Effects of Awe on Memory for Script-Typical and –Irrelevant Narrative Detail

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Effects of Awe on Memory for Script-Typical and –Irrelevant Narrative Detail Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 1 Going Off Script: Effects of Awe on Memory for Script-Typical and –Irrelevant Narrative Detail Alexander F. Danvers & Michelle N. Shiota Arizona State University Date of Revision Submission: December 28, 2016 Corresponding Author: Michelle N. Shiota Department of Psychology Arizona State University P.O. Box 871104 Tempe, AZ 85287-1104 Tel: (480) 727-8628 Fax: (480) 965-8544 Email: [email protected] Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 2 Abstract People often filter their experience of new events through knowledge they already have, e.g., encoding new events by relying on prototypical event “scripts” at the expense of actual details. Previous research suggests that positive affect often increases this tendency. Three studies assessed whether awe—an emotion elicited by perceived vastness, and thought to promote cognitive accommodation—has the opposite effect, reducing rather than increasing reliance on event scripts. True/false questions on details of a short story about a romantic dinner were used to determine whether awe (1) reduces the tendency to impute script-consistent but false details into memory, and/or (2) promotes memory of unexpected details. Across studies we consistently found support for the first effect; evidence for the second was less consistent. Effects were partially mediated by subjective awe, and independent of other aspects of subjective affect. Results suggest that awe reduces reliance on internal knowledge in processing new events. Keywords: Awe, Positive Emotion, Cognition, Memory Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 3 There are moments in life where one is struck anew by the vastness, beauty, and complexity of the surrounding world. Standing at the rim of the Grand Canyon, or sitting on the roof of a friend’s house looking up at the stars, one seems to shed the perspective and expectations bounded by mundane, day-to-day experience. At these times, people may feel that they see the world in a new light, considering deeper questions and opening their minds to what the universe has to offer. But do moments of awe change the way people process information at a concrete level? The three studies presented here build on previous research on the effects of awe on cognitive processing, asking whether awe helps suppress people’s tendency to see what they expect to see, and whether awe facilitates taking in new information from the environment. Positive Affect and Cognitive Processing A rich research tradition demonstrates that emotions influence perception, interpretation, and memory of information from one’s surroundings, sometimes in subtle ways. While much of this research has emphasized implications of emotional stress or distress for cognitive processing, many studies have focused on characterizing differences between positive- and negative-valence affect. Much research has suggested that positive and negative mood states each tend to facilitate a particular processing style: global, heuristic-driven, and top-down processing for positive mood versus local, systematic, detail-oriented, and bottom-up processing for negative mood (Schwarz & Bless, 1991; Schwarz & Clore, 2007; Storbeck & Clore, 2005). In a happy mood people attend more strongly to global than to local aspects of complex figures (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005) and, in a “rose-colored glasses effect,” find consumer products more appealing (Pham 2007). Positive affect has also been found to increase individuals’ reliance on stereotypes (e.g., Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser, 1994; Huntsinger, Sinclair, & Clore, 2009), judgment heuristics (Ruder & Bless, 2003), and number rather than quality of Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 4 persuasive arguments (Bless, Mackie, & Schwarz, 1992; Mackie & Worth, 1989) in processing information about new people or making decisions. Although these cognitive shortcuts are efficient and adaptive in certain situations, helping people to get the gist of new information quickly, they tend to impair accurate encoding of detail. For example, studies have found that people in a positive mood are more likely to rely on event scripts in encoding details of a novel story about a commonplace kind of event (Bless, Schwarz, Clore, Golisano, & Rabe, 1996). In studies documenting this effect, participants complete an experimental mood manipulation, then listen to a lengthy story about a kind of event for which people in the mainstream United States are expected to have a rich mental prototype, such as a couple going out to a romantic dinner. When later completing true/false questions about whether certain details were present in the story, participants in a positive mood show a heightened tendency toward contamination of their memory by the event script—they identify as true details that you might expect in a romantic dinner, but that were not in fact present in the story (Bless et al., 1996). Research documenting these effects typically uses cognitive tasks whose content is unrelated to the preceding affect induction. For example, Bless and colleagues (1996) induced positive mood by asking participants to write about a happy time in their lives, or to watch clips from the films Dead Poets Society or Flashdance—affective stimuli unrelated to the “romantic dinner” story or to the subsequent memory task. This enhances the internal validity of such studies, controlling cognitive task details and reducing the likelihood that cognitive effects are driven solely by properties of the affect-changing stimulus, as distinct from the affect evoked by that stimulus. This approach also has important implications for theory, demonstrating “carryover effects” – activation of cognitive processing styles that are maintained beyond Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 5 exposure to emotion-eliciting or mood-changing stimuli. In this way, researchers can document cognitive effects of short-lived emotions that can linger beyond the emotions themselves, as well as effects of moods that are thought to be inherently longer-lasting and less stimulus bound (for more on the distinction between emotion and mood, see Beedie, Terry, & Lane, 2005). Taken together, the research reviewed above suggests that positive moods commonly encourage activation of and reliance on internal knowledge structures (heuristics, cognitive schemas and scripts) relevant to one’s current situation, at the expense of close attention to details of the actual situation (Fiedler, 2001). Various theories have been proposed regarding the mechanisms and boundary conditions of this effect. According to affect-as-information (AAI) theory, for example, affect valence is used to guide the investment of effort in information processing (Clore, 1992; Schwarz & Clore, 2007). Positive mood signals that the environment is safe and predictable, so it is okay to rely on heuristics and internalized knowledge, whereas negative mood signals a problem requiring more careful attention and systematic processing. The affect as cognitive feedback (ACF) model, an updating of AAI theory, holds that instead of preferentially activating a particular processing style, affect valence acts as a meta- cognitive signal in which positive affect increases confidence and promotes continued use of one’s current or default style, whereas negative affect prompts a switch to a different style (Bless, 2001; Huntsinger, Isbell, & Clore, 2012; Huntsinger, Isbell, & Clore, 2014; Isbell, Lair, & Rovenpor, 2013). For example, in contrast to theorizing that positive affect always increases global attentional focus (Isbell, 2004; Isbell, Burns, & Haar, 2005), positive mood has been found to increase local focus in subsequent tasks, provided that local focus was primed prior to mood manipulation (Huntsinger, Clore, & Bar-Anan, 2010). The typical effects of positive mood highlighted by AAI theory are presumably observed because, in the absence of a reason to Awe and Memory for Narrative Detail 6 expend effort on systematic, bottom-up information processing, reliance on heuristics and stored, internal knowledge is the default cognitive style (Schwarz, 2001). Hedonic contingency theory (Wegener & Petty, 1994) offers a different account of the findings above. According to this model, the core impact of positive affect is to increase motivation to maintain one’s pleasant mood. While this decreases one’s motivation to carefully, systematically process content that is unpleasant, serious, or counter-attitudinal (common among studies examining mood effects on processing of persuasive messages), it may increase systematic attention to inherently pleasant stimuli. This moderating effect was supported by multiple studies (Wegener, Petty, & Smith, 1995). While differing in regard to the theorized mechanism of affect valence effects, the AAI, ACF, and hedonic contingency models have a few things in common. First, they assume that the effects of positive affect/mood are consistent – they do not postulate or test differences among varieties of positive emotion. Assuming that the default cognitive style and information- processing task are held constant, therefore, different positive-valence emotions should show similar effects. Second, all three theories assume that changing one’s default information processing style is effortful, and thus requires some form of motivation. Third, in each theory, motivation to change one’s processing style is altered by the subjective experience of positive affect, which either warns that the current style may be problematic (AAI and ACF), or sensitizes the individual
Recommended publications
  • Finding the Golden Mean: the Overuse, Underuse, and Optimal Use of Character Strengths
    Counselling Psychology Quarterly ISSN: 0951-5070 (Print) 1469-3674 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ccpq20 Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec To cite this article: Ryan M. Niemiec (2019): Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths, Counselling Psychology Quarterly, DOI: 10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 Published online: 20 May 2019. Submit your article to this journal View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ccpq20 COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 ARTICLE Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec VIA Institute on Character, Cincinnati, OH, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY The science of well-being has catalyzed a tremendous amount of Received 28 February 2019 research with no area more robust in application and impact than Accepted 8 May 2019 the science of character strengths. As the empirical links between KEYWORDS character strengths and positive outcomes rapidly grow, the research Character strengths; around strength imbalances and the use of strengths with problems strengths overuse; strengths and conflicts is nascent. The use of character strengths in understand- underuse; optimal use; ing and handling life suffering as well as emerging from it, is particularly second wave positive aligned within second wave positive psychology. Areas of particular psychology; golden mean promise include strengths overuse and strengths underuse, alongside its companion of strengths optimaluse.Thelatterisviewedasthe golden mean of character strengths which refers to the expression of the right combination of strengths, to the right degree, and in the right situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Awakening to Awe, Intro
    1 AWAKENING TO AWE: PERSONAL STORIES OF PROFOUND TRANSFORMATION Kirk J. Schneider, Ph.D. In press, Jason Aronson (an imprint of Rowman Littlefield) Dedication: To William James and Rollo May: Torchbearers for mystery 2 Table of Contents PREFACE ……………………………………………………………………………4 INTRODUCTION: AWE-WAKENING…………………………………………..8 The Nature and Power of Awe………………………………………………………..11 PART I: OUR AWE-DEPLETED AGE…………………………………………..13 Chapter 1: What Keeps Us From Venturing?.........................................................15 Happiness vs. Depth…………………………………………………………………..16 Fiddling While Rome Burns: The “Answer” Driven Life……………………………20 Chapter 2: The Stodgy Road to Awe………………………………………………..22 Objective Uncertainty, Held Fast……………………………………………………...25 Awe as Adventure, Not Merely Bliss………………………………………………….25 Chapter 3: The Awe Survey: Format and Background……………………………26 PART II: AWE-BASED AWAKENING: THE NEW TESTAMENTS TO SPIRIT Chapter 4: Awakening in Education………………………………………………..30 Jim Hernandez: From Gang Leader to Youth Educator………………………………39 Interview with Donna Marshall, Vice-Principle at Jim’s School……………………...63 Julia van der Ryn and a Class in Awe-Inspired Ethics………………………………...68 Chapter 5: Awakening from Childhood Trauma………………………………….79 J. Fraser Pierson and the Awe of Natural Living………………………………………79 Candice Hershman and the Awe of Unknowing……………………………………....104 Chapter 6: Awakening from Drug Addiction…………………………………..…122 3 Michael Cooper and Awe-Based Rehab………………………………………………122 Chapter 7: Awakening through Chronic Illness………………………………….148
    [Show full text]
  • Supporting Grieving Students | a Handbook for Teachers & Administrators | Vii Viii | Stanfordchildrens.Org Introduction
    Supporting Grieving Students A HANDBOOK FOR TEACHERS AND ADMINISTRatORS ii | stanfordchildrens.org Supporting Grieving Students A HANDBOOK FOR TEACHERS AND ADMINISTRatORS Supporting Grieving Students | iii iv | stanfordchildrens.org Created by the Family Guidance and Bereavement Program, Lucile Packard Children’s Hospital Stanford. This content was created by Stanford Children’s Health with information courtesy of the Dougy Center. Special thanks to the LPCH Auxiliaries Program for sponsorship. Supporting Grieving Students | v CONTENts Introduction ix MODULE 1 | Understanding Grieving Children 2 Common Responses of the Grieving Child or Teen 4 How to Tell When Students Need Additional Help 5 MODULE 2 | Developmental Issues of Grieving Students 6 The Grieving Infant and Toddler 8 The Grieving Preschool Child 9 The Grieving Elementary School Student 10 The Grieving Middle School Student 12 The Grieving High School Student 13 MODULE 3 | How Teachers Can Help Grieving Students 14 Your Important Role In Helping Students Cope with a Death 15 Groundwork for Dealing with Grieving Students in Your Class 15 MODULE 4 | Optimal Support for Grieving Students at School 16 Helping All Students Understand Death 17 Ongoing Support for a Grieving Student 17 How Teachers Can Help Grieving Families 21 Words and Actions that Offer Comfort and Invite Communication 23 Words and Actions that Don’t Offer Comfort 24 Common Difficult-to-Manage Behavioral Grief Reactions 25 Key Points to Remember 26 Frequently Asked Questions 28 What Administrators Can Do to Help
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship Between Time Perception and Awe-Inspiring
    The relationship between time perception and awe- inspiring landscapes by day- and night-time ALLYNE E.A. GROEN, S1745670 MARKETING COMMUNICATION AND DESIGN, THOMAS J.L. VAN ROMPAY April 2020 Abstract OBJECTIVE: Various researchers have started to study the effects of the emotion ‘awe’ on people over the last few years. The focus of the current study is on the effects awe-inspiring, natural landscapes on time perception. This research will show what kind of landscape would be most suitable for, for instance, a waiting room. On top of that, time of day is used as a moderating variable to see if this influences time perception as well. The concepts of connectedness, alertness, nervousness and stress are taken into account as extra test variables in this research to see if landscapes can improve an environment through those feelings as well. METHOD: Using VR (Virtual Reality), digital landscapes were created varying in ‘level of awe’ and ‘time of day’. This resulted in a 2 (high-awe versus low-awe) by 2 (daytime versus night-time) between subject design. Participants (n=127) were shown a 45-second-long VR animation of a natural landscape, after which they filled in a questionnaire with validated scales related to all the aforementioned variables. RESULTS: The analysis of the data shows that while night-time, low-awe landscapes made time go by faster, they also made participants feel more alert. Participants influenced by a daytime, high-awe environment, however, were more likely to spend more of their time on helping another person and felt significantly less nervous.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Work Practice with Individuals and Families Who Have Experienced Pregnancy Loss
    Smith ScholarWorks Theses, Dissertations, and Projects 2014 Social work practice with individuals and families who have experienced pregnancy loss Sandra J. Stokes Smith College Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Stokes, Sandra J., "Social work practice with individuals and families who have experienced pregnancy loss" (2014). Masters Thesis, Smith College, Northampton, MA. https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses/817 This Masters Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations, and Projects by an authorized administrator of Smith ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Sandra Stokes Social Work Practice with Individuals and Families Who Have Experienced Pregnancy Loss ABSTRACT Approximately 10 to 15 percent of known pregnancies worldwide end in miscarriage and an additional one in 160 pregnancies end in stillbirth. Because of a lack of evidence-based research to support interventions in this work, social workers have come to rely on their own practical experience. This study investigated social workers' professional practice experience in order to outline the frequency and impact of pregnancy loss and to assess the formation and maintenance of current practices for effective and efficient treatment. This study utilized in- depth, qualitative interviews to further understand how clinicians in this field define and conceptualize their work. The major findings from this set of data were in the areas of: preparatory experience, fundamental approach to patients, and the ways in which clinicians continue to educate themselves on the topic of pregnancy loss. Two important themes emerged from clinicians’ experiences in perinatal social work; the first was the impact of clinicians' personal experiences with childbirth and child rearing and the second was the ways in which social workers understood their roles within interdisciplinary teams.
    [Show full text]
  • Inspired to Create: Awe Enhances Openness to Learning and the Desire for Experiential Creation
    Inspired to Create: Awe Enhances Openness to Learning and the Desire for Experiential Creation MELANIE RUDD CHRISTIAN HILDEBRAND KATHLEEN D. VOHS (Forthcoming at the Journal of Marketing Research) Melanie Rudd is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Marketing and Entrepreneurship at the University of Houston, C. T. Bauer College of Business, 334 Melcher Hall, Houston, TX 77204-6021 (email: [email protected]; phone: 713-743-5848). Christian Hildebrand is an Associate Professor of Marketing Analytics at the Institute of Management at the University of Geneva, 40 Bd du Pont d’Arve, 1211 Genève 4, Switzerland (email: [email protected]; phone: +41 22 379-8589). Kathleen D. Vohs is a Distinguished McKnight University Professor and the Land O’ Lakes Chair in Marketing at the University of Minnesota, Carlson School of Management, 321 19th Avenue South, Minneapolis, MN 55455- 9940 (email: [email protected]; phone: 612-625-8331). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Melanie Rudd. 1 Inspired to Create: Awe Enhances Openness to Learning and the Desire for Experiential Creation Abstract Automated fabrication, home services, and premade goods pervade the modern consumer landscape. Against this backdrop, this research explored how an emotion—awe—could motivate consumers to instead partake in experiential creation (i.e., activities wherein one actively produces an outcome) by enhancing their willingness to learn. Across eight experiments, experiencing awe (vs. happiness, excitement, pride, amusement, and/or neutrality) increased people’s likelihood of choosing an experiential creation gift (vs. one not involving experiential creation), willingness to pay for experiential creation products (vs. comparable ready-made products), likelihood of creating a bespoke snack (vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Analysis of Joy and Awe in Four Cultures Daria B
    James Madison University JMU Scholarly Commons Dissertations The Graduate School Spring 2017 Being and beholding: Comparative analysis of joy and awe in four cultures Daria B. White James Madison University Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/diss201019 Part of the Counselor Education Commons, and the Multicultural Psychology Commons Recommended Citation White, Daria B., "Being and beholding: Comparative analysis of joy and awe in four cultures" (2017). Dissertations. 150. https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/diss201019/150 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the The Graduate School at JMU Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of JMU Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Being and Beholding: Comparative Analysis of Joy and Awe in Four Cultures Daria Borislavova White A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty of JAMES MADISON UNIVERSITY In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the degree of Doctor in Philosophy Department of Graduate Psychology May 2017 FACULTY COMMITTEE: Committee Chair: Dr. Debbie Sturm Committee Members: Dr. Lennis Echterling Dr. Cara Meixner Dedication I dedicate this dissertation to all the wonderful people who shared the most precious “eternal moments” of their lives with me. To all of you, I offer my heartfelt gratitude. I have rejoiced in reading and rereading your stories, living with your memories, learning from you all how to be be-filled and beholding. I write in remembrance of two beloved people who died in 2008 – my brother Ivaylo, whose bear hugs, warmth and sharing nature were a shelter for me, and my friend Mirela, whose pure soul, tinkling laughter and sharp intelligence are irreplaceable.
    [Show full text]
  • Welcome to Positive Psychology 1
    01-Snyder-4997.qxd 6/13/2006 5:07 PM Page 3 Welcome to Positive Psychology 1 The gross national product does not allow for the health of our children,...their education, or the joy of their play. It does not include the beauty of our poetry or the strength of our marriages; the intelligence of our public debate or the integrity of our public officials. It measures neither wit nor courage; neither our wisdom nor our teaching; neither our compassion nor our devotion to our country; it measures everything, in short, except that which makes life worthwhile. —Robert F. Kennedy, 1968 he final lines in this 1968 address delivered by Robert F. Kennedy at T the University of Kansas point to the contents of this book: the things in life that make it worthwhile. In this regard, however, imagine that some- one offered to help you understand human beings but in doing so would teach you only about their weaknesses and pathologies. As far-fetched as this sounds, a similar “What is wrong with people?” question guided the thinking of most applied psychologists (clinical, counseling, school, etc.) during the 20th century. Given the many forms of human fallibility, this question produced an avalanche of insights into the human “dark side.” As the 21st century unfolds, however, we are beginning to ask another question: “What is right about people?” This question is at the heart of the burgeoning initiative in positive psychology, which is the scientific and applied approach to uncovering people’s strengths and promoting their positive functioning. (See the article “Building Human Strength,”in which positive psychology pioneer Martin Seligman gives his views about the need for this new field.) 3 01-Snyder-4997.qxd 6/13/2006 5:07 PM Page 4 4 LOOKING AT PSYCHOLOGY FROM A POSITIVE PERSPECTIVE Building Human Strength: Psychology’s Forgotten Mission MARTIN E.
    [Show full text]
  • AWE and WAITING 1 Awe-Full Uncertainty: Easing
    Running head: AWE AND WAITING 1 Awe-Full Uncertainty: Easing Discomfort During Waiting Periods Kyla Rankin1 Sara E. Andrews1 Kate Sweeny1 1University of California, Riverside In press at the Journal of Positive Psychology (2019) AWE AND WAITING 2 Abstract Waiting for uncertain news is a common and stressful experience. We examined whether experiencing awe can promote well-being during these uncomfortable periods of uncertainty. Across two studies (total N = 729), we examined the relationship between trait awe and well- being as participants awaited feedback on a novel intelligence test or ratings from peers following a group interaction. These studies further examined the effect of an awe induction, compared to positive and neutral control conditions, on well-being. We found partial support for a relationship between trait awe and well-being during waiting periods, particularly with positive emotion. We also found partial support for the benefits of an awe induction: People consistently experienced greater positive emotion and less anxiety in the awe condition compared to a neutral control condition, although these benefits did not always improve upon the positive control experience. Importantly, these benefits emerged regardless of one’s predisposition to experiencing awe. Keywords: awe, well-being, emotion, uncertainty Abstract word count: 149 AWE AND WAITING 3 Waiting for uncertain news is a common and stressful experience. People may feel uncertain as they wait to learn the outcome of a cancer screening, a job interview, a home purchase, or an academic exam. Although many examples of uncertain waiting periods come readily to mind, still relatively little is known about the best way to manage the thoughts and feelings that arise as people await the uncertain outcome.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychological Benefits of Wea
    Intuition: The BYU Undergraduate Journal of Psychology Volume 11 Issue 1 Article 6 2015 The Psychological Benefits of weA Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/intuition Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation (2015) "The Psychological Benefits of we,A " Intuition: The BYU Undergraduate Journal of Psychology: Vol. 11 : Iss. 1 , Article 6. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/intuition/vol11/iss1/6 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals at BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Intuition: The BYU Undergraduate Journal of Psychology by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. et al.: Psychological Benefits of Awe Psychological Benefits of Awe Many people, regardless of their unique personality The Psychological Benefits of Awe and life experience, have felt the powerful emotion of awe. Its triggers are as unique and complex as human personality: a glimpse of the vast and glittering Milky Way galaxy on a clear night, watching a sunrise from the ridge of a mountain, hearing the heart-rending melody of an orchestral suite, or other moving events. In such experiences, one perceives something sufficiently unexpected as to motivate the updating of one's mental schemas (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). The experience of awe may benefit the individual By Rachel Maxwell Kitzmiller immediately as well as far into the future (Ekman, 1992; Abstract Frederickson, 2001). However, studies of the potential Experiencing awe is beneficial to mental health. Benefits of therapeutic benefits of awe are limited.
    [Show full text]
  • Does Awe Increase Timelessness, Helpfulness, and Overall Well-Being?
    Does Awe Increase Timelessness, Helpfulness, and Overall Well-Being? Allison Leach California College of the Arts 1890 Grove Street, Apt #3, San Francisco CA 94117 [email protected] ABSTRACT Does awe expand the perception of time? If so, how does Existing research links induced awe to an increase in time that sense of timelessness influence behavior? Two experi- availability, relative to other emotional states [3]. Awe typi- ments were conducted using university students to investi- cally requires the experiencer to devote time to savor pre- gate awe’s impact on time perception, emotion, purchasing sent feelings and sensations. Increasing time availability has preferences, and helpfulness. In Experiment 1, participants been associated with greater well-being and volunteerism, watched either an awe-inspiring nature video, a boredom- and the preference to acquire experiences as opposed to ma- inducing instructional video, or no video. Afterwards par- terial goods [3]. ticipants answered a questionnaire about mood, time and purchasing preferences, and were presented with a volunteer However, studying awe can present certain difficulties. Awe opportunity by a confederate. In Experiment 2, participants can be troublesome to capture in a lab because of its proxim- watched an awe-inspiring nature video or no video, and ity to other positive emotions. Scale and correlation present filled out a similar questionnaire. Analyses of both experi- other issues: one can’t exactly reconstruct Niagara Falls in ments failed to show a significant relationship between awe, a lab, and awe is often experienced when time pressure is time perception, purchasing preferences and helpfulness. already minimal. Psychologists have not devoted much at- tention to awe because it has not been shown to have a dis- Author Keywords tinctive facial expression [2].
    [Show full text]
  • Awestruck! How the New Science of Awe Can Make Us Happier
    NURSES: Institute for Brain Potential (IBP) is accredited as a provider of nursing continuing professional development by the American Nurses Credentialing Center’s Commission on Accreditation. Institute for Brain Potential is approved as a provider of continuing education by California Board of Registered Nursing, Provider #CEP13896, Interactive Webcast and Florida Board of Nursing. This program provides 6 contact hours. PSYCHOLOGISTS: Institute for Brain Potential is approved by the American Psychological Association to sponsor continuing education for psychologists. Institute Friday, February 12, 2021 for Brain Potential maintains responsibility for this program and its content. This program provides 6 CE credits. Institute for Brain Potential is approved as a provider of continuing education by the Florida Board of Psychology. This course provides 6 hours of CE credit. COUNSELORS, SOCIAL WORKERS & MFTs: Institute for Brain Potential has been Interactive Webcast approved by NBCC as an Approved Continuing Education Provider, ACEP No. 6342. Programs that do not qualify for NBCC credit are clearly identified. Institute for Brain Potential is solely responsible for all aspects of the programs. This program provides 6 Friday, February 12, 2021, 9 AM – 4 PM (PST) clock hours. Institute for Brain Potential, ACE Approval Number: 1160, is approved to offer You will need a computer with internet access and speakers or social work continuing education by the Association of Social Work Boards headphones to participate in the webcast. (ASWB) Approved Continuing Education (ACE) program. Organizations, not individual courses, are approved as ACE providers. State and provincial regulatory boards have the final authority to determine whether an individual course may be accepted for continuing education credit.
    [Show full text]