992). Research methods for measurement of heart rate and respira- tion. Biological Psychology, 34 , 93–130. Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Rueda, M. R., & Tang, Y. (2010). Training effortless attention. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention. A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 409–424). Cambridge: MIT Press. Prinzel, L. J., Freeman, F. G., Scerbo, M. W., Mikulka, P. J., & Pope, A. T. (2000). A closed-loop system for examining psychophysiological measures for adaptive task allocation. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 10 , 393–410. Privette, G. (1983). Peak experience, peak performance, and fl ow: A comparative analysis of posi- tive human experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6 , 1361–1368. Putman, P., & Roelofs, K. (2011). Effects of single cortisol administrations on human affect reviewed: Coping with stress through adaptive regulation of automatic cognitive processing. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36 , 439–448. Quinn, R. W. (2005). High performance experience in the design of national security. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50 , 610–641. Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98 , 676–682. 220 References

Rani, P., Sarkar, N., Liu, C. (2005). Maintaining optimal challenge in computer games through real-time physiological feedback. Paper presented at the 11th Human–Computer Interaction International, Las Vegas, NV, USA. Rathunde, K. (2010). Experiential wisdom and optimal experience: Interviews with three distin- guished lifelong learners. Journal of Adult Development, 17, 81–93. Redondo, M., & Del Valle-Inclán, F. (1992). Decrements in heart rate variability during memory search. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 13 , 29–35. Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic motiva- tion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22 , 24–33. Reeve, J., Olson, B. C., & Cole, S. G. (1985). Motivation and performance: Two consequences of winning and losing in competition. Motivation and Emotion, 9 , 291–298. Reeve, J., Olson, B. C., & Cole, S. G. (1987). Intrinsic motivation in competition: The intervening role of four individual differences following objective competence information. Journal of Research in Personality, 21 , 148–170. Reinhardt, C., Lau, A., Hottenrott, K., & Stoll, O. (2006). Flow-Erleben unter kontrollierter Beanspruchungssteuerung: Ergebnisse einer Laufbandstudie [Flow experiences under con- trolled running work load: Results of a laboratory treadmill study]. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 13 , 140–146. Rheinberg, F. (1982). Zweck und Tätigkeit [Goal and activity] . Habilitationsschrift, Fakultät für Psychologie. Ruhr-Universität Bochum. Rheinberg, F. (1986). The motivational analysis of high-risk sport. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention and volition (pp. 249–259). Berlin: Springer. Rheinberg, F. (1989). Zweck und Tätigkeit. Motivationspsychologische Analysen zur Handlungsveranlassung. [Goal and activity. Motivational psychology analyses of action initiation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Rheinberg, F. (1991). Flow-experience when motorcycling: A study of a special human condition. In R. Brendicke (Ed.), Safety, environment, future (pp. 349–362). Bochum: Institut für Zweiradsicherheit (ifZ). Rheinberg, F. (1993). Anreize engagiert betriebener Freizeitaktivitäten. Ein Systematisi- erungsversuch [Incentives of dedicated leisure activities: A suggestion for systematization]. Unveröffentlichtes Manuskript, Psychologisches Institut der Universität Potsdam. Rheinberg, F. (2002). Freude am Kompetenzerwerb, Flow-Erleben und motivpassende Ziele [Enjoyment of learning, fl ow experience, and motive-congruent goals]. In M. von Salisch (Ed.), Emotionale Kompetenz entwickeln. Grundlagen in Kindheit und Jugend (pp. 179–206). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. Rheinberg, F. (2006). Motivation . Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and fl ow-experience. In H. Heckhausen & J. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2010). Motive training and motivational competence. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 510–548). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Rheinberg, F., Iser, I., & Pfauser, S. (1997). Freude am Tun und/oder zweckorientiertes Schaffen? Zur transsituativen Konsistenz und konvergenten Validität der AF-Skala [Doing something for fun and/or for gain? Transsituational consistency and convergent validity of the Incentive Focus Scale]. Diagnostica, 2, 174–191. Rheinberg, F., & Manig, Y. (2003). Was macht Spaß am Graffi ti-Sprayen? Eine induktive Anreizanalyse [What motivates to draw graffi ti? An inductive analysis of incentives]. Report Psychologie, 4 , 222–234. Rheinberg, F., Manig, Y., Kliegl, R., Engeser, S., & Vollmeyer, R. (2007). Flow bei der Arbeit, doch Glück in der Freizeit. Zielausrichtung, Flow und Glücksgefühle [Flow during work but happiness during leisure time: Goals, fl ow-experience, and happiness]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 51 , 105–115. References 221

Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2003). Flow-Erleben in einem Computerspiel unter experimentell variierten Bedingungen [Flow experience in a computer game under experimentally controlled conditions]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 211 , 161–170. Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Engeser, S. (2003). Die Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens [The assess- ment of fl ow experience]. In J. Stiensmeier-Pelster & F. Rheinberg (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 261–279). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Rogatko, T. P. (2009). The infl uence of fl ow on positive affect in college students. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10 , 133–148. Rose, R. M., Jenkins, C. D., Hurst, M., Herds, J. A., & Hall, R. P. (1982). Endocrine activity in air traffi c controllers at work. II. Biological, psychological and work correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology , 7 , 113–123. Rotenberg, V. S. (1993). Richness against freedom: Two hemisphere functions and the problem of creativity. European Journal for High Ability, 4 , 11–19. Rothermund, K., & Meiniger, C. (2004). Stress-buffering effects of self-complexity: Reduced affective spillover or self-regulatory processes? Self and Identity, 3 , 263–282. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80 , 1–28. Rueda, M., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005a). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 573–594. Rueda, M., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005b). Training, maturation and genetic infl uences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102 , 14931–14936. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & the PDP Research Group (Eds.). (1986). Parallel distrib- uted processing (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Russel, W. D. (2001). An examination of fl ow state occurrence in college athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24 , 83–107. Russel, J. A., & Caroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125 , 3–30. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic moti- vation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55 , 68–78. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74 , 1557–1585. Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Ego-involved persistence: When free-choice behavior is not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion, 15 , 185–205. Ryan, R., Williams, G., Patrick, H., & Deci, E. (2009). Self-determination theory and physical activity: The dynamics of motivation in development and wellness. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 6 , 107–124. Salanova, M., Bakker, A. B., & Llorens, S. (2006). Flow at work: Evidence for an upward spiral of personal and organizational resources. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7 , 1–22. Sales, S. M. (1969). Organizational role as a risk factor in coronary disease. Administrative Science Quarterly, 14 , 325–336. Sansone, C. (1986). A question of competence: The effects of competence and task feedback on intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51 , 918–931. Sansone, C. (1989). Competence feedback, task feedback, and intrinsic interest: An examination of process and context. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25 , 343–361. Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between having to and wanting to. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 343–374). New York: Academic. Sapolski, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids infl uence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21 , 55–89. Sato, I. (1988). Bosozoku: Flow in Japanese motorcycle gangs. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of fl ow in conscious- ness (pp. 92–117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 222 References

Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Group creativity: Music, theater, collaboration . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schacter, D. L., & Bruckner, R. L. (1998). On the relationship among priming, conscious recollec- tion, and intentional retrieval: Evidence from neuroimaging research. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 70 , 284–303. Schallberger, U., & Pfi ster, R. (2001). Flow-Erleben in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Untersuchung zum ‘Paradox der Arbeit’ mit der Experience Sampling Method (ESM) [Flow experiences in work and leisure: An experience sampling study about the paradox of work]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 45 , 176–187. Schattke, K. P. (2011). Flow experience as consequence and self-determination as antecedents of congruence between implicit and explicit motives (Doctoral Dissertation). Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany. Retrieved from http://mediatum.ub.tum.de/ doc/1078244/1078244.pdf . Schiefele, U., & Roussakis, E. (2006). Die Bedingungen des Flow-Erlebens in einer experimentellen Spielsituation [Experimental conditions for experiencing fl ow in computer games]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie mit Zeitschrift für angewandte Psychologie und Sprache & Kognition, 214, 207–219. Schiepe, A. (2011, July). The fl ow of power: The effect of the power motive and implicit self control on fl ow experience. Paper presented at the 16th General meeting of the European Association for Social Psychology, Stockholm. Schmid, J. (2007). Flow-Erleben und Achtsamkeit. Neue Paradigmen der psychomotorischen Gesundheitsförderung [Flow experience and . New paradigms of psychomotoric health promotion]. Motorik, 3 , 30–34. Schüler, J. (2007). Arousal of fl ow experience in a learning setting and its effects on exam perfor- mance and affect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 21 , 217–227. Schüler, J. (2010). Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongru- ence on fl ow experience. Motivation and Emotion, 34 , 2–14. Schüler, J. (2012). Self-effi cacy mediates the relationship between the fl ow experience of climbers and their risky behavior . Manuscript in preparation. Schüler, J., & Brunner, S. (2009). The rewarding effect of fl ow experience on performance in a marathon race. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 10 , 168–174. Schüler, J., & Engeser, S. (2009). Incentives and fl ow-experience in learning settings and the moderating role of individual differences. In M. Wosnitza, S. A. Karabenick, A. Efklides & P. Nenninger (Eds.), Contemporary Motivation Research: From Global to Local Perspectives (339–358). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2008). A high implicit affi liation motive does not always make you happy: A corresponding explicit motive and corresponding behavior are further needed. Motivation and Emotion, 32 , 231–242. Schüler, J., & Langens, T. A. (2007). Psychological crisis in the marathon race and the buffering effects of self-verbalization. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37 , 2319–2344. Schüler, J., & Pfenninger, M. (2011). Flow impairs risk perception in kayakers. In B. D. Geranto (Ed.), Sport psychology (pp. 237–246). New York: Nova. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67 , 1–38. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessing implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profi les, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77 , 71–86. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (Eds.). (2010). Implicit motives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Schultheiss, O. C., Campbell, K. L., & McClelland, D. C. (1999). Implicit power motivation mod- erates men’s testosterone responses to imagined and real dominance success. Hormones and Behavior, 36 , 234–241. Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikov, B., & Schad, D. J. (2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test using the Picture Story Exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91 , 72–81. References 223

Schwabe, L., Oitzl, M. S., Philippsen, C., Richter, S., Bohringer, A., Wippich, W., & Schachinger, H. (2007). Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans. Learning & Memory, 14 , 109–116. Scollon, C. N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling: Promises and pitfalls, Strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 5–34. Seifert, T., & Heddersen, C. (2010). Intrinsic motivation and fl ow in skateboarding: An ethno- graphic study. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11 , 277–292. Selye, H. (1983). Selye’s guide to stress research (Vol. 3). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30 , 475–486. Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engage- ment in high school classrooms from the perspective of fl ow theory. School Psychology Quarterly, 18 , 158–176. Shin, D. C., & Johnson, D. M. (1978). Avowed happiness as an overall assessment of the quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 5 (475), 492. Showers, C. J., & Kling, K. C. (1996). Organization of self-knowledge: Implications for recovery from sad mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 , 578–590. Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997). Common blood fl ow changes across visual tasks: Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9 , 648–663. Siddle, D. A., Lipp, O. V., & Dall, P. (1996). The effects of task type and task requirements on the dissociation of skin conductance responses and secondary task probe reaction time. Psychophysiology, 33 , 73–83. Siegel, P., & Weinberger, J. (1998). Capturing the “mommy and I are one” merger fantasy: The oneness motive. In R. F. Bornstein & J. M. Masling (Eds.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories, Vol. 7. Empirical perspectives on the psychoanalytic unconscious (pp. 71–97). Washington, DC: APA. Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest – the curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 57–60. Skinner. B. F. (1971). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Knopf. Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (2009). Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H. D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affi liation, and power motives all at once: the Multi-Motive-Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74 , 126–145. Solomon, P. (1961). Sensory deprivation: A symposium held at Harvard Medical School . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112 , 140–154. Stanton, S. J., Hall, J. L., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2010). Properties of motive-specifi c incentives. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Stein, G. L., Kimiecik, J. C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S. A. (1995). Psychological antecedents of fl ow in recreational sport. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 , 125–135. Steiner, S., Diehl, B., Engeser, S., & Kehr, H. M. (2011). Nachhaltigkeits-Innovationen durch Nutzerintegration: Implikationen für eine optimierte NutzerInnenansprache und Gestaltung von Innovationsworkshops [Sustainable innovations through user integration: implications for optimization of addressing users and designing innovation workshops]. Zeitschrift für Umweltpsychologie, 15 , 52–70. Steptoe, A., Riiddel, H., & Neus, H. (1985). Clinical and methodological issues in cardiovascular psychophysiology . Berlin: Springer. 224 References

Stewart, A. J., & Chester, N. L. (1982). Sex differences in human social motives: Achievement, affi liation, and power. In A. J. Stewart (Ed.), Motivation and society. A volume in honor of David C. McClelland (pp. 172–218). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Stoll, O., & Lau, A. (2005). Flow-Erleben beim Marathonlauf – Zusammenhänge mit Anforderungspassung und Leistung. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 12 , 75–82. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18 , 643–662. Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about answers: The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology . San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Sutton, R. C. (2005). Peak performance of groups: An examination of the phenomenon in musical groups. Dissertation International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 65(10-A), 3908. Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104 , 17152–17156. Task Force of The European Society of Cardiology and The North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (Membership of the Task Force listed in the Appendix). (1996). Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. European Heart Journal, 17 , 354–381. Tauer, J. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1999). Winning isn’t everything: Competition, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35 , 209–238. Thatcher, A., Wretschko, G., & Fridjhon, P. (2008). Online fl ow experiences, problematic Internet use and Internet procrastination. Computers in Human Behavior, 24 , 2236–2254. Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., & Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, pre- frontal neural function, and cognitive performance: The neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37 , 141–153. Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed achievement motives: Concordance and predictive validity. Journal of Personality, 70 , 729–755. Trudewind, C. (1975). Häusliche Umwelt und Motiventwicklung [Home environment and motiva- tion] . Göttingen: Hogrefe. Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2009). Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: Introduction to the special section. Psychological Assessment, 21 , 457–462. Tsai, W.-C., Chen, H.-W., & Cheng, J.-W. (2009). Employee positive moods as a mediator linking transformational leadership and employee work outcomes. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 20 , 206–219. Uijtdehaage, S. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2000). Accentuated antagonism in the control of human heart rate. Clinical Autonomic Research, 10 , 107–110. Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Review Neuroscience, 10 , 397–409. Vallerand, R. J., Gauvin, L. I., & Halliwell, W. R. (1986). Effects of zero-sum competition on children’s intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Social Psychology, 126(4), 465–472. Vallerand, R., & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of perceived competence on intrinsic moti- vation: A test of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 94–102. Vansteenkiste, M., & Deci, E. L. (2003). Competitively contingent rewards and intrinsic motivation: Can losers remain motivated? Motivation & Emotion, 27(4), 273–299. Van Boxtel, A., & Jessurun, M. (1993). Amplitude and bilateral coherency of facial and jaw-elevator EMG activity as an index of effort during a two-choice serial reaction task. Psychophysiology, 30 , 589–604. Van Schaik, P., & Ling, J. (2011). An experimental analysis of experiential and cognitive variables in web navigation. Human-Computer Interaction Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation . New York: Wiley. References 225

Walker, C. J. (2010). Experiencing fl ow: Is doing it together better than doing it alone? The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5 , 3–11. Wang, C. K. J., Liu, W. C., & Khoo, A. (2009). The psychometric properties of dispositional fl ow scale-2 in Internet gaming. Current Psychology, 28 , 194–201. Wanner, B., Ladouceur, R., Auclair, A. V., & Vitaro, F. (2006). Flow and dissociation: Examination of mean levels, cross-links, and links to emotional well-being across sports and recreational and pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22 , 289–304. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 , 1063–1070. Webster, J., Trevino, L. K., & Ryan, L. (1993). The dimensionality and correlates of fl ow in human- computer interactions. Computers in Human Behavior, 9 , 411–429. Weibel, D., Wissmath, B., Habegger, S., Steiner, Y., & Groner, R. (2008). Playing online games against computer- vs. human-controlled opponents: Effects on presence, fl ow, and enjoyment. Computers in Human Behavior, 24 , 2274–2291. Weimar, D. (2005). Stress und Flow-Erleben. Eine empirische Untersuchung zur Bedeutung von Kognitionen, Emotionen und Motivation bei Lehramtsstudierenden, Referendaren und Lehrern. [Stress and fl ow-experience. An empirical investigation of the meaning of cognitions, emotions an motivation in teachers and teachers-in-training]. Berlin: Logos. Weinberger, J., Cotler, T., & Fishman, D. (2010). The duality of affi liative motivation. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 71–88). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complementary? In E. Tory Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York, NY: Guilford. Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation: From mechanism to cognition . Oxford: Markham. Wells, A. J. (1988). Self-esteem and optimal experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi (Ed.), Flow: The psychology of optimal experience (pp. 327–341). New York: Harper and Row. Wheeler, L., & Nezlek, J. (1977). Sex differences in social participation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 , 742–754. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121 , 331–354. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66 , 297–333. Wicklund, R. A., & Duval, T. S. (1971). Opinion change and performance facilitation as a result of objective self-awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7 , 319–342. Wilson, E., & Moneta, G. B. (in press). Flow as a way of coping: A qualitative study of the meta- cognitions of fl ow. In: Psychology of coping: New research . Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. 767–779. Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychosocial values by medical students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 , 767–779. Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive . New York, NY: Free. Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in women – and men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 , 510–519. Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text. Unpublished manu- script, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpretation of lives . New York: McGraw-Hill. Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105 , 230–250. Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1977). Power motive reliability as a function of retest instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45 , 436–440. 226 References

Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1978). The power motive. In H. London & J. E. Exner (Eds.), Dimensions of personality (pp. 391–448). New York: Wiley. Wong, M. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Affi liation motivation and daily experience: Some issues on gender differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 , 154–164. Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology . New York: Columbia University Press. World Health Organization. (1994). International statistical classifi cation of diseases and related health problems. ICD-10 . Geneva: WHO. Wright, J. J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2007). Further explorations into the conundrum of fl ow pro- cess. Journal of Occupational Science, 14 , 136–144. Wulf, G. (2007a). Attentional focus and motor learning: A review of 10 years of research (Target article). In E.-J. Hossner & N. Wenderoth (Eds.), Wulf on attentional focus and motor learning [Special issue]. Bewegung und Training, 1 , 14. Wulf, G. (2007b). Attention and motor skill learning . Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit- formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18 , 459–482. Yuan, Q.-H., Hu, J.-B., Wang, Y.-H. (2009). A study on psychometric properties of Chinese ver- sion of Flow State Scale in Chinese college students. J ournal of Clinical Psychology, 17 , 559–561. ZPID. (2011). Leibniz-Zentrum für Psychologische Information und Dokumentation . http://www. zpid.de/ . Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zuckerman, M. (2006). Sensation seeking and risky behavior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On the importance of self- determination for intrinsically motivated behaviour. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4 , 443–446. Zurbriggen, E. L. (2000). Social motives and cognitive power-sex associations: Predictors of aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 , 559–581. Index

A Autotelic Ability , 16, 55, 62, 77, 81, 93, 94, 98, 116, personality , 14, 72, 140, 149, 152, 159, 118, 126, 140, 149, 156, 158–161, 166, 165–186, 189, 190, 195 170, 171, 175, 178, 180, 181, 183, 185, quality , 67, 69, 70, 84 186, 189, 195 Avoidance orientations , 171 Above average , 36, 54–57, 62, 63, 72, 75, 79 Absolute difference model , 37–38 Academic performance , 80 B Achievement Behaviorism , 8, 9 fl ow , 88, 176–180, 184 Boredom , 2, 9, 10, 26–29, 33, 67, 71, 72, 75, fl ow motive , 170–171, 173–175, 183 78, 95, 116, 125, 142, 144, 157, 163, motivation , 11, 22, 40, 61, 79, 179 170, 192 motive , 58, 64, 90–92, 94, 99, 102, 104, Bottom-up , 77, 85, 197 105, 169–175, 183, 189 Boundary conditions , 52, 53, 63, 168–169, orientation , 60, 61 183, 185, 189 situations , 87–89, 188, 195 Brain , 141, 149–152, 154, 157–163, 197 Action opportunities , 89, 91, 93, 94, 96, 100–104, 106, 107, 188 C orientation , 61, 67, 72, 135, 170, 178, 181 Cardiovascular measures , 143, 156–158, 189 Activity-oriented approach , 69, 70 Causal effects , 66, 83 Activity-specifi c incentives , 12–13 Centering of attention , 2–4, 27, 130 Addiction , 6, 125–129, 134, 152, 189 CET. See Cognitive evaluation theory (CET) Affective , 8, 21, 57, 65–85, 88, 131, 143, 148, Channel model , 34–36, 38, 39, 54, 56, 159, 171–174, 176, 179, 180, 188 144, 145 Affi liation-intimacy motive , 90–93, 95–100, Classical test theory , 41, 44 104–107, 173, 183 Classifi cation system , 34, 36, 50 Affi liative situations , 96, 98, 106 Clear goals , 3, 52, 53, 63, 89, 133 Aggregation of experience Clinical , 18, 22, 129, 198 sampling data , 32, 37 Clustering , 78 Antecedents , 43, 44, 46, 48, 49, 52, 53, Cognitive , 46, 65–85, 112, 134, 141–145, 148, 61–63, 84 149, 151, 154, 159, 162, 164, 176, 179, Anxiety , 2, 4, 5, 9, 10, 13, 26–29, 33, 72, 116, 188, 197–198 125, 126, 144, 163, 192, 198 Cognitive capacity , 77, 197 Approach orientations , 61, 169, 171 Cognitive evaluation theory Arts , 18, 22, 133 (CET) , 109–121, 189 Autonomy orientation , 61 Combat fl ow , 131–132

227 228 Index

Compatibility effect , 60, 76 E Compensatory model of work motivation and EDA. See Electrodermal activity (EDA) volition , 94 Educational , 17, 22, 125 Competence , 10, 61, 69, 79, 92–96, 104, 106, Effort , 2, 76, 88, 93, 106, 117, 129, 140, 142, 107, 109–117, 119–121, 126, 130, 134, 143, 146, 148–150, 152–154, 157–160, 155, 189, 201 162, 163, 170 Competition , 81, 102 Electrodermal activity (EDA) , 141–143, Complexity , 27, 45, 48, 167, 188, 189, 192 158–159, 161 Componential approach , 27, 40–49, 192, 193 Electromyography , 152–153 Components , 2–8, 12, 14, 18–20, 40, 41, 43, Emotional dialectics , 181 49, 62, 67–69, 72, 76, 79, 82, 84, 93, Enjoyment , 2, 6, 8–12, 16, 21, 39, 52, 57, 60, 94, 99, 128, 130, 135, 159, 171–175, 64, 67, 68, 70–75, 82, 109–121, 140, 183, 192–194, 196, 201 149, 152, 163, 166, 189 Conditions , 4, 5, 17–20, 34, 35, 37, 50–53, 56, Environmental characteristic , 59, 131 58, 60–64, 67, 70, 75, 76, 78, 83, 94, ESF. See Experience sampling form (ESF) 110, 112, 129, 132, 133, 135, 140, 148, ESM. See Experience sampling 158, 163, 168–170, 178, 183–185, 189 method (ESM) Confi rmatory factor analysis , 39 Ethic , 134–135 Confounding variables , 84 Experience fl uctuation model , 34–36, 49, 50 Consequence , 60, 66, 69–74, 84, 118, 130, Experience of time , 3, 52, 64 134, 143, 144, 155 Experience sampling form (ESF) , 14, 15, 19, Construct validity , 39–41, 47, 48 30–33, 97, 98, 196 Consumer behavior , 17 Experience sampling method (ESM) , 7, 14, Continuous , 47, 120, 193 15, 17, 19, 21, 24, 30–33, 35–37, 40, phenomenon , 192, 193 43, 45–48, 70, 72, 73, 75, 83, 88, 89, Correlational , 7, 19, 53, 66, 73, 74, 79, 80, 97, 98, 110, 111, 121, 167, 175, 82–84, 197 196–197 Correspondence of means and ends Experimental , 20, 54, 56, 66, 67, 71–73, 75, approach , 69 76, 78, 81–84, 141–143, 152, 153, 158, Cortisol , 142, 143, 153–158, 161, 164, 189 162, 180, 197 Creativity , 8, 15–17, 22, 80, 114, 124, 141, 188 Explicit motives , 88, 92–95, 105, 106 Cross-sectional , 19, 82, 197 Extension memory , 176, 178–179

D F Daily experience , 14–15, 17, 19, 21, 22, 27, Fear of failure , 61, 169, 173, 179, 184, 195 30–40, 45, 47, 48, 50, 88, 188 of control , 2–5, 10, 12, 19, 20, 27, 52, Day reconstruction method (DRM) , 45, 499 62, 64, 125, 130 Deep concentration , 77, 150 Fit , 37, 43, 52–64, 67, 71, 72, 75, 76, 79, 84, Deep fl ow , 28, 29, 45, 57, 58, 76, 192, 197 104, 190, 193, 201 Defi nition , 2, 4, 6, 18, 21, 23, 24, 27, 69, 70, Flow 124, 142, 144, 148, 150, 159–160, 166, channel , 7, 36, 54–56, 72, 144, 145 171, 179 complexity , 27 Demands , 2, 27, 52, 66, 92, 116, 141, 170, 190 in groups , 18, 88, 99–100, 105 Depletion , 62, 64, 77, 169 indicator , 140 Deprivation , 10, 133, 198 intensity , 29, 45, 48, 56–58, 62, 129, 192 Development , 2, 9, 11, 37, 43, 45–47, 73, 113, model , 9, 10, 18, 50–64, 117, 166, 169, 120, 167, 168, 182–184, 195, 199 188, 190, 192, 193, 198, 199 Dialectical principle , 167, 180–183 personality , 168 Disinterested interest , 167 prevalence , 27, 29, 40, 43, 45, 47, 48 Dispositional fl ow scale-2 , 41, 42, 168, 175 questionnaire , 24, 25, 27, 47, 48, Dopamine , 141, 143, 151–152, 161, 162 168, 192 DRM. See Day reconstruction method (DRM) short scale , 3, 56, 82, 196, 201 Dynamical system theory , 46 state scale-2 , 41, 42 Index 229

theory , 9–11, 24, 25, 29, 30, 33–37, 39, 44, Intrinsic motivation , 22, 39, 52, 60, 61, 68–71, 46, 47, 52–54, 57, 63, 68, 74, 78, 84, 73, 74, 84, 112–114, 116, 117, 88, 104, 109–121, 133, 144, 147, 161, 119–121, 133, 165, 188, 189, 194 163, 168, 170, 176, 178–179, 183, 189 Involvement , 16, 32, 33, 39, 52, 64, 67–73, 75, Flow-affect-relationship , 74–75 82, 97, 114, 117–118, 121, 124, 157 Flow hypothesis of motivational competence , 92–96, 106 Free-choice task , 71 L Frequency , 29, 57–58, 64, 74, 76, 81, 157, Latent construct , 41, 44, 45 167, 168, 175, 191–194 Leadership , 60, 103, 105 Functional analysis , 179 Learning , 6, 12, 13, 16, 17, 20, 22, 69, 80, 82, Functional approach , 176–177, 190 83, 88, 90, 92, 93, 111, 112, 124, 129, Funktionslust , 11 134, 136, 141, 171–173, 180 environments , 83 Leisure , 12–14, 16, 17, 22, 27, 32–34, 41, 48, G 112, 124, 129, 133, 196, 199 Game addiction , 128–129 Life satisfaction , 73–77, 83, 196 Games , 9, 20, 65, 79, 82, 111, 112, 117–120, Longitudinal , 19, 76, 82–84, 195–197 125, 126, 143, 152, 183, 195 Loss of self-consciousness , 2, 3, 11, 15, 27, Goal , 2, 5, 20, 30, 33, 46, 53, 59, 60, 69, 96, 41, 52, 166, 168, 193, 194 110–112, 118, 121, 125, 130, 134, 166, 167, 169, 171, 184 M Measurement , 17–19, 22–50, 63, 66, 70, 73, H 81, 135, 167–168, 171–174, 183, 188, Happiness , 7–8, 15, 21, 22, 32, 33, 39, 73–76, 193, 196 129, 134, 140 method , 23–26, 29–32, 40–41, 45–48 Heart rate , 156, 157 Media use , 17, 22 Heart rate variability (HRV) , 62, 143, 157 Merging of action and awareness , 2–4, 27, 40, Hierarchical linear modeling , 37 49, 66, 128, 149, 165 Human–computer interaction , 17, 22, Metacognition , 46 128, 142 Metaskills , 167 Mission , 2, 21 Motivation , 5, 30, 52, 66, 90, 111, 124, 145, I 165, 190 Immediate feedback , 5, 128, 133 Motivational orientation , 67, 111, 112, 116, Immersion , 2, 4, 20, 21 190, 196 Implicit motives , 89, 90, 92–96, 104–106, 176, Multilevel modeling , 37 183 Music , 13, 16, 18, 22, 75, 79, 80, 82, 97, 99, Incentives , 6, 12–15, 59, 88–92, 94–98, 133, 135, 153, 157 100–102, 104–107, 167, 169, 174, 199 Intensity , 25, 29, 30, 41, 44, 45, 48, 49, 52, 56–59, 61, 62, 64, 74, 76, 129, 141, N 148, 154, 167, 168, 175, 191–194 Need for achievement , 75, 169, 195 Intention , 110, 119, 176, 177, 179, 185 Negative memory , 176–179, 182, 184 affect , 76, 93, 152, 171, 172, 174, 176, 184 Interest , 2, 8, 11, 17–19, 21, 22, 30, 39, 69, 70, consequences , 18, 31, 112, 127–130, 134, 72–73, 115, 120, 126, 128, 140, 142, 135, 137, 140 143, 150, 158, 166, 167, 171, 172, 174, 181, 184, 190, 196, 198, 199 Internal locus of control , 61, 67, 72, 170, 181 O Internet addiction , 128–129 Octant model , 34, 54, 56, 63, 72, 76 Interview , 9, 13, 19, 117, 191 Operant motive test (OMT) , 90, 171–175, 183 230 Index

Opportunities , 5, 9, 10, 16, 39, 89, 91–107, Relaxation , 7, 27, 72, 73, 156, 158, 161, 164, 167, 181, 185, 188, 198 184, 195, 196 Optimal challenge , 36–39, 45, 109, 110, 112, Religious experiences , 18, 22 114–120, 144, 148, 160, 163, 184, 189 Restored positive affect , 181, 183 Optimal experience , 2, 6–7, 17, 21, 45, 83, Revised model , 54, 58–59, 166 110, 111, 121, 133 Reward/Rewarding , 2, 5, 7, 9–11, 15, 16, 18, Optimal motivational , 124 20–22, 52, 58, 66, 68, 78–79, 88–90, Optimal stimulation , 10 98, 102, 104, 105, 110, 117, 126, 127, Overload , 67, 71, 72, 78, 157, 170 129–134, 136, 137, 151–152, 156, 162, 166, 180, 192, 194, 195 Risk-taking , 125, 129–131, 169, 189 P Peak experience , 22, 45, 127 Perceived autonomy , 109, 119–120, 189 S Perceived competence , 109, 110, 112–117, Self-consciousness , 1–3, 11–12, 15, 19, 27, 41, 119–121, 189 44, 52, 61, 62, 64, 66, 77, 97, 128, 166, Performance , 2, 6, 7, 17–19, 21, 22, 45, 65–84, 168, 193, 194 87, 88, 90, 91, 95, 112–118, 121, 124, Self-determination approach , 69 126–128, 133, 134, 142, 144, 146, Self-regulation , 46, 77, 169–170, 189 148–152, 154, 156–159, 167, 168, 171, Self-regulatory abilities , 170 181, 182, 185, 188, 194, 197–198, 201 Skills , 5, 6, 9–11, 13, 14, 16, 19, 21, 22, 25, 26, Personal causation , 11 28–30, 33, 34, 36, 37, 47–48, 50, Personality , 1, 8, 13, 14, 18, 22, 39, 48, 51–53, 52–59, 61–64, 66–67, 70–73, 75, 76, 58, 61, 64, 72, 73, 75, 79, 80, 92, 106, 78, 79, 81, 84, 85, 87–92, 95, 98, 99, 135, 140, 150, 159, 165–185, 189, 190, 104–106, 109–111, 115, 117, 118, 126, 195 129–130, 137, 143, 144, 148, 149, 157, Personality systems interaction (PSI) , 159, 166, 167, 170, 185, 188, 190, 193, 176–179, 183, 184 195, 198, 201 Personalized power motive , 101–102 Skills-demands-compatibility , 53, 157 Person characteristics , 59 Socialized power motive , 101–104 Physiology , 141, 143, 148–151, 154, 162, 193 Social situations , 88, 96–105, 107, 188 Play , 8–11, 13, 22, 58, 64, 81–82, 89, 100, Sports , 3, 6, 7, 12, 17, 18, 20, 22, 40, 79–82, 112, 118, 166, 167, 179, 188, 198 88, 94, 99, 100, 112, 124, 130, 133, Positive affect , 73–77, 85, 101, 102, 113, 197, 199 128, 142, 152, 162, 171–185, Standardization of experience 188, 190 sampling data , 45 Power Standardized questionnaire , 19, 31, 41 motive , 90–93, 95, 100–104, 188 State-oriented , 170 situations , 88, 100–104, 107 Stress , 7, 13, 93, 140, 142–147, 150, 153–156, Precondition , 66–68, 70, 72–74, 79, 84, 85 158, 159, 161, 163, 164, 189, 196 Precursors , 10–12, 21 Structure , 5, 9, 10, 21, 31, 45, 48, 52, 53, 58, Processing styles , 77–78, 85, 197 64, 78, 95, 98, 99, 102, 103, 105, 106, Psychophysiology , 139–147, 156, 161, 162 188, 190, 197, 198

Q T Quadrant model , 32–34, 36, 49, 50, 54, 89 Top-down , 78, 85, 197 Trait , 3, 27, 39–49, 61, 135, 140, 159, 166–170, 175, 176, 180–186 R Transformation of time , 66, 166, 168 Reasons for action , 9, 194–195 Reduced positive affect , 177, 180–181, 184 Regression modeling approach , 36–40, 188 U Regulatory fi t , 57, 59, 60, 64, 190 Unambiguous feedback , 3, 41, 49, 52, 53, Regulatory mode theory , 60 63, 87 Index 231

V W Validity , 19, 30, 39–41, 45, 48, 83, 175–176, Well-being , 15–18, 22, 51, 74, 93, 95, 102, 188–190 128, 133, 134, 169, 181, 185, 188 Value , 16, 31–34, 52, 56–64, 84, 97, 125, 133, 134, 188, 190–191, 196 Volitional depletion , 169 Y Yes-or-no phenomenon , 191, 192