www.nature.com/scientificreports

OPEN Development of Social Attention and Levels in Maltreated Children Shizuka Suzuki1,2,3, Takashi X. Fujisawa1,4, Nobuko Sakakibara1,4, Toru Fujioka2, Shinichiro Takiguchi1,4 & Akemi Tomoda1,3,4 ✉

Child maltreatment (CM) is a major risk factor for various psychopathologies but also adversely afects social development. Research on oxytocin (OT) is currently drawing attention as an endocrine basis for social development. In this study, we investigated the relationship between visual attention to social cues and salivary OT levels in children exposed to CM. The results revealed that the CM group had a signifcantly lower percentage of gaze fxation for the human face eye area and lower salivary OT levels compared to the typical development group. Moreover, a path analysis suggested that gaze fxation for the eye area was a mediator of the relationship between salivary OT levels and social-emotional problems in the CM group. These results suggest that lower endogenous OT levels in maltreated children may lead to atypical development of their visual attention to eyes as a social cue, resulting in social-emotional problems.

Child maltreatment (CM) includes physical abuse, sexual abuse, psychological abuse, and neglect, such as an inadequate caregiving environment or feeding, and is defned as “any act or series of acts of commission or omis- sion by a parent or other caregiver that results in harm, potential for harm, or threat of harm to a child”1. Tis potential threat also includes exposure to domestic violence between parents. CM is not only a major risk factor for the development of psychopathologies, such as anxiety, substance abuse, psychosis, and personality disorders, but it is also associated with a host of neuropsychological and neurocognitive consequences2. Moreover, previous studies have reported that CM adversely afects social development during childhood. Children with a history of CM were found to have signifcantly poorer skills in social interactions with peers and maintaining self-control, as well as various behavioral problems3,4. In addition, these defcits in social develop- ment domains resulting from CM have been shown to cause secondary problems, such as reduced self-esteem and depressive symptoms5, which can also adversely afect academic performance6. To take precautionary measures for maltreated children, CM needs to be detected as early as possible, creating a further need for a wider survey of related felds. CM is a global problem with adverse lifelong psychological and social functioning consequences; therefore, biomarkers and screening methods for early detection and intervention are necessary2. Previous research has used eye-tracking technology to investigate visual attention patterns for early detection of atypical social development during childhood7,8. Gaze fxation duration for social stimuli, such as human faces or motions, predicts an individual’s ability to interpret others’ intentions and the meaning of social situa- tions9. Tus, eye-tracking technology has several advantages for investigating visual attention during childhood7, allowing researchers to identify, with high precision and accuracy, what a participant is looking at and for how long. Moreover, eye-tracking technology applies to all populations, from infants to adults, irrespective of their verbal ability10. Terefore, diferent aspects of visual attention to social cues can be similarly investigated across various participant characteristics, such as age, gender, and clinical conditions such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have been researched10. Several studies have demonstrated that CM induces impaired social functions as well as atypical social development in conjunction with neuroendocrine dysfunction11–15. Moreover, recent research has reported that in children with CM, gaze fxation duration for emotional faces is signifcantly lower than that of children without CM16. Although it has ofen been suggested that children with neurodevelopmental

1Division of Developmental Higher Brain Functions, United Graduate School of Child Development, University of Fukui, Fukui, Japan. 2Department of Science of Human Development, Faculty of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Fukui, Fukui, Japan. 3Department of Child and Adolescent Psychological Medicine, University of Fukui Hospital, Fukui, Japan. 4Research Center for Child Mental Development, University of Fukui, Fukui, Japan. ✉e-mail: [email protected]

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 1 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

CM (n = 21) TD (n = 29) Statistics P value Male participants, n (%) 11 (52.4) 12 (41.4) χ2(1)=1.37 0.44 Age (years), Mean (SD) 5.5 (0.4) 4.8 (0.3) t(48)=1.37 0.18 Types of maltreatment, n (%) Physical abuse 4 (19) 0 (0) χ2(1)=6.0 0.14 Emotional abuse 10 (48) 0 (0) χ2(1)=18 <0.001 Neglect 16 (76) 0 (0) χ2(1)=32.49 <0.001 Sexual abuse 0 (0) 0 (0) — — Duration (years) of 3.2 (1.88) 0 (0) t(48)=6.13 <0.001 maltreatment, Mean (SD) Duration (years) elapsed from 3.7 (1.84) 0 (0) t(48)=10.8 <0.001 maltreatment, Mean (SD) Duration (years) of residing in 3.5 (0.47) 0 (0) t(48)=8.80 <0.001 childcare facility, Mean (SD) ACE total, Mean (SD) 3.95 (1.6) 0.07 (0.26) t(48)=12.92 <0.001 SDQ total, Mean (SD) 11.48 (7.47) 5.9 (3.68) t(48)=3.49 <0.001 CMTI, Mean (SD) 135.38 (29.65) 100.17 (12.50) t(48)=5.75 <0.001

Table 1. Participants’ demographic and psychological behavioral characteristics. CM: Childhood Maltreatment, TD: Typical Development, ACE: Adverse Childhood Experience (Felitti et al., 1998), SDQ: Strength and Difculties Questionnaire (Goodman et al., 2010), CMTI: Checklist for Maltreated Infant (Izumi et al., 2009).

disorders such as ASD or intellectual disability are at heightened risk for CM17, their commonality and heteroge- neity in the atypical development of social attention have not been clarifed, and the biological basis has not been researched. Oxytocin (OT) signaling represents one of the most critical systems involved in human . OT, a neuropeptide secreted from the posterior pituitary, not only has physiological functions in labor and lactation, but it is also critical for post-natal mother-child bonding18. Recently, increasing evidence indicates that OT plays an important role in modulating social behavior in diverse species19. Previous human studies have suggested that OT administration facilitates sociability, such as inferring the mental state of others from viewing their eye region20,21, and that endogenous OT has a signifcant role in visual attention development to social cues during childhood22. OT level changes have also been suggested in the hormonal dysregulation associated with CM12. Previous studies have reported that women with a history of CM have lower OT concentrations in cerebrospinal fuid, and children who have experienced CM and lack attachment formation with a primary caregiver have atypical OT secretion patterns13–15. In this study, we investigated the relationship between visual attention to social cues and salivary OT levels in children with a history of CM compared to children with TD. Although several studies have examined the rela- tionship between CM and peripheral OT levels, or between CM and atypical social development, the association between the two in children with CM remains unclear. We measured visual attention patterns to social cues using eye-tracking, and salivary OT levels using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (ELISA), to investigate the rela- tionships among these factors. Tus, our main hypothesis was that poor visual attention to social cues, and lower salivary OT levels would be observed in children with CM. In addition, we have previously reported that endog- enous OT afects the development of individual diferences in visual attention to social cues in healthy children18. Terefore, the subsidiary hypothesis is that the severity of developmental problems in CM is related to the atypical development of social attention based on endogenous OT dysfunction. Results Demographics, psychological, and behavioral assessments. We measured gaze fxation for vari- ous social cues and salivary OT levels and carried out several psychological and behavioral assessments in ffy children with or without exposure to CM. To eliminate the potential efects of age and gender, the groups were matched for these two factors22–24. Te demographics, psychological, and behavioral characteristics of each group are listed in Table 1. First, we examined the diferences between the groups (CM and TD) for the distribution of gender and the mean of their age using chi-square and t-tests, and confrmed that the two groups were matched for gender and age (X2(1) = 1.37, p = 0.44; t(48) = 1.37, p = 0.18). Next, when the diferences between the groups were examined for the mean levels of the psychological and behavioral adaptation using the t-test, the CM group had signifcantly higher levels of adverse childhood experiences, social-emotional problems, and psychiatric symptoms compared to the TD group (ACE: t(48) = 12.92, p < 0.001; SDQ: t (48) = 3.49, p < 0.001; CMTI: t(48) = 5.75, p < 0.001)27–29. Tese results indicated that the CM group had lower adaptive psychological and behavio- ral function than the TD group.

Between-group comparisons of gaze fixation. The percentage of gaze fixation met the criteria of > 70% in all participants, with no signifcant diferences between the groups on average (t(48) = −0.66, p = 0.511). Gaze fxation duration percentages for the four diferent types of social cues are presented in Fig. 1 (a: the human face, b: people and geometry, c: biological motion, and d: fnger pointing; Snapshots of each type of stimulus can be found in ref. 24). Stimuli were presented as short movies, and each type of cue had three areas of

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 2 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

Figure 1. Te percentage of gaze fxation duration of each AOI between the TD and CM groups: (a) Human face, (b) People and geometry, (c) Biological motion, and (d) Finger pointing. Te vertical axis indicates the percentage of gaze fxation on each type of social cue. AOI, Area-of-Interest. TD, Typical Development. CM, Childhood Maltreatment.

interest (AOIs). Te frst AOI represented higher social cues with higher saliency (“high social”), the second AOI represented lower or non-social cues with higher saliency (“low social”), and the third AOI represented another area with lower saliency (“background”). A two-way (2 × 3) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to examine the diferences in the percentage of gaze fxation between the groups (CM, TD) for each AOI. For human face stimuli, we did not observe a main efect of group (F(1, 48) = 0.09, p = 0.763), but a group × AOIs interaction (F(2, 96) = 3.16, p = 0.047). As a result of testing the simple main efect of the group for each AOI, a signifcant diference between the groups was con- frmed only in high social (F(1, 144) = 6.25, p = 0.014), but not in low social and background (F(1, 144) = 1.54, p = 0.216; F(1, 144) = 1.04, p = 0.309). Tese results indicate that the fxation percentage on high social (eyes area) was signifcantly lower in the CM group than the TD group, but there was no signifcant diference in the fxation percentage for low social (mouth area) or background (other area) (Fig. 1a). On the other hand, for the other three types of social cues (people and geometry, biological motion, and fnger pointing), we observed no main efects on the group for the fxation percentage (F(1, 48) < 0.01, p = 0.951; F(1, 48) = 0.10, p = 0.760; F(1, 48) = 0.06, p = 0.812, respectively) and no group × AOI interactions (F(2, 96) = 1.21, p = 0.302; F(2, 96) = 2.32, p = 0.103; F(2, 96) = 0.22, p = 0.800, respectively; Fig. 1b-d), although the main efects of AOIs confrmed that high social had a higher fxation percentage than low social and background (F(2, 96) = 105.92, p < 0.001; F(2, 96) = 52.90, p < 0.001; F(2, 96) = 191.85, p < 0.001, respectively). In addition, the efect of age on fxation per- centage was not confrmed in high social AOIs, including human face stimuli, with observed diferences between groups, and the efect was found to be limited in some low social and background AOIs (Table S1). Tese results indicate that the CM group does not demonstrate any atypical patterns in visual attention to social cues other than human faces.

Between-group comparison of salivary OT levels. To examine the relationship between endogenous OT levels and CM, we compared between-group salivary OT levels using independent t-tests and found that salivary OT levels were signifcantly lower in the CM group than the TD group (t(47) = −2.03, p = 0.048; Fig. 2).

Relationship between gaze fxation and psychological,behavioral adaptation levels in the CM group. To investigate the relationship between atypical gaze fxation and psychological, behavioral adaptation levels in children with CM, we conducted a correlation analysis (Pearson’s r) with the psychological and behavio- ral scales for the high social area (eyes) in human face stimuli that showed a decrease in the percentage of gaze fx- ation in the CM group. Te results indicated a signifcant negative correlation with the social-emotional problems

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 3 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

Figure 2. Te percentage of salivary OT levels. OT, Oxytocin. TD, Typical Development. CM, Childhood Maltreatment.

Figure 3. Path model for the mediation efect of visual attention on eyes in the relationship between salivary OT levels and social-emotional problems in maltreated children. OT, Oxytocin. AOI, Area-of-Interest. SDQ, Strength and Difculties Questionnaire.

(SDQ: r = −0.44, p = 0.048), but there was no correlation between the severity of adverse childhood experiences and psychiatric symptoms with exposure to CM (ACE: r = −0.24, p = 0.299; CMTI: r = −0.33, p = 0.139).

Relationship between gaze fxation and salivary OT levels in the CM group. As described in the previous section, we observed a signifcant negative correlation between gaze fxation for the eye area in the human face (high social) and psychological and behavioral adaptation levels in the CM group. We then per- formed a correlation analysis for the CM group to confrm the relationship between gaze fxation and salivary OT levels, which demonstrated that salivary OT levels tended to correlate with percentage of fxation times for the eyes area in the human face, although the correlations did not reach a signifcant level (r = 0.42, p = 0.057). We also conducted a path analysis to examine whether gaze fixation for eyes mediated the relationship between salivary OT levels and social-emotional problems, despite no signifcant correlation in the direct rela- tionship (r = −0.14, p = 0.334)28,29. As illustrated in Fig. 3, the salivary OT levels were a signifcant predictor of gaze fxation for eyes (β = 0.42, t = 2.08, p = 0.038). When salivary OT levels and gaze fxation for eyes were entered simultaneously as predictors of social-emotional problems (as measure by SDQ scores), the percentage of gaze fxation for eyes was a signifcant predictor (β = −0.46, t = −2.09, p = 0.037), while salivary OT levels were not (β = 0.06, t = 0.29, p = 0.775). Te overall model excluding the path from OT levels to SDQ ft the data well (X2(1) = 0.08, p = 0.786, GFI = 0.997, AGFI = 0.984, RMSEA < 0.001, AIC = 10.081), while the indirect efect of salivary OT levels on social-emotional problems was estimated to remain moderate (β = 0.20). Tus, these fnd- ings suggest that visual attention to eyes mediates the relationship between endogenous OT levels in saliva and social-emotional problems,and the efect is restrictive. Discussion We investigated the relationship between visual attention to social cues and salivary OT levels in children with CM and TD. Te results revealed that the CM group had a signifcantly lower percentage of gaze fxation for the human face eye area compared to the TD group. We also found that salivary OT levels were lower in the CM group than the TD group. Moreover, the gaze fxation duration for the eye area in the CM group was negatively correlated with the degree of social-emotional problems and salivary OT levels. Accordingly, a path analysis suggested that gaze fxation for the eye area was a mediator of the relationship between salivary OT levels and social-emotional problems in the CM group. Tese results suggest that lower endogenous OT levels induced by exposure to CM may lead to atypical development of visual attention to eyes as a social cue, resulting in social-emotional problems in children with CM. We found that visual attention to the eyes was lower in the CM group than in the TD group and that this was related to poor social-emotional functioning in the CM group. Previous studies have reported that children with CM had attention biases for human facial expressions compared to non-CM children, and that attention bias was

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 4 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

associated with social-emotional problems16,30. Our results are consistent with these fndings in that exposure to CM is associated with atypical development of visual attention to human faces, which is, in turn, associated with social-emotional problems in maltreated children. In contrast, other research has reported that children with disinhibited attachment disorder exposed to CM showed no attention bias to human faces compared with TD children31, suggesting that attention bias and social-emotional symptoms may change dynamically, depending on the type and timing of CM exposure32,33. On the other hand, lower visual attention in the CM group was not asso- ciated with the severity of adversity experience (ACE) or psychiatric symptoms associated with CM (trauma and sensory dysregulation; CMTI), suggesting that reduced social attention in the CM group is not directly related to the severity of CM exposure and associated psychiatric symptoms. Typically, the gaze of others plays an important role as a social signal because it indicates the presence of a person or object that the person is paying attention to or interest in34. Terefore, studies have reported that people generally tend to pay attention to others’ eyes35. Tere are two possible explanations for why visual attention to eyes is lower in the CM group: “gaze avoidance” and “atypical distribution of attention.” According to the former explanation, it has been reported that people with avoid eye contact because of threats associated with gazing36, and CM experience has also been found to induce higher social anxiety37. Terefore, the high level of social anxiety induced by CM may lead to gaze avoidance. Tis notion is also largely consistent with the results of an earlier study that found that higher stress experiences in early childhood results in lower visual attention to facial expressions38. It is also possible that CM children’s visual attention tends to be directed to other areas (e.g., other face parts, situations) rather than the eye area. Regarding the latter explanation, we also note that visual attention to social cues excluding eye area in human faces (people and geometry, biological motions, and fnger pointing) were not signifcantly associated with CM exposure or OT levels in our current results. Previous studies targeting neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD in particular, have reported that the ASD group has a diferent distribution pattern of attentional allocation to social cues than the TD group8,39. It has been reported that children with ASD have reduced visual attention to social cues, such as human eyes, people, and biological motions8,39 while showing visual interest in inorganic cues such as geometric patterns40. Similarly, our previous study that presented the same stimuli also found that the ASD group had a lower gaze fxation for the human eye or people than the TD group, while fxation of the mouth or geometry area was higher41. However, the reduced visual attention of the CM group was specifcally observed for the eye area in the current study. Taken together, the atypical visual attention of the CM group may be more rational to consider based on “gaze avoidance” rather than based on the “atypical distribution of attention” to general social cues as observed in the ASD group, which may involve endogenous OT dysfunction. Many studies have reported endogenous OT dysfunction in maltreated children19,21,23. Fries et al. found that early neglected children had lower OT levels than control children afer interaction with their mothers21. Tese results are consistent with the current study’s fndings on the relationships between lower OT levels and child- hood adversity, as represented by CM. Although several studies on the long-lasting efects of CM and efects on psychological health in adulthood, Heim et al. reported that adult women with a CM history had signif- cantly lower OT levels extracted from cerebrospinal fuid compared to women without a CM history13. Similarly, Opacka-Jufry et al. also demonstrated that early childhood adverse experiences were negatively associated with plasma OT levels in adult men42. Although it should be noted that the fndings from adulthood studies were com- pared with the results of the current study, the previous fndings may be consistent with the association of CM exposure to reduce the endogenous OT levels in the CM group. Te gaze fxation analysis revealed a positive association between salivary OT levels and fxation for the human face eye area in the CM group. Several previous studies have suggested that OT exerts a positive efect on attention to social cues, including the eye region20,21. Similarly, impairments in social functioning, such as those seen in children with ASD, have been reported to be related to oxytocinergic system dysfunction43, and previ- ous eye-tracking studies have revealed that children with ASD exhibit lower or altered visual attention to social cues8,10. Furthermore, our path analysis confrmed that reduced gaze fxation for the eye region mediated the relationship between salivary OT levels and social-emotional problems in maltreated children, although no direct relationship was found between the two. Tese fndings suggest that endogenous OT is involved in the control of visual attention to social cues in maltreated children, particularly in regard to the human face eye area, and that lower OT levels indirectly afect social-emotional problems associated with atypical development in visual atten- tion. Tis is the frst study to elucidate the relationship between decreased social attention and social-emotional problems in maltreated children and to suggest that they may have dysfunctional endogenous OT. Tis study has some limitations. First, the study had a relatively small sample size and used a cross-sectional design that precludes the identifcation of causal links between salivary OT levels, CM, and social-emotional problems. It was also difcult to consider potential impact diferences related to the type or timing of CM. Longitudinal studies with larger sample sizes are necessary to better identify these associations. Second, the sub- sequent environmental factors afer CM could not be controlled in this study because only children in residential childcare facilities were recruited as CM group participants. Similar investigations should be conducted with children in foster care. Tird, none of the children in this study had a diagnosis of neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD in children aged 3 years or older, but the possibility could not be excluded in children under 2 years. In addition, as children in residential childcare facilities were sampled, it was difcult to obtain parental confr- mation of the developmental difculties evident before the children entered the facility. Longitudinal studies that compare CM children with innately high-risk ASD children are necessary. Fourth, saliva sampling included only a single daytime measure. Although no signifcant diurnal variation in OT was confrmed, it is necessary to investigate potential variations over multiple time points44. Finally, the chil- dren’s cognitive abilities (IQ/DQ) and socioeconomic status (SES) were not controlled in group comparisons in this study. In this study, it was difcult to assess their cognitive abilities on a single scale because we studied chil- dren aged 2–9, in particular, to quantitatively assess the 12 children in the TD group using Denver II (Table S2).

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 5 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

Terefore, the cognitive ability could not be analyzed as a covariate. Although both groups used IQ/DQ < 70 as an exclusion criterion, there were also substantial diferences in cognitive abilities between the groups, as sug- gested in previous studies. In our previous study, using the same instrument for children with ASD, their reduced attention to social cues were not associated with IQ41. Tis fnding may indicate that reduced social attention in the CM group in this study is not directly attributed to cognitive abilities. Regarding SES, it was difcult to obtain information on parents’ SES using a standardized scale, such as the Hollingshead two-factor index among chil- dren in residential care facilities. However, it was also confrmed in the current study that there was no signifcant diference in the distribution between groups in the questionnaire item about whether there were any fnancial difculties in the past or present (X2(1) = 2.89, p = 0.089; Table S2). Despite these limitations, this study sheds light on the neurobehavioral underpinnings of social-emotional problems caused by an environmental factor, i.e., the adverse experiences of children with CM. In conclusion, our results demonstrated signifcantly lower visual attention to eyes, a salient social cue, in the CM group than the TD group; and that salivary OT levels were lower in the CM group than the TD group. Moreover, salivary OT levels were positively related to visual attention to the eyes area in the CM group. Tese results suggest that CM exposure is associated with a reduction in endogenous OT, and that the reduction mod- ulates atypical visual attention to the eyes in the CM group. In addition, the reduced social attention in the CM group was associated with the degree of social-emotional problems, and social attention was a mediator in the relationship between oxytocin and social-emotional problems. Tese results suggest that the development of social attention for eyes is modulated by endogenous OT, which may exert an efect on social-emotional problems in maltreated children. In the future, it is important to clarify the neural basis of visual attention to social cues by combining physiological indicators such as brain imaging. Methods Participants. Fify-three Japanese children aged 2–9 years (25 boys, 28 girls; mean age ± SD: 5.2 ± 1.8 years) participated in this study. Te maltreated group consisted of 21 children with maltreatment experiences (CM group) who were living in residential childcare facilities in Japan. All of the children in the CM group had a history of physical, sexual, or emotional abuse or neglect before coming to the facility. Te control (TD) group consisted of 32 typically developing children with no history of maltreatment and who did not meet the DSM-5 criteria as assessed by administered licensed pediatric-psychiatric clinicians (SS, TF, and AT)45. Te TD chil- dren were recruited from the local community. Te children over 6 years had a full-scale quotient (IQ) > 70 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition or the Tanaka Binet Intelligence Scale- Fifh Edition (Japanese version of the Stanford-Binet Test), or a full-scale developmental intelligence quotient (DQ) > 70 on the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development. Te children under 5 years completed the Denver Developmental Screening Test (Denver II)46–49. Subsequently, three children who were assessed as having sus- pected or confrmed developmental delay were excluded. Te protocol of this study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Fukui (Assurance no. 20140142), and conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki. All parent(s) or childcare facility directors provided written informed consent for participation in this study.

Gaze pattern measurement. We measured gaze pattern for children using Gazefnder (JVC KENWOOD Corporation, Kanagawa, Japan), an eye-tracking system for responses to visual stimuli22®,24,41. Te apparatus recorded the percentage of fxation duration allocated to specifc AOIs on a video monitor22,24,41. Te Gazefnder is equipped with infrared light sources and cameras with a 19-inch transistor monitor (1280 × 1024 pixels).® Using corneal refection techniques, the Gazefnder records the X and Y coordinates of the eye position at a frequency of 50 Hz (i.e., 3000 data collections/minute).® Stimuli presented by the Gazefnder consisted of short movies, including four categories of social cues, which were (a) human faces, (b) people and ®geometric patterns, (c) the biological motion of a human, and (d) objects with or without fnger pointing. First, the human faces of (a) are considered representative of social stimuli50, and humans appear to naturally pay attention to the face, especially to the eye area22,51. Next, the people and geometry of (b) were based on the preference paradigm movie and measures the degree of visual attention to social cues underlying the hypothesis that individuals with atypical development such as ASD tend to react to inorganic cues, such as geometric patterns relative to social stimuli40. The biological motion movie of (c) also measures the degree of attention to social cues underlying the hypothesis that humans show an innate preference for the biological motion to facilitate adaptive interaction with others52. Finally, fnger pointing of (d) is a signifcant part of the ability to establish join attention with others53, and various studies have reported a defcit in joint attention as one of the salient signs of atypical devel- opment54. Tus, based on the previous fndings, the above four types of movies were presented as social cues by Gazefnder . Tree AOIs were set within each stimulus (Fig. 1a-d). Te frst AOI, high social, represented higher social cues ®with higher saliency (“eyes” in [a], “people” in [b], “upright fgure” in [c], and “object with pointing” in [d]). Te second AOI, low social, represented lower or non-social cues with higher saliency (“mouth” in [a], “geometry” in [b], “inverted fgure” in [c], and “object without pointing” in [d]). Te third AOI, background, rep- resented the other areas with lower saliency22,24,41. Snapshots of the four types of stimuli presented can be found in the reference24.

Procedure and stimuli. Te experiments were conducted in a quiet room at the childcare facility for the CM group and at the university research laboratory for the TD group during the daytime from 9:00 to 17:00 (Table S2). Te children were seated on a small chair, approximately 70 cm in front of the eye-tracking monitor. Parent or institution staf were instructed not to assist the children during the research procedure. To obtain calibration information, the children were instructed to look at the animated animal displayed in one of fve locations on the monitor. If the calibration quality was poor on any of these points, the calibration process was

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 6 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

repeated. Before the task, the children were informed that pictures of faces, people, and objects would be shown on the monitor, and they were instructed to look at them for as long as they could without looking away. Stimulus movies were organized in a defnitive order, and the sequence was presented only once as a trial. Between the stimulus movies, an attention-catching animation with a voice saying “Hey! Look!” was presented in the center of the monitor to reorient the children’s attention to the stimuli22,24,41. Te trial was repeated in case the percentage of gaze fxation for the screen did not meet the criteria of > 70%.

Salivary OT levels measurement. We collected saliva samples using Salivettes (Sarstedt, Rommelsdorf, Germany). Parents or facility staf were instructed to place the collected cotton in the children’s’® mouth and chew it for one minute until it was saturated with saliva22,24. Tis process was repeated to collect two cotton samples. Te saliva samples were then stored frozen at −80 °C, and were lyophilized overnight at −20 °C to concentrate them 2- to 4-fold. Prior to the assay, the dry samples were reconstituted in the assay bufer using a commercial OT enzyme immunoassay kit (Enzo Life Sciences, Inc., NY, USA). Tese protocols were consistent with those of pre- vious studies on adults55–57 as well as on 5- to 19-month-old children22,24. Each sample was examined in duplicate, and the concentrations were calculated using the microplate reader, according to relevant standard curves. Te average intra-assay coefcients of variation (CV) was 8.2%.

Psychological and behavioral characteristics assessment. In addition to the clinical assessments, we evaluated the participants’ psychological and behavioral characteristics using the following three scales under the supervision of pediatric clinicians: the Adverse Childhood Experience (ACE) Questionnaire was used to assess the severity of ACEs, which addresses ten individual ACEs under three categories: abuse, neglect and household dysfunction (e.g., parental divorce, domestic violence)25,58. Te ACE questionnaire is a reliable and valid measure of childhood adversity that has been used extensively in large-scale ACE studies58. In this study, we used a Japanese version of nine items that were modifed for Japanese children59. Te ACE score ranges from 0–9 and is the total number of childhood adversities (only one count per type of abuse) experienced before 18 years of age. Although no clear cut-of value was set in the ACE questionnaire, a recent review of the research sug- gests that participants who had ACE scores of 4 or more were at increased risk of all health outcomes compared to those who had no ACEs60. To assess the social-emotional problems, parents or caregivers at the residential childcare facility completed the Strength and Difculties Questionnaire (SDQ) consisting of 25 items, which is widely used across diferent cultures and has confrmed reliability and validity in both the original and Japanese versions61,62. Te SDQ is a simple adaptive functioning screen for children,and it is divided into fve categories to assess children’s internalized (emotional and relationships) and externalized (conduct and hyperactivity) prob- lems. Prosocial scores are excluded from the total scores as they are classifed as a positive quality26. Te items were scored on a 0–2 scale (not true, somewhat true, certainly true), and the total difculty score is calculated by adding the results of the scales (excluding the prosocial behavior scale), which can range from 0 to 40. From the total difculties score, the SDQ enables researchers to classify subjects as normal, borderline, or abnormal, based on cut-of points. In the Japanese version, 12 or less are assessed as “normal,” and 13 or more are assessed as “borderline” or “abnormal”62. To assess the severity of psychiatric symptoms observed in maltreated children in a residential childcare facility, parents or caregivers in residential childcare facility completed the Checklist for Maltreated Infant (CMTI) consisting of 81 items, including three subscales: trauma, attachment, and sensory regulation. Te items were rated on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a very great degree), and the sum of the three subscales generates a total score, which can range from 81 to 324. Te cut-of threshold for the total score is not defned. Reliability and validity analyses have proved CMTI to be an efective and prac- tical tool27.

Statistical analysis. We compared the group diferences (CM and TD) between the demographics and clin- ical characteristics using chi-square and t-tests. To examine the efects of groups on each type of social cue (face, people, motion, and pointing), we computed the percentage of gaze fxation on the three AOIs and performed a two-way ANOVA with them as the dependent variable, and with AOIs (high social, low social, background) as the within-subjects variable and group (CM, TD) as the between group variable. In addition, the diference in salivary OT levels between groups was also examined using a t-test. Next, to examine the associations between the severity of clinical characteristics, atypical visual attention to social cues, and endogenous OT levels in the CM group, we calculated the correlation coefcients between their clinical scale scores, the percentage of gaze fxation in AOIs of social cues showing diferences between groups, and the salivary OT levels. Finally, to test the validity of the model that OT dysfunction is involved in the adaptive functioning of the CM group via atypical social attention, we examined the relationship between the variables using a path analysis. Te signifcance level was set to p < 0.05, and all of the statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics and Amos for Windows, version20 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Received: 8 November 2019; Accepted: 13 April 2020; Published: xx xx xxxx References 1. Leeb, R. T., Paulozzi, L. J., Melanson, C., Simon, T. R. & Arias, I. Child maltreatment surveillance: Uniform defnitions for public health and recommended data elements, version 1.0. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1, 409–438, https://doi. org/10.1177/1077559599004001005 (2008). 2. Teicher, M. H. & Samson, J. A. Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology: A case for ecophenotypic variants as clinically and neurobiologically distinct subtypes. Am. J. Psychiatry. 170, 1114–1133, https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070957 (2013). 3. Becker-Weidman, A. Efects of early maltreatment on development: A descriptive study using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II. Child. Welfare. 88, 137–161, https://doi.org/10.1039/c7lc01004d (2009).

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 7 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

4. Darwish, D., Esquivel, G. B., Houtz, J. C. & Alfonso, V. C. Play and social skills in maltreated and non-maltreated preschoolers during peer interactions. Child. Abuse Negl. 25, 13–31, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00228-3 (2001). 5. Kim, J. & Cicchetti, D. Longitudinal trajectories of self-system processes and depressive symptoms among maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child. Development. 77, 624–639, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00894.x (2006). 6. Leiter, J. School performance trajectories afer the advent of reported maltreatment. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 29, 363–382, https://doi. org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2006.09.002 (2007). 7. Karatekin, C. Eye-tracking studies of normative and atypical development. Developmental Rev. 27, 283–348, https://doi. org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.006 (2007). 8. Chita-Tegmark, M. Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye tracking studies. Res. Dev. Disabil. 48, 79–93, https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011 (2016). 9. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. & Cohen, D. Visual fxation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 59, 809–816, https://doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.59.9.809 (2002). 10. Guillon, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduel, S. & Rogé, B. Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye-tracking studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 279–297, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.013 (2014). 11. Alink, L. R., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J. & Rogosch, F. A. Longitudinal associations among child maltreatment, social functioning, and cortisol regulation. Dev. Psychol. 48, 224–236, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024892 (2012). 12. Mizushima, S. G. et al. Efect of the nature of subsequent environment on oxytocin and cortisol secretion in maltreated children. Front. Psychiatry. 6, 173, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00173 (2015). 13. Heim, C. et al. Lower CSF oxytocin concentrations in women with a history of childhood abuse. Mol. Psychiatry. 14, 954–958, https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.112 (2009). 14. Fries, A. B. W., Ziegler, T. E., Kurian, J. R., Jacoris, S. & Pollak, S. D. Early experience in humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating social behavior. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 17237–17240, https://doi.org/10.1073/ pnas.0504767102 (2005). 15. Pierrehumbert, B. et al. Oxytocin response to an experimental psychosocial challenge in adults exposed to traumatic experiences during childhood or adolescence. Neuroscience. 166, 168–177, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.12.016 (2010). 16. Mastorakos, T. & Scott, K. L. Attention biases and social-emotional development in preschool-aged children who have been exposed to domestic violence. Child. Abuse Negl. 89, 78–86, https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CHIABU.2019.01.001 (2019). 17. McDonnell, C. G. et al. Child maltreatment in autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability: results from a population-based sample. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 60, 576–584, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12993 (2019). 18. Galbally, M., Lewis, A. J., Ijzendoorn, Mv & Permezel, M. Te role of oxytocin in mother-infant relations: a systematic review of human studies. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry. 19, 1–14, https://doi.org/10.3109/10673229.2011.549771 (2011). 19. Insel, T. R. Te Challenge of Translation in Social Neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and afliative behavior. Neuron. 65, 768–779, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005 (2010). 20. Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., Berger, C. & Herpertz, S. C. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol. Psychiatry. 61, 731–733, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015 (2007). 21. Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B. & Dadds, M. R. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol. Psychiatry. 63, 3–5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026 (2008). 22. Nishizato, M., Fujisawa, T. X., Kosaka, H. & Tomoda, A. Developmental changes in social attention and oxytocin levels in infants and children. Sci. Rep. 7, 2540, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02368-x (2017). 23. Seltzer, L. J., Ziegler, T., Connolly, M. J., Prososki, A. R. & Pollak, S. D. Stress-induced elevation of oxytocin in maltreated children: evolution, neurodevelopment, and social behavior. Child. Dev. 85, 501–512, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12136 (2014). 24. Fujisawa, T. X., Tanaka, S., Saito, D. N., Kosaka, H. & Tomoda, A. Visual attention for social information and salivary oxytocin levels in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: An eye-tracking study. Front. Neurosci. 8, 295, https://doi.org/10.3389/ fnins.2014.00295 (2014). 25. Felitti, V. J. et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults - impact on children. Am. J. Prev. Med. 14, 245–258, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8 (1998). 26. Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L. & Ploubidis, G. B. When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised fve subscales on the Strengths and Difculties Questionnaire (SDQ): data from British parents, teachers and children. J. Abnorm. Child. Psychol. 38, 1179–1191, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x (2010). 27. Izumi, M. & Okuyama, M. Development of checklist for maltreated young children. Psychiatria et. Neurologia Paediatrica Japonica. 49, 121–130 (2009). 28. Hayes, A. F. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun. Monogr. 76, 408–420 (2009). 29. Shrout, P. E. & Bolger, N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods. 7, 422–445 (2002). 30. Koizumi, M. & Takagishi, H. Te relationship between child maltreatment and . PLoS One. 9, e86093, https:// doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086093 (2014). 31. Miellet, S., Caldara, R., Gillberg, C., Raju, M. & Minnis, H. Disinhibited reactive attachment disorder symptoms impair social judgements from faces. Psychiatry Res. 215, 747–752, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.004 (2014). 32. Schalinski, I. et al. Type and timing of adverse childhood experiences differentially affect severity of PTSD, dissociative and depressive symptoms in adult inpatients. BMC Psychiatry. 16, 295, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-1004-5 (2016). 33. Fujisawa, T. X. et al. Type and timing of childhood maltreatment and reduced visual cortex volume in children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 20, 216–221, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.07.018 (2018). 34. Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmilof-Smith, A., Grant, J. & Walker, J. Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic signifcance of the eyes? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 13, 379–398, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1995.tb00687.x (1995). 35. YarbusHC.Eye movements during of complex objects. In Eye Movements and Vision,171–211; https://doi. org/10.1007/978-1-4899-5379-7_8 (1967). 36. Greist, J. H. Te diagnosis of social phobia. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 56, 5–12 (1995). 37. Heim, C. & Nemerof, C. B. Te role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: Preclinical and clinical studies. Biol. Psychiatry. 49, 1023–1039, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X (2001). 38. Humphreys, K. L., Kircanski, K., Colich, N. L. & Gotlib, I. H. Attentional avoidance of fearful facial expressions following early life stress is associated with impaired social functioning. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 57, 1174–1182, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12607 (2016). 39. Falck-Ytter, T., Rehnberg, E. & Bölte, S. Lack of visual orienting to biological motion and audiovisual synchrony in 3-year-olds with autism. PLoS One. 8, e68816, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068816 (2013). 40. Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R. & Desmond, J. Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 68, 101–109, https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113 (2011). 41. Fujioka, T. et al. Gazefnder as a clinical supplementary tool for discriminating between autism spectrum disorder and typical development in male adolescents and adults. Mol. Autism. 7, 19, https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0083-y (2016). 42. Opacka-Jufry, J. & Mohiyeddini, C. Experience of stress in childhood negatively correlates with plasma oxytocin concentration in adult men. Stress. 15, 1–10, https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011.560309 (2012).

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 8 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

43. Kosaka, H. et al. Oxytocin efcacy is modulated by dosage and oxytocin receptor genotype in young adults with high-functioning autism: A 24-week randomized clinical trial. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e872, https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.152 (2016). 44. Amico, J. A., Tenicela, R., Johnston, J. & Robinson, A. G. A time-dependent peak of oxytocin exists in cerebrospinal fuid but not in plasma of humans. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 57, 947–951, https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-57-5-947 (1983). 45. American psychiatric association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Fifth edition (DSM-5). Arlington: American Psychiatric Association (2013). 46. WechslerD. Wechsler intelligence scale for childrenfourth editionUSA. San Antonio: Pearson (2003). 47. Sugihara, K.et al. (Eds.)Tanaka Binet Intelligence Scale V.Tokyo, Japan: Taken Shuppan (2003). 48. Ikuzawa, M., Matsushita, H., &Nakazawa, A.(eds.). Guides for the Use of the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development2001. Kyoto: Kyoto International Social Welfare Exchange Center (2002). 49. Frankenburg, W. K., Dodds, J., Archer, P., Shapiro, H. & Bresnick, B. Te DenverII: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 89, 91–97 (1992). 50. Pelphrey, K. A. et al. Scanning of Faces in Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 249–261, https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016374617369 (2002). 51. Jones, W. & Klin, A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature. 504, 427–431, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12715 (2013). 52. Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G. & Jones, W. Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature. 459, 257–261, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07868 (2009). 53. Paparella, T., Goods, K. S., Freeman, S. & Kasari, C. Te emergence of nonverbal joint attention and requesting skills in young children with autism. J. Commun. Disord. 44, 569–583, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis (2011). 54. Warreyn, P., Roeyers, H., Van Wetswinkel, U. & De Groote, I. Temporal coordination of joint attention behavior in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 501–512, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0184-0 (2007). 55. Carter, C. S. et al. Oxytocin: Behavioral associations and potential as a salivary biomarker. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1098, 312–322, https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1384.006 (2007). 56. Daughters, K. et al. Salivary oxytocin concentrations in males following intranasal administration of oxytocin: A double-blind, cross-over study. PLoS One. 10, e0145104, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145104 (2015). 57. Lebowitz, E. R. et al. Salivary oxytocin in clinically anxious youth: Associations with separation anxiety and family accommodation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 65, 35–43, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.007. (2016). 58. Cheong, E. V., Sinnott, C., Dahly, D. & Kearney, P. M. Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and later-life depression: perceived social support as a potential protective factor. BMJ Open. 7, e013228, https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013228 (2017). 59. Matsuura, N., Hashimoto, T. & Toichi, M. Examination of juvenile delinquency, adverse childhood experiences and maltreatment: Evidence based survey of using ACE questionnaire in juvenile training schools. Hyogo Univ. Teach. Educ. Journal. 30, 215–223 (2007). 60. Hughes, K. et al. Te efect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public. Health. 2, e356–e366, https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 (2017). 61. Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C., Vermulst, A. A. & Janssens, J. M. Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difculties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 13, 254–274, https://doi. org/10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2 (2010). 62. Matsuishi, T. et al. Scale properties of the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difculties Questionnaire (SDQ): A study of infant and school children in community samples. Brain Dev. 30, 410–415, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2007.12.003 (2008).

Acknowledgements We thank Dr. Aki Yazawa for assistance with data collection and the staf at the Research Center for Child Mental Development for their clerical support.Tis work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for “Creating a Safe and Secure Living Environment in the Changing Public and Private Spheres” from the Japan Science and Technology Corporation (JST)/Research Institute of Science and Technology for Society (RISTEX). Tis study was also partially supported by Scientifc Research (A), (B), Challenging Exploratory Research (#19H00617, #15H03106, #17K19898 to AT), Scientifc Research (C) (#18K02480 to TXF), and Early-Career Scientists (#18K13109 to ST) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. Tis study was also supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientifc Research from Japan-United States Brain Research Cooperation Program, AMED under Grant Number JP20gk0110052, and research grants from the Takeda Science Foundation (to AT). Te funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Author contributions A.T. conceived the project. S.S., T.X.F. and A.T. designed the experiments. S.S., T.X.F., N.S., T.F., S.T. and A.T. performed the experiments, collected and analyzed the data. S.S. and T.X.F. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the fnal manuscript. Competing interests Te authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to A.T. Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints. Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional afliations.

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 9 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ www.nature.com/scientificreports

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre- ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Te images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per- mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

© Te Author(s) 2020

Scientific Reports | (2020)10:7407 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64297-6 10